Introduction

Dysregulation of the serotonergic system has been implicated in the pathophysiology of depression (Maes and Meltzer 1995). This is based on studies reporting lower plasma availability of the serotonin (5-HT) precursor tryptophan for uptake into the brain, reduced cerebrospinal fluid concentration of the 5-HT metabolite 5-hydroxyindoleacetic (5-HIAA) and decreased platelet 5-HT uptake in depressed patients, suggesting diminished brain 5-HT uptake and metabolism (Maes and Meltzer 1995; Neumeister et al. 2004). The level of 5-HT in the synaptic cleft is mainly regulated by the 5-HT transporter (5-HTT), which is targeted by most antidepressants (Meyer et al. 2004; Suhara et al. 2003; Voineskos et al. 2007). Interestingly, the human 5-HTT gene, located on chromosome 17q11.1–q12 (Lesch et al. 1994), contains a 5-HTT-linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) with two functional variants: the short (s) form and the long (l) form (Greenberg et al. 1999; Heils et al. 1996). The s form of this variant is less active, resulting in reduced transcriptional efficiency of the 5-HTT gene, decreased 5-HTT expression, and reduced 5-HT uptake relative to the l form (Greenberg et al. 1999; Heils et al. 1996). The s allele has been associated with increased risk of depression (Collier et al. 1996; Gonda et al. 2005, 2006; Joiner et al. 2003), poorer responses to the antidepressant effects of SSRIs (Serretti et al. 2007; Smeraldi et al. 1998), increased vulnerability to tryptophan depletion (Neumeister et al. 2002; Roiser et al. 2006), and abnormal emotional processing (Hariri et al. 2002; Marsh et al. 2006).

A direct association between 5-HTTLPR genotypes and depression-related phenotypes appears to be relatively weak (Lotrich and Pollock 2004). Roiser et al. (2005) only found abnormal emotional processing in 3,4 methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, or Ecstasy) users carrying the s allele. MDMA prevents uptake of 5-HT and stimulates the release of 5-HT, causing long-term changes to the 5-HT system. This suggests that s allele carriers may only have a predisposition to serotonergic vulnerability that may be unmasked through challenges to the serotonergic system. This is also in line with contemporary theoretical models of gene involvement in depression that postulate that genetic vulnerability does not affect depression directly; rather, 5-HTTLPR may enhance susceptibility for depression particularly in the face of severe life events or stressful experiences, which may also challenge the brain serotonergic system (Firk and Markus 2007). Hence, depression is often preceded by stress (Van Praag 2004), and epidemiological studies have already demonstrated a positive association between the s allele of the 5-HTTLPR and a depressive reaction to stressful life events (e.g., Caspi et al. 2003; for a recent review see Uher and McGuffin 2008). This is in line with the hypothesis that stress may cause depression due to complex interactions between the neuroendocrine stress system and the serotonergic system, particularly in individuals with a biological (genetic) vulnerability related to their serotonergic system (Firk and Markus 2007; Van Praag 2004). Although explorations of the involvement of interactions between serotonergic vulnerability in s allele 5-HTTLPR and stress in the onset of affective symptoms are a major challenge in biological psychiatry today, this interaction has not yet been experimentally investigated.

On the other hand, the relatively small direct association between 5-HTTLPR genotypes and depression-related phenotypes may also be explained by recently identified functional variants within the l allele (LA and LG) that may underestimate the effect of 5-HTTLPR. This is because the LG allele has a low rate of 5-HTT expression comparable to the s allele and is relatively common in Caucasian and African American populations (Hu et al. 2006; Zalsman et al. 2006).

