Introduction

Female brood desertion in avian species with bi-parental care remains poorly understood, mostly because since Trivers’ (1972) influential paper, the evolution of mating systems has been considered through the lens of the necessity of male care. When bi-parental care is not essential to rear offspring, males are often selected to desert or at least to reduce effort invested into the current brood. Owing to higher reproductive potential (i.e. higher number and rate of gametes production), males may benefit more than females from mating with additional partners, i.e. “re-mating hypothesis” (Trivers 1972; Bennett and Owens 2002; Orians 2011). Nevertheless, in some species it is possible for the female to desert the brood, leaving the partner to continue the care (e.g. Griggio et al. 2005; Rossmanith et al. 2009; Morton et al. 2010; Korpimäki et al. 2011; Béziers and Roulin 2016).

Considering the re-mating hypothesis in a broader context, the brood is deserted by the sex with a higher chance to re-mate (Székely and Williams 1995; Szekely and Cuthill 2000). Although males are favoured by basic sex differences in reproductive potential, the chances for re-mating may be circumstance-dependent. For example, in the Kentish plover, Charadrius alexandrinus, much depends on the current operational sex ratio in the adult population, and since it is often male-biased, females have a higher chance for successful re-mating, and so they are the deserting sex (Blashine and Blomqvist 1999; Pilastro et al. 2001; Webb et al. 2002). Opportunity for re-mating, however, reduces with progress of the breeding season (Szekely and Cuthill 2000; Griggio 2015) and for single-brooded species is non-existent. Examining such species may actually help to understand the most basic conditions for brood desertion.

The little auk (dovekie), Alle alle, is an example of a species for which the re-mating hypothesis fails to explain female brood desertion. For this small seabird, bi-parental care is crucial to raise the single chick successfully (Kidawa et al. 2012), and only at the end of the nesting period females desert their broods. Male parents continue the care and accompany their fledgling in its first flight to sea (Harding et al. 2004; Wojczulanis-Jakubas and Jakubas 2012), where it is believed they stay with the young for several weeks (Stempniewicz 2001). Little auks breed exclusively in the High Arctic zone (Stempniewicz 2001), during a short, two month long nesting period (4 weeks of incubation, and 3–4 weeks of chick rearing). They have to exploit the narrow window of time of the Arctic summer and do that with great precision. Females start egg-laying a week after ambient temperature achieves a constant (above 0 °C) level, and the fledging period terminates a week before ambient temperature drops again below 0 °C level (Moe et al. 2009). In these conditions, a second breeding attempt is impossible. Even re-nesting birds that lost their brood very early in the season, have chicks with lower body mass and survival rate compared to the chicks from non-replacement broods (Jakubas and Wojczulanis-Jakubas 2013). Thus, little auk females deserting their brood do not benefit from re-mating.

Another hypothesis to explain brood desertion is that it is triggered by decline in a parent’s body condition (Wojczulanis-Jakubas et al. 2012). Indeed, body condition of provisioning adults has been proven to affect decisions about brood desertion in other avian species (body condition hypothesis), and there are two forms of that relationship. The first is when the parent deserting the brood is in relatively good body condition, which favours re-mating (Barta et al. 2002; Bleeker et al. 2005). The second form is that the parent deserts the brood due to poor body condition, thus, leaving the brood secures its own survival (Gratto-Trevor 1991; Pierce 1997; Roskaft 2006). However, in little auks no evidence has been found to support the body condition hypothesis in either form. First, parental effort of males and females is similar, if not male-biased (Harding et al. 2004; Welcker et al. 2009b; Wojczulanis-Jakubas et al. 2009, 2014a), which partly eliminates a potential reason for the expected poorer body condition of females. Secondly, the body condition of females has been analysed in detail, and their size-adjusted body mass, haematological parameters, and stress hormones have all been found to be similar to those of males (Wojczulanis-Jakubas et al. 2012, 2014a, b, 2015). Moreover, body condition of females is not considerably worse at the end of the chick-rearing period compared to earlier stages of breeding (Wojczulanis-Jakubas et al. 2012). Even potentially costly egg production by the female (the egg mass constitutes 20% of female body mass; Stempniewicz 2001), seems to be compensated early in the season (Jakubas et al. 2008; Wojczulanis-Jakubas et al. 2014b). However, even if females do less than males, and even if their body condition is quite good at the end of the chick rearing period (and similar to that of males), it is still possible they are more prone than males to costs of breeding, and so may pay for those costs during the wintering period (Wojczulanis-Jakubas and Jakubas 2012). This sex-dependent cost of breeding may be related to basic sex differences in anatomy and physiology (i.e. females are slightly smaller and have a different hormonal balance). Thus, female little auks could desert their broods to secure their own future survival (as suggested for semipalmated sandpipers, Calidris pusilla, Gratto-Trevor 1991). The male partner might also benefit from female desertion, as the next season pair members are very likely to breed together (Stempniewicz 2001).

