Skip to main content
Log in

Effect of visuo-proprioceptive mismatch rate on recalibration in hand perception

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We estimate our hand’s position by combining relevant visual and proprioceptive cues. A cross-sensory spatial mismatch can be created by viewing the hand through a prism or, more recently, rotating a visual cursor that represents hand position. This is often done in the context of target-directed reaching to study motor adaptation, the systematic updating of motor commands in response to a systematic movement error. However, a visuo-proprioceptive mismatch also elicits recalibration in the relationship between the hand’s seen and felt position. The principles governing visuo-proprioceptive recalibration are poorly understood, compared to motor adaptation. For example, motor adaptation occurs robustly whether the cursor is rotated quickly or slowly, although the former may involve more explicit processes. Here, we asked whether visuo-proprioceptive recalibration, in the absence of motor adaptation, works the same way. Three groups experienced a 70 mm visuo-proprioceptive mismatch about their hand at a Slow, Medium, or Fast rate (0.84, 1.67, or 3.34 mm every two trials, respectively), with no error feedback. Once attained, the 70 mm mismatch was maintained for the remaining trials. Total recalibration differed significantly across groups, with the Fast, Medium, and Slow groups recalibrating 63.7, 56.3, and 42.8 mm on average, respectively. This suggests a slower mismatch rate may be less effective at eliciting recalibration. In contrast to motor adaptation studies, no further recalibration was observed in the maintenance phase. This may be related to the distinct mechanisms thought to contribute to perceptual recalibration via cross-sensory cue conflict versus sensory prediction errors.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Availability of data and materials

The data and analysis code used in this manuscript are available at https://osf.io/te4kp/.

References

  • Abeele S, Bock O (2001) Sensorimotor adaptation to rotated visual input: different mechanisms for small versus large rotations. Exp Brain Res 140:407–410

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alais D, Burr D (2004) The ventriloquist effect results from near-optimal bimodal integration. Curr Biol 14:257–262

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Beers RJ, Sittig AC, van der Gon JJD (1999) Localization of a seen finger is based exclusively on proprioception and on vision of the finger. Exp Brain Res 125:43–49

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Beers RJ, Wolpert DM, Haggard P (2002) When feeling is more important than seeing in sensorimotor adaptation. Curr Biol 12:834–837

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Block HJ, Bastian AJ (2010) Sensory reweighting in targeted reaching: effects of conscious effort, error history, and target salience. J Neurophysiol 103:206–217

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Block HJ, Bastian AJ (2011) Sensory weighting and realignment: independent compensatory processes. J Neurophysiol 106:59–70

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Block H, Bastian A, Celnik P (2013) Virtual lesion of angular gyrus disrupts the relationship between visuoproprioceptive weighting and realignment. J Cogn Neurosci 25:636–648

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Block HJ, Liu Y (2023) Visuo-proprioceptive recalibration and the sensorimotor map. J Neurophysiol 129:1249–1258

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Block HJ, Sexton BM (2020) Visuo-proprioceptive control of the hand in older adults. Multisensory Research 34:93–111

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Butler AA, Héroux ME, Gandevia SC (2017) Body ownership and a new proprioceptive role for muscle spindles. Acta Physiol 220:19–27

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cressman EK, Henriques DYP (2009) Sensory recalibration of hand position following visuomotor adaptation. J Neurophysiol 102:3505–3518

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cressman EK, Henriques DYP (2010) Reach adaptation and proprioceptive recalibration following exposure to misaligned sensory input. J Neurophysiol 103:1888–1895

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cressman EK, Salomonczyk D, Henriques DYP (2010) Visuomotor adaptation and proprioceptive recalibration in older adults. Exp Brain Res 205:533–544

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crowe A, Keessen W, Kuus W, van Vliet R, Zegeling A (1987) Proprioceptive accuracy in two dimensions. Percept Mot Skills 64:831–846

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ernst MO, Banks MS (2002) Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature 415:429–433

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foley JM, Held R (1972) Visually directed pointing as a function of target distance, direction, and available cues. Percept Psychophys 12:263–268

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ghahramani Z, Wolpert DM, Jordan MI (1997) Computational models for sensorimotor integration. In: Morasso PG, Sanguineti V (eds) Self-Organization, Computational Maps and Motor Control. North-Holland, Amsterdam, pp 117–147

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ghahramani Z (1995). Computation and psychophysics of sensorimotor integration. MIT1995

  • Harris CS (1963) Adaptation to displaced vision: visual, motor, or proprioceptive change? Science 140:812–813

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hsiao A, Lee-Miller T, Block HJ (2022) Conscious awareness of a visuo-proprioceptive mismatch: effect on cross-sensory recalibration. Frontiers in Neuroscience 16. https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2022.958513. Accessed 8 Sep 2022

  • Kaernbach C, Munka L, Cunningham D (2002) Visuomotor adaptation: Dependency on motion trajectory. In: Dynamic perception. St. Augustin, pp 177–182. http://www.uni-kiel.de/psychologie/emotion/team/kaernbach/publications/2002_kae&al_bochum.pdf

  • Kagerer FA, Contreras-Vidal JL, Stelmach GE (1997) Adaptation to gradual as compared with sudden visuo-motor distortions. Exp Brain Res 115:557–561

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kilteni K, Maselli A, Kording KP, Slater M (2015) Over my fake body: body ownership illusions for studying the multisensory basis of own-body perception. Front Hum Neurosci 9:141