One accepted method to study behavioral and cognitive effects of transiently reduced 5-HT availability in humans is the acute tryptophan depletion (ATD) strategy (for a review see Fusar-Poli et al. 2006; Young et al. 1985). In this procedure, brain 5-HT is reduced through depletion of plasma tryptophan (TRP, a precursor of 5-HT) availability for uptake into the brain by intake of a TRP-free amino acid mixture that contains all of the essential amino acids except for TRP. This raises incorporation of TRP into protein synthesis and reduces TRP relative to the sum of the other large neutral amino acids (LNAAs) with which TRP competes for uptake into the brain (i.e., Fernstrom and Wurtman 1971; Gessa et al. 1974; Maes and Meltzer 1995; Moja et al. 1991). Evidence for reduced 5-HT neurotransmission after ATD comes from studies measuring 5-HT metabolites (5-HIAA) in cerebrospinal fluid (Carpenter et al. 1998; Williams et al. 1999). In addition, there is also plenty of evidence showing that the ATD procedure is the most suitable method to study brain 5-HT vulnerability. For instance, mood-lowering effects of tryptophan depletion are clearly found in depressive patients in remission (Delgado et al. 1990, 1999; Moreno et al. 2000; Ruhe et al. 2007), whereas in healthy individuals such mood-lowering effects seem to depend on the 5-HTTLPR genotype (Neumeister et al. 2002, 2006; Roiser et al. 2007; Walderhaug et al. 2007) and family history of depression (Benkelfat et al. 1994; Klaassen et al. 1999; Neumeister et al. 2002). Individuals with a positive family history of depression are more vulnerable to the mood-lowering effects of ATD than individuals without a family history of depression, suggesting an (innate) vulnerability of the serotonergic system in relatives of depressive patients (Benkelfat et al. 1994; Klaassen et al. 1999; Neumeister et al. 2002). However, the effects of ATD on mood in individuals with different 5-HTTLPR profiles are contradictory. Neumeister et al. (2002) reported mood lowering in healthy s allele carriers, in particular in those with a positive family history of depression. Walderhaug et al. (2007) found mood lowering following ATD only in women homozygous for the l and s allele, whereas other studies did not find mood-lowering effects at all (Marsh et al. 2006; Roiser et al. 2007). Therefore, we suggest that the possession of the s allele, which is very common in the population, may not alone increase risk, but may induce enhanced susceptibility to ATD only in the context of stress.

The aim of the present study was to investigate the effects of ATD in subjects with different 5-HTTLPR profiles before and after stress exposure on affective and cognitive–attentional changes that are thought to be critical in the etiology and maintenance of depression. A selective attention and memory bias for negative information is the most commonly recognized characteristic of depression and depression vulnerability (e.g., Bradley et al. 1995; Mogg and Bradley 2005; Rinck and Becker 2005). For instance, depressive patients show memory biases for negative information in explicit memory free recall tasks (e.g., Bradley et al. 1995; Lim and Kim 2005) and slowed responses to happy words compared to sad words during affective go/no-go (AGNG) tasks (Murphy et al. 1999; Erickson et al. 2005). In addition, comparable attentional biases are also found in healthy individuals following ATD, further suggesting that serotonergic factors are particularly involved (Murphy et al. 2002). Based on the literature and the data reviewed above, we hypothesized that the 5-HTTLPR genotype mediates ATD- and stress-induced changes in mood and cognitive–behavioral indications of affective biases. To our knowledge, this is the first time the depressogenic effects of reducing brain 5-HT by ATD are investigated in 5-HT-vulnerable s allele 5-HTTLPR carriers before and after stress exposure.

Materials and methods

Participants

Undergraduate students at Maastricht University (N = 200) completed a screening questionnaire concerning general information (health, smoking and drinking habits, caffeine consumption, use of psychoactive drugs, past and present treatment with medication, weight and height, eating habits, and more). Furthermore, the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck et al. 1961) was completed to verify the absence of depressive symptomatology.