Thus, in this study we examined a possible relationship between the duration of female parental care over the chick and her wintering survival. For that purpose we analysed the duration of parental care over the chick in regard to adult wintering survival at two colonies, Hornsund and Magdalenefjorden (Spitsbergen, Norway). The two colonies located along the West coast of Spitsbergen, ca 300 km apart, differ in terms of environmental conditions in the little auk foraging areas (Jakubas et al. 2011). In Hornsund, the birds forage in the shelf area, which is primarily influenced by cold water masses, abundant in energy rich, Arctic zooplankton. In Magdalenefjorden area the shelf is more influenced by warm water masses, which are dominated by smaller, less caloric, Atlantic zooplankton (Kwasniewski et al. 2010). The inter-colony differences in foraging conditions have the potential to differentiate birds’ parental effort (Karnovsky et al. 2010, 2011; Hovinen et al. 2014b; Kidawa et al. 2015; Amélineau et al. 2016), which, in turn, may affect duration of parental care and/or adult survival. Although, in an evolutionary sense, long-lived species, such as the little auk, are predicted to prioritize self-maintenance and thus survival, at the cost of current reproduction (Beaulieu et al. 2011; Ghalambor and Martin 2016), parents may increase their parental investment in the short term when it is necessary, to secure the current breeding attempt. A study on little auk parents with experimentally increased flight costs during the chick-rearing period revealed that they are able to increase parental effort to some extent (Harding et al. 2009). Also, it has been revealed that artificially increased chick-begging intensity (by the exogenous corticosterone administration) induced a higher feeding frequency by little auk parents (Kidawa et al. 2017). An extension of parental effort, however, may affect parents’ survival. Thus, we assumed that duration of female parental care in the little auk, even if related to the future survival of the female, is flexible. Indeed, the duration of parental care over the chick varies among females (Wojczulanis-Jakubas and Jakubas 2012) and seems to be different in Hornsund and Magdalenefjorden. Although it has not been examined deliberately in Hornsund, and this study is the first investigating it systematically, existing data suggest that females from that colony disappear earlier (a week before chicks fledge; Harding et al. 2004) than females from Magdalenefjorden, where the issue was analysed in detail (0–4 days before chicks fledge; Wojczulanis-Jakubas and Jakubas 2012).

Examining in detail the duration of parental care over the chick provided by little auk females in the two colonies, we expected there would be inter-colony differences. We hypothesised that females from Hornsund, who invest in chick-rearing for a shorter amount of time, have higher wintering survival compared to females from Magdalenefjorden. As a control, we compared female vs male survival in the two colonies, expecting no sex differences in the colony with a more favourable foraging conditions in Hornsund, and higher survival of males than females in Magdalenefjorden colony with less favourable foraging conditions. Moreover, to further comprehend a triggering mechanism of brood desertion by little auk females, we examined the duration of female parental care over the chick in relation to the parental effort (foraging trip frequency and duration).