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • van der Kooij K, Brenner E, van Beers RJ, Schot WD, Smeets JBJ (2013) Alignment to natural and imposed mismatches between the senses. J Neurophysiol 109:1890–1899

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krakauer JW, Ghilardi MF, Ghez C (1999) Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching. Nat Neurosci 2:1026–1031

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liu Y, Sexton BM, Block HJ (2018) Spatial bias in estimating the position of visual and proprioceptive targets. J Neurophysiol 119:1879–1888

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin TA, Keating JG, Goodkin HP, Bastian AJ, Thach WT (1996) Throwing while looking through prisms. I. Focal olivocerebellar lesions impair adaptation. Brain 119(PT4):1183–1198

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mirdamadi J, Seigel C, Husch S, Block HJ (2022) Somatotopic specificity of perceptual and neurophysiological changes associated with visuo-proprioceptive realignment. Cereb Cortex. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhab280

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mostafa AA, ’t Hart BM, Henriques DYP (2019) Motor learning without moving: proprioceptive and predictive hand localization after passive visuoproprioceptive discrepancy training. PLoS ONE 14:e0221861

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Munoz-Rubke F, Mirdamadi JL, Lynch AK, Block HJ (2017) Modality-specific changes in motor cortex excitability after visuo-proprioceptive realignment. J Cogn Neurosci 29:2054–2067

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Näätänen R (2001) The perception of speech sounds by the human brain as reflected by the mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic equivalent (MMNm). Psychophysiology 38:1–21

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9:97–113

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Redding GM, Wallace B (1996) Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 22:379–394

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rossi C, Bastian AJ, Therrien AS (2021) Mechanisms of proprioceptive realignment in human motor learning. Curr Opin Physio 20:186–197

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruttle JE, ’t Hart BM, Henriques DYP (2018) The fast contribution of visual-proprioceptive discrepancy to reach aftereffects and proprioceptive recalibration. PLoS ONE 13:e0200621

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Salarian A (2023) Repeated Measures ANOVA (https://www.mathworks.com/matlabcentral/fileexchange/22088-repeated-measures-anova), MATLAB Central File Exchange. Retrieved Aug 14 2023

  • Salomonczyk D, Cressman EK, Henriques DYP (2011) Proprioceptive recalibration following prolonged training and increasing distortions in visuomotor adaptation. Neuropsychologia 49:3053–3062

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salomonczyk D, Henriques DYP, Cressman EK (2012) Proprioceptive recalibration in the right and left hands following abrupt visuomotor adaptation. Exp Brain Res 217(2):187–196. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2985-4

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salomonczyk D, Cressman EK, Henriques DYP (2013) The role of the cross-sensory error signal in visuomotor adaptation. Exp Brain Res 228:313–325

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Samad M, Chung AJ, Shams L (2015) Perception of body ownership is driven by Bayesian sensory inference. PLoS ONE 10:e0117178

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Smeets J, van den Dobbelsteen JJ, de Grave DDJ, van Beers RV, Brenner E (2006) Sensory integration does not lead to sensory calibration. Proc Natl Acad Sci 103:18781–18786

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Smith MA, Ghazizadeh A, Shadmehr R (2006) Interacting adaptive processes with different timescales underlie short-term motor learning. PLoS Biol 4(6). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0040179

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Stefanics G, Kremláček J, Czigler I (2014) Visual mismatch negativity: a predictive coding view. Front Hum Neurosci 8:666

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • The MathWorks Inc. (2019) MATLAB version: 9.7.0 (R2019b), Natick, Massachusetts: The MathWorks Inc. https://www.mathworks.com

  • Tiitinen H, May P, Reinikainen K, Näätänen R (1994) Attentive novelty detection in humans is governed by pre-attentive sensory memory. Nature 372:90–92

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wali M, Lee-Miller T, Babu R, Block HJ (2023) Retention of visuo-proprioceptive recalibration in estimating hand position. Sci Rep 13:6097

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Welch RB (1978) Perceptual Modification. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Welch RB (1986) Adaptation of space perception. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Welch RB, Warren DH (1980) Immediate perceptual response to intersensory discrepancy. Psychol Bull 88:638–667

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welch RB, Widawski MH, Harrington J, Warren DH (1979) An examination of the relationship between visual capture and prism adaptation. PerceptPsychophys 25:126–132

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Yin PB, Kitazawa S (2001) Long-lasting aftereffects of prism adaptation in the monkey. Exp Brain Res 141(2):250–253. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002210100892

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

Funding was provided by National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (Grant No. R01 NS112367).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

RB: conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, investigation, methodology, software, visualization, writing—original draft. TLM: formal analysis, writing–review and editing. MW: conceptualization, writing—review and editing. HJB: conceptualization, formal analysis, funding acquisition, supervision, writing—review and editing.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hannah J. Block.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors have no competing interests to declare that are relevant to the content of this article.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was approved by the Indiana University Institutional Review Board and performed in accordance with the ethical standards as laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments. Written informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Consent for publication

The authors affirm that participants gave informed consent for their de-identified data to be shared for research purposes.

Additional information

Communicated by Winston D Byblow.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Babu, R., Lee-Miller, T., Wali, M. et al. Effect of visuo-proprioceptive mismatch rate on recalibration in hand perception. Exp Brain Res 241, 2299–2309 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-023-06685-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-023-06685-8

Keywords

Navigation