Participants were excluded if they reported chronic and current illness; history of psychiatric or medical illness; family history of depression; medication use; metabolic, hormonal, or intestinal diseases; irregular diets or deviant eating habits; drug use; or excessive (more than 10 units per week) alcohol use. Participants’ health was checked with standardized medical questionnaires that were evaluated by a trained doctoral-level psychologist under the supervision of a medical doctor. We acknowledge that, although participants reported not having any medical illness, a medical examination might have been a likely additional method to further ensure participants’ physical health. Participants included in the study revealed normal body mass indexes (BMI in kg/m2 between 19 and 26), were non-smokers and were requested not to use alcohol before or during the study.

From the subjects showing interest in taking part in the experiment and fulfilling to the in- and exclusion criteria, 86 participants were invited to attend an initial buccal sample extraction session. During this session, buccal cell samples were taken via swabs to genotype all individuals for 5-HTTLPR. Participants were compensated (€5) for participation in this buccal sample extraction phase of the study. All subjects had to give informed consent to participate during this buccal DNA extraction session.

Participants with the S/S, S/LG, or LG/LG genotype classified as S′/S′ and participants with the LA/LA genotype classified as L′/L′ were selected for the experiment and received oral and written information about the study. Eighteen subjects with the S′/S′ genotype and 17 subjects with the L′/L′ genotype completed the experiment. The S′/S′ group and the L′/L′ group did not differ with respect to sex, age, BMI, or BDI scores (all p > 0.05). Demographic characteristics are presented in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographic characteristics

The study was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of the Academic Hospital Maastricht (CTCM azM; Maastricht; The Netherlands), and the procedures followed were in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975 as revised in 1983. All subjects gave their informed consent to participate in the experiment and were paid 150€ for participation.

Design

A placebo-controlled, double-blind, crossover design was used. During two experimental sessions, subjects were monitored for affective processing before and after acute stress exposure either following intake of a TRP-free (TRP–) or a TRP-containing balanced (TRP+) amino acid mixture. The order of presentation of the TRP– and TRP+ condition was counterbalanced within groups and both experimental sessions were separated by at least 1 week. Before and 4.5 h after intake of the amino acid mixtures, blood samples were taken to measure the effect of ATD on plasma amino acid concentrations and the TRP/LNAA ratio. Female subjects were tested in the follicular phase of their menstrual cycle (days 4–10) or when actually taking oral contraceptives.

Procedure

All participants were instructed to fast overnight; only water or tea without sugar was permitted. On each experimental morning, participants arrived for testing at 08:30 a.m., 09:00 a.m., 09:30 a.m., and 10:00 a.m., respectively, and a first blood sample was taken to obtain baseline plasma TRP and LNAA levels. A second blood sample was drawn 4.5 h after intake of the amino acid mixture (TRP− or TRP+) to assess changes in total TRP concentrations and TRP/LNAA ratios. At baseline and 4.5 h following intake of the amino acid mixture, subjects completed a survey measuring vegetative side effects. Five hours after administration of the amino acid mixture, participants’ mood was assessed with the Profile of Mood States (POMS; Wald and Mellenbergh 1990) and the Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS; Watson et al. 1988). Mood assessment was followed by the affective go/no-go task (to measure attentional biases) and the emotional related free recall task (to measure memory biases). Then, participants were exposed to an uncontrollable stress task (Markus–Peters Arithmetic (MPA) task), again followed by mood assessment and the affective go/no-go task and the emotional related free recall task. Further, two pre-stress salivary cortisol samples were taken (at 4.5 h (t −30) and immediately before the MPA task (t 0)) and two post-stress cortisol samples were taken (immediately after the MPA task (t +20) and 10 min after finishing the MPA task (t +30)) to measure stress-related activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis. Between intake of the amino acid mixture and exposure to laboratory tasks, the subjects were able to study or to read magazines in a separate private room. They had free access to water and decaffeinated tea. Two hours after administration of the amino acid mixture, they received a standardized protein-poor lunch as previously used in ATD studies (Riedel et al. 1999; Sobczak et al. 2002a, b). At the end of each test day, subjects received a high protein snack and bananas, which are natural sources of l-tryptophan to facilitate a quick recovery from possible negative effects of ATD.