Materials and methods

Study area

The two breeding colonies in which we carried out the present study, Hornsund (SW Spitsbergen, 77°00’ N, 15° 33′ E) and Magdalenefjorden (NW Spitsbergen, 79° 35’N, 11°05′ E), are both considered the biggest breeding aggregations of the little auk in Spitsbergen (Keslinka et al. 2019). Both colonies are under the influence of two currents – the Sørkapp Current which carries cold, less saline Arctic water, and the West Spitsbergen Current (an extension of the Norwegian Atlantic Current), which transports warmer, more saline Atlantic water (Cottier et al. 2005; Piechura and Walczowski 2009). However, due to an oceanographic system of the water masses, the range of the two currents are different for the two colonies. The Hornsund area is primarily under influence of the cold water current, while Magdalenefjorden area is dominated by the warm one (Tuomo and Harald 2001; Walczowski and Piechura 2007) (Fig. S1). Thus, the birds from Hornsund utilize mainly cold water masses with energy-rich prey within the shelf area (8–93 km from the colony; Karnovsky et al. 2003; Jakubas et al. 2013, 2014), while those from Magdalenefjorden forage in less profitable warm water masses (Kwasniewski et al. 2010). Besides, birds from Magdalenefjorden also utilize more distant but profitable foraging areas, at the edge of the sea ice (150 km from the colony; Jakubas et al. 2012, 2013). Consequently, to meet chick energy requirements, birds from Magdalenefjorden (compared to those from Hornsund), tend to perform fewer chick feedings and make longer foraging trips [in terms of the distance (Jakubas et al. 2013) but not necessarily in terms of time (Kidawa et al. 2015; see also a comparison of study seasons in the supplementary materials)].

Fieldwork

It was not possible to record data blind because our study involved focal animals in the field, nevertheless the sex of birds during the observations was known and the person (KWJ) performing molecular sexing was oblivious of birds identity.

To examine food provisioning and/or duration of parental care of little auk males and females, we performed direct 24–48 h continuous observations in the two colonies (Hornsund and Magdalenefjorden; for more details about the observations see Wojczulanis-Jakubas and Jakubas 2012) and/or continuous 48 h video recording (Hornsund) of individually marked individuals (Table 1; the mode of registering birds behaviour was related to logistics). For that purpose, ca two weeks before the onset of the first observation/recording session in a given season, we marked both breeding partners at focal nests using the system of plastic rings on legs and marks painted on the breast feathers, both of unique colour combinations (see Table 1 for details on sample sizes). This marking system allowed us to reliably recognize an individual if present in the nest site area. All these birds were also registered in a capture-recapture database (see details below).

Table 1 Schedule, sample size and duration for the direct observations and video recordings. Number of nests in brackets indicate the same nest being monitored for the two stages of the chick rearing period in given season
Table 2 The three top models with ∆ AIC < 2 for the full data set (see Table S3 for a ranking of all the fitted models)

We followed all the focal nests for hatching and fledging by daily inspection of the nest content starting a week before the expected time of hatching and fledging. Thus, we knew the age of the chicks in all the nests with 1-day precision. In all the nests we followed the male partner if he continued the care after the female desertion and recorded whether the chick fledged successfully.

During observations, we continuously observed focal individuals and noted their presence in the colony every 10 min. We recorded all events of chick-feeding [bird appears after a period of absence in the colony, and enters the nest with a gular pouch filled with food; see more details on the observation routine in (Wojczulanis-Jakubas and Jakubas 2012; Grissot et al. 2019)]. For automatic recording of colony attendance of birds and chick feeding frequency, we set video cameras (four types, commercial HD models, with 1-s time lapse mode) at each focal nest. The camera recorded a 3 m radius of the focal nest entrance continuously for ≥ 48 h. We manually screened the video material in search of marked individuals using VLC software (VideoLAN, France). As for the direct observations, we noted the birds’ presence/absence and feeding frequency but with 1-s precision.

To analyse little auk survival, we used capture-recapture data collected in seasons 2001–2017 in Hornsund and 2007–2011 in Magdalenefjorden. The birds considered in the present study were captured by hand while incubating and/or brooding in nests. We considered only nests for which we could identity both members of the pair. We ringed each new capture with a unique number (Stavanger, Norway) and we took a small blood or feather sample for the purpose of molecular sexing (following the methods described in Jakubas and Wojczulanis 2007). In total, we considered in the analysis 737 birds from Hornsund, and 269 from Magdalenefjorden (for details see Tables S1, S2).