Acute tryptophan depletion

A reduction in brain 5-HT was accomplished by ATD through the use of a tryptophan-free collagen–protein (CP) amino acid drink (Blokland et al. 2004; Evers et al. 2005; Sambeth et al. 2008). To obtain a drinkable mixture, 100 g of the protein powder was mixed with 200 ml of tap water. During the PLC condition the intake of the mixture was directly followed by ingestion of four capsules containing 1.2 g l-TRP (Sigma, Zwijndrecht; The Netherlands), whereas during the ATD condition, intake was directly followed by ingestion of four capsules containing lactose (all capsules were taken with a little water). See Table 2 for the amino acid composition of the different conditions (Evers et al. 2005).

Table 2 Composition (grams) of the gelatin-based protein (all values are grams per 100 g of each mixture)

This ATD method differs from the classic methodology by including a gelatin-based hydrolyzed CP that contains the entire range of amino acids (except for l-TRP) in the form of peptides. After administration, these peptides are decomposed into amino acids and the mechanism of depletion is identical to the classical ATD method.

Stress exposure

A mental arithmetic task, performed under noise stimulation, was used as an uncontrollable stress situation (Markus–Peters Arithmetic task). Subjects were given 18 successive 1-min trials during which they had to solve a specific number of multiple choice mental arithmetic problems (the criterion) under time constraints, while at the same time continuous 75, 80, or 85 dB industrial noise was presented to them through headphones. They were led to believe that the intensity of the noise depended on their performance; if they failed the criterion, the noise intensity was chosen by the computer during the next trial; if they met the criterion, they could choose the intensity of the noise. In fact, the criterion was manipulated so that all subjects continued to fail on each trial. This task has been demonstrated to induce psychological and physiological stress and to be perceived as highly uncontrollable (Markus et al. 1998, 2000a, b; Peters et al. 1998). In the present study, an adapted version of the MPA was used during which subjects had to perform under audience observation. Furthermore, subjects were told that a video analysis of their performance was conducted to analyze their non-verbal behavior.

Subjective mood ratings

Profile of mood states

Changes in mood were measured using the Dutch shortened version of the POMS questionnaire (Wald and Mellenbergh 1990), offered at the computer-screen as a VAS scale ranging from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree”. The POMS comprises five different subscales for mood, ranging from anger, depression, fatigue, and tension that refer to a negative mood state, to vigor concerning a positive mood.

Positive and Negative Affect Scale

The PANAS (Watson et al. 1988) consists of two 10-item mood scales, a positive mood scale and a negative mood scale. The 20 items are randomly presented on a computer screen and participants have to respond on a scale ranging from 1 (totally disagree) to 5 (totally agree).

Emotional related free recall

To test emotionally related immediate and delayed free recall, an adapted version of the 30 Word Learning Test (30 WLT) was used (e.g., Smeets et al. 2006; Sobczak et al. 2002b). The 30 WLT is an adapted version of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Task (Lezak 2004) In this test, participants are presented with a list of 30 words (ten negative, ten positive, and ten neutral words matched for frequency, word length, and emotional valence, chosen from Hermans and De Houwer’s (1994) list of Dutch words that have been rated for familiarity and affectivity) in random order. Each word was presented on the computer monitor for 3 s with an inter-stimulus interval of 1 s. The participants had to rate how relevant each word was to their personal concerns on a scale ranging from 1 (not at all relevant) to 5 (extremely relevant). Following the encoding task, the participants were given 2 min to complete an immediate written recall task. A delayed recall took place after 30 min. Four comparable lists (matched for frequency, word length, and emotional valence) were used and presentation order was counterbalanced across participants.