Data analysis

Data sources. Data on Hornsund female brood desertion are analysed here for the first time, while descriptive data on the duration of female parental care in Magdalenefjorden have already been presented in Wojczulanis-Jakubas and Jakubas 2012. Here, data from Magdalenefjorden are used in comparison with Hornsund.

Duration of female parental care over the chick. Since the exact day of colony departure was not known for all the females (for some females there was 1–4 days gap between the last observation and the first day of confirmed absence; see Fig. 1 mid panel for exact number of these females for each colony) we treated these records as censored data (i.e. unknown exact time of female desertion) in the Cox model (Fig. 1 bottom panel). For all the analyses, where we considered duration of female parental care over the chick, we averaged the last observation of the female and the first day of her confirmed absence, and we refer to this as the estimated duration of female parental care over the chick (also estimated chick age at the female desertion).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Probability of female brood desertion (upper panel), number of females observed in the colony (middle panel), and number of censored data (i.e. as uncompleted associated with lower weight in the analysis; bottom panel) at successive days of chick’s life in Hornsund (blue) and Magdalenefjorden colony (orange)

In Hornsund, we collected data on the duration of female parental care for three seasons (2004, 2016, 2017), and in Magdalenefjorden for two seasons (2009 and 2010) (Table 1). There were inter-seasonal differences in the duration of female parental care (but not in duration of the nesting period) in the Hornsund colony, where the period of chick rearing by females increased over the years. For Magdalenefjorden we did not observe such differences (Fig. S3, S4). This inter-seasonal difference in the Hornsund colony may be an effect of inter-annual fluctuations in foraging conditions but it is also possible that ongoing climate change are having an effect. If female brood desertion is a flexible trait that is related to environmental conditions, as we advocate in the present study, deteriorating environmental conditions in the Hornsund area (indeed the annual influx of warm water masses in Hornsund is greater at present than was observed a decade ago; Promińska et al. 2017) might explain the longer duration of female parental care. Nevertheless, for the purposes of the present study, we pooled data from all the seasons for each colony so we could compare various traits between the two colonies without the loss of degrees of freedom.

To compare duration of female parental care between the two colonies we fitted a Cox proportional hazards regression model using survminer (Kassambara et al. 2018) and survival (Therneau and Lumley 2019) packages in R software (R Core Team 2018). We tested this relationship with a likelihood ratio test (LRT) and considered it significant at p value of <0.05. Since inter-colony differences in duration of female parental care may be related to inter-colony differences in duration of the nesting period, we also compared the duration of nesting period between the two colonies. To do so, we performed a permutation test, randomizing the duration of nesting period between the colonies, calculating the inter-colony difference and then comparing it with the observed one in total of 1000 iterations. If the observed difference in duration of the nesting period between the two colonies was within the central 95% differences obtained in the randomisation procedure, then we considered it statistically insignificant.

Survival. To analyse winter survival of adult males and females from the two colonies (capture-recapture data), we used the Cormack-Jolly-Seber model (CJS) (see, for example, McCrea and Morgan 2014), which provides estimates of annual survival (ϕ) whilst accounting for imperfect detection (capture probability p). We fitted models with all possible combinations of site and sex covariates in the survival ϕ and capture parameter p. We compared the models using an information-theoretic approach, with AIC as the chosen criterion (Zuur et al. 2009), with ∆AIC = AIC – min (AIC). We further considered only models with ∆AIC < 2 (Burnham and Anderson 2002).

The differences in the number of years for each data set (Hornsund: 15, Magdalenefjorden: 5) did not affect the parameter estimates obtained from the capture-recapture model as we did not look at time-dependent results. To check our results, we also fitted the data sets separately (i.e. run the same models for each colony) and we found that the estimates were similar to the ones presented in results section.

We also fitted models to the subset of capture-recapture data containing only records of females, for which we collected data of their duration of parental care (i.e. 56 females, with 25 from Hornsund, and 31 from Magdalenefjorden; hereafter the female subset). However, for these data the site and the duration of female parental care covariates were correlated. A solution to address this collinearity issue would be to fit a capture-recapture model in which survival was dependent on the duration of female parental care for each site separately. However, we were unable to do so for two reasons. First, we were unable to estimate capture probability for the female subset from Magdalenefjorden (the data did not contain zeroes between capture occasions). Second, when we fit the capture-recapture model for the Hornsund female subset the survival parameter estimates reached the boundaries. Because of all these limitations we included the data for the 56 females in our analysis but we did not consider site covariates, being aware of the possible effect of muliticolinearity (thus these results should be treated with caution). In this analysis, the survival probability depended on the presence covariate (i.e. estimated chick age at the female desertion) and the capture probability was constant. For significance, we established 95% confidence intervals for the regression line with a bootstrap method. We performed all this survival modelling in R (R Core Team 2018).

Parental effort. To analyse the relationship between a female parental effort and her duration of parental care over the chick, we focused on the number of chick feedings performed by each female per 24 h, and the duration of her foraging trips. For this analysis, we used data for only two of the three seasons for Hornsund, as we did not have data from the mid-chick rearing period in 2004. We calculated the number of feedings per 24 h from the 48 h unit of observation/recordings, counting the number of feedings in the whole unit performed by the female and dividing it by 2. Since duration of foraging trips in the little auk has a bimodal distribution (Steen et al. 2007; Welcker et al. 2009a; Wojczulanis-Jakubas et al. 2010; Brown et al. 2012) we established division for short- and long-lasting trips. As the distribution of duration of foraging trips may be season- or site- dependent (Welcker et al. 2009a), initially we established the cut-off value for each season/colony separately, based on the algorithm proposed by Welcker et al. (2009a), where the best cut-off value is that which minimizes the sum of the variances of both trip types given their log-normal distribution. It resulted in four different cut-off values: 7.5 h (Hornsund 2016), 7.9 h (Hornsund 2017), 6.6 h (Magdalenefjorden 2009), and 7.3 h (Magdalenefjorden 2010) (Fig. S2). However, these cut-off values did not differ remarkably, therefore, we also considered a single cut-off value for the whole data set (set on 8.8 h) following Wojczulanis-Jakubas et al. (2010). This single cut-off value is based on the assumption that the long trips performed during the chick-rearing period serve primarily the adults’ self-maintenance (although the birds bring food for the chick from long trips too), and so they should last as long as the foraging trips performed by adults during the incubation period, when the birds are free of chick provisioning duty (Wojczulanis-Jakubas et al. 2010). Since, both approaches to trips split gave qualitatively the same results in the analysis of the relationship between the duration of foraging trips of a female and her duration of parental care over the chick, for simplicity we finally presented results based on the single cut-off value split.

We analysed the estimated duration of female parental care over the chick using linear regression (linear model; LM), with the number of chick-feedings and colony as explanatory variables. We also included in the model the interaction between the colony and the number of feeds by females, so we could model differences on the effect of feeding rates on the estimated duration of female parental care between the sites. Then, we analysed the relationship between the estimated duration of female parental care and the duration of foraging trips for females, using linear mixed models, separately for each trip type. We regressed the duration of female parental care with duration of the trip and the colony as explanatory variables. To control for a possible effect of pseudoreplication (multiple foraging trips performed by each female), we included female identity in the model as a random factor. The interaction between the colony and the mean duration of trips was also included in the model, so we could model differences on the effect of the mean duration of the trip on the estimated duration of female parental care over the chick among both sites. We checked assumptions of all the models using visual inspection of the residuals.

We performed all the analysis in R environment (R Core Team 2018), using ggplot2 (Wickham et al. 2018) and ggpubr (Kassambara 2018) packages for plotting.

Results

Duration of female parental care. Overall, females deserted their chick when it was between 16 and 30 days old. On average, females in Hornsund deserted the brood three days earlier than females from Magdalenefjorden. The mean estimated chick age at the female desertion was 22.6 (SE ± 0.52) in Hornsund and 25.6 (SE ± 0.42) in Magdalenefjorden. Even considering that some data were censored (i.e. being uncompleted had a different weight in the analysis), the inter-colony difference held true (LRT = 10.36, p < 0.001; Fig. 1). In both colonies, chicks fledged on average on 26 day of life (Hornsund: SE = 0.37, Magdalenefjorden: SE = 0.31; Fig. 2, and Fig. S4 for values separate for seasons). This difference in chicks age at the moment of female brood desertion resulted in proportionally longer periods of male-only care in Hornsund. Consistently, as female little auks stayed longer with the brood in Magdalenefjorden, some of Magdalenefjorden chicks did not experience uni-parental care.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Density distribution of the duration of nesting period (i.e. chicks age at fledging) in Hornsund and Magdalenefjorden colony (left panel), and bootstrap distribution of the inter-colony difference in mean day of the chicks fledging (right panel); the black vertical line at 0, being within the range of the permutated distribution, indicates no significant difference between the two colonies in the duration of the nesting period