Affective go/no-go

A modified version of the affective go/no-go task described by Murphy et al. (1999) was used in the present study. In this task, happy and sad words are presented on the screen one-by-one for 300 ms followed by an inter-stimulus interval of 900 ms during which participants must make or withhold a button press response on the basis of the emotion of the word. The task comprises two practice blocks and 12 experimental blocks. Each block contains 18 words (nine happy words and nine sad words), and subjects are instructed that they must respond either to the happy words or to the sad words before each block. Every two blocks, the targets and the distractors change, words that were previously targets become distractors, and words that were previously distractors become targets (SSHHSSHHSSHHSS or HHSSHHSSHHSSHH). Due to this arrangement, shift blocks and non-shift blocks can be studied.

Vegetative side effects

In order to measure possible side effects of the amino acid mixtures, a list (five-point scales) of vegetative side effects was completed before and 4.5 h after intake. The list contained the following items: feeling cold, feeling hot, dizziness, perspiration, blurred vision, nausea, palpitations, dry mouth, and abdominal complaints.

Biochemical measurements

Plasma samples

Blood samples were collected in 5 ml Vacutainer tubes containing sodium heparin for amino acid measurements. The sodium heparin tube was centrifuged at 5,000 rpm for 10 min at 4°C. Subsequently, the supernatants were directly stored at −80°C until analysis. Plasma amino acid analyses were conducted with HPLC, making use of a 2–3-μm Bischof Spherisorb ODS II column. The plasma tryptophan ratio was calculated by dividing the plasma tryptophan concentration by the sum of the other large neutral amino acids, i.e., valine, isoleucine, leucine, tyrosine, and phenylalanine.

Salivary cortisol

Cortisol samples were obtained by using the Salivette sampling device (Sarstedt®, Etten-Leur, The Netherlands). With this procedure, saliva was collected in small cotton swabs and stored (−25°C) immediately upon collection until centrifugation. Saliva samples were centrifuged at 2,650×g max for 3 min at 20°C. Salivary free cortisol levels were determined in duplicate by direct radioimmunoassay (University of Liège, Belgium), including a competition reaction between 125iodohistamine-cortisol and anti-cortisol serum made against the 3-CMO-BSA conjugate. After incubation of 100 μl of saliva at 4°C overnight, separation of free and antibody-bound 125iodohistamine-cortisol was performed via a conventional “second antibody” method. In order to reduce sources of variability, all samples were analyzed in the same assay.

Buccal cells for polymorphism assessment

Buccal cell samples for measuring triallelic variants of the 5-HTT-linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) were obtained using sterile swabs (Omni Swabs, Whatman’s Hertogenbosch, The Netherlands). Genomic DNA was isolated from buccal swabs using QIAamp DNA Mini Kits from Qiagen (Westburg, Leusden, The Netherlands) for determination of the 5-HTTLPR genotype. Briefly, genotyping was performed using the polymerase chain reaction protocol according to Wendland et al. (2006) with the oligonucleotide primers 5′-TCCTCCGCTTTGGCGCCTCTTCC-3′ and 5′-TGGGGGTTGCAGGGGAGATCCTG-3′. Triallelic variants were reclassified into a biallelic model as follows: S/S, S/LG, and LG/LG were classified as S′/S′ and LA/LA as L′/L′ (Neumeister et al. 2006; Walderhaug et al. 2007; Zalsman et al. 2006).

Statistical analysis

The main research questions were analyzed by means of repeated measures multivariate and univariate analyses of variance (MANOVA and ANOVA) by using the General Linear Model (SPSS 12.0 for Windows) with one between-subjects factor genotype (S′/S′ vs. L′/L′) and two within-subjects factors treatment (TRP+ vs. TRP−) and stress (pre-stress vs. post-stress) or time (t 0 vs. t 4.5 or t 5; cortisol analysis: time (t −30 vs. t 0 vs. t +20 vs. t +30)) on the several dependent measures. Order was included as centered covariate. For the