Survival. We first considered the capture-recapture data for all individuals. We fitted all the possible models, which included all combinations in terms of site and sex covariates in the survival (ϕ) and the capture probability (p). The three best models in terms of AIC (Table 2; further results are given in the supplementary material in Table S3) included site and sex dependencies in both parameters. For the best model, both survival and capture probabilities depended on the site covariate. The survival probability for this model was estimated higher in Hornsund (ϕ = 0.75, SE = 0.07) than in Magdalenefjorden (ϕ = 0.44, SE = 0.10). In contrast, the capture probability was estimated lower in Hornsund (p = 0.34, SE = 0.07) than in Magdalenefjorden (p = 0.76, SE = 0.25). In the second best model, survival again only depended on the site covariate, and for this parameter the estimates were identical to those in the top model. On the other hand, in the second best model, the capture probability depended both on the sex and site covariates, and the corresponding capture probabilities were p = 0.33 (SE = 0.10) for females in Hornsund and p = 0.35 (SE = 0.10) for males in Hornsund; p = 0.75 (SE = 0.26) for females in Magdalenefjorden and p = 0.77 (SE = 0.26) for males in the same site. In the third best model the survival probability depended on both covariates, and the capture probability depended only on the site covariate. In this model the survival probabilities were ϕ = 0.75 (SE = 0.09) for both females and males in Hornsund, and ϕ = 0.43 (SE = 0.11) for females in Magdalenefjorden and ϕ = 0.44 (SE = 0.12) for males in the same site. The capture probabilities were similar to those in the best model. These results show that the probability of survival was much higher in Hornsund than in Magdalenefjorden, however the capture probability was much lower in Hornsund. The estimates obtained in the second and third best models in terms of AIC suggest that survival and capture probabilities were similar for both sexes.

Considering the female subset data (with both colonies pooled) we found a negative trend in the relationship between the duration of female parental care over the chick and their survival, (α = 1.20 (0.19), βpresence = −0.25 (0.19), where alpha and beta are the intercept and slope respectively corresponding to the logit transformed survival probability; Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Estimated survival probability for both sites as a function of the estimated duration of female parental care over the chick (expressed as estimated chick age at the female desertion). The 95% bootstrapped confidence interval is shown by the dashed lines

Parental effort. There was no apparent relationship between duration of female parental care over the chick and the number of chick-feedings or the duration of short or long foraging trips performed in the mid-chick rearing period (Table 3; Fig. S5, S6).

Table 3 Output of models describing association between estimated time of colony departure of females and colony/N feedings/short trips/long trips/interactions. N feedings – number of the chick feedings performed by female per 24 h, LT – long foraging trips, ST – short trips

Discussion

As expected, we found little auk females from Hornsund departed the colony on average 3 days earlier that those from Magdalenefjorden. Since duration of nesting period was similar in the two colonies, this difference indicated proportionally longer parental investment of females in the Magdalenefjorden colony. The two colonies differ in terms of environmental conditions in the little auk foraging areas, with Hornsund having usually more favourable conditions (see, for example, Jakubas et al. 2011; Kidawa et al. 2015). Thus, the inter-colony difference in the duration of female parental care over the chick seems to be related to these conditions, rather than a feature of the colony. The inter-seasonal difference in the duration of female parental care over the chick in Hornsund (Fig. S3) seems to support this conclusion. Thus, female little auks stay with the brood longer in less favourable conditions, probably in this way compensating for food shortages. In other words, they stay with the brood as long as it is necessary. Adjustment of parental behaviour by female little auks to current environmental conditions is consistent with the great flexibility of the species observed in various contexts related to foraging (Jakubas et al. 2011, 2016, 2020; Brown et al. 2012; Gremillet et al. 2012; Amélineau et al. 2016).

Although female brood desertion as a response to environmental condition, being a background question of the present study, seems to be the most parsimonious explanation of the overall variation in the duration of little auk female parental care over the chick, we cannot also exclude another factor also influencing female behaviour. In Magdalenefjorden as well as in Hornsund there are both early- and late-deserting females. An alternative (or additional) explanation to the observed variation could be a variation coordination within pairs of parental responsibilities. It has been found that degree of parental coordination is strongly associated with the ultimate consequence of sexual conflict such as brood desertion, with low coordination with mates resulting in earlier brood desertion of one parent (Baldan and Griggio 2019). A recent study on little auk parental strategies has showed that parents do coordinate their chick provisioning, avoiding overlapping of long foraging trips in regard to each other (i.e. securing even in time food delivery; Wojczulanis-Jakubas et al. 2018). Parental coordination has been found to be highly variable and could not be explained by environmental conditions (Grissot et al. 2019). In such a context, female brood desertion in the little auk could be linked to low coordination. We are not currently able to test for such a possibility but this issue definitely warrants a thorough study.

Although long-lived species are not expected to prioritize offspring care at the cost of their own survival (Beaulieu et al. 2011; Ghalambor and Martin 2016), in the short term they could extend their parental investment in order to ensure that the current breeding attempt is successful. In this context, we expected that female little auks from Magdalenefjorden colony would extend their parental care to secure the current breeding attempt and that could further influence their winter survival. We found that, indeed, the relationship between survival probability and estimated duration of female parental care over the chick was negative - the longer the female stayed with the brood the lower was the probability of her survival. However, due to data limitations explained above, we were unable to obtain any site-specific results. Therefore, to confirm this relationship, future work should focus on the collection of additional data for several more seasons. Besides, the duration of female parental care over the chick was not related to their parental effort earlier in the chick rearing period, and survival of males and females was similar both in Hornsund and Magdalenefjorden colony. Thus, although female brood desertion and winter survival are linked, the relationship is not that straightforward.

Survival of birds from Magdalenefjorden was in general lower compared to Hornsund. This difference is likely to be related to the difference in environmental conditions between the two colonies (Jakubas et al. 2011; Hovinen et al. 2014a; Kidawa et al. 2015). Although both colonies experience inter-annual differences in environmental conditions, overall, parental birds in Magdalenefjorden work harder than those in Hornsund. The difference in effort is shown by more distant and sometimes also longer duration of foraging flights in the former colony (Jakubas et al. 2011, 2013; Kidawa et al. 2015; our results on difference in the duration of short foraging trips also support the conclusion). Thus, birds from Magdalenefjorden seem to pay a cost of breeding in less favourable foraging conditions in terms of their lower winter survival. Obviously, this may not be the only explanation for the inter-colony differences in the adult survival. We cannot exclude that, for instance, the difference is generated at the wintering grounds. Hovinen et al. (2014) have reported that annual survival rates of adult little auks breeding on Svalbard are influenced not only by summer sea surface temperatures (SST) but also winter values of the North Atlantic Oscillation (NAO) - an increase in both parameters is related to decreases in little auk survival. Although wintering areas of Magdalenefjorden population are yet undescribed, it is likely that birds from Hornsund utilize different wintering areas, given the high variability in the choice of wintering grounds described so far (Fort et al. 2013; P. Dufour et al. unpublished data). If so, birds from the two colonies might be exposed to different wintering conditions, which in turn might affect their survival.

Our results demonstrate that female brood desertion in the little auk is a flexible trait dependent on environmental conditions. However, although staying with the brood longer was negatively associated with their winter survival, the relationship was not that straightforward, as the duration of female parental care over the chick was not related to their parental efforts performed earlier in the chick rearing period, and survival of males and females was similar in both colonies despite distinct environmental conditions. These results all together suggest that little auk females rather do not jeopardize their survival by an extended parental care. Hence, the next step in examining the reasons why females desert the brood in the little auk should be perhaps reversing the question and making it male-oriented: why do males stay with their chicks longer than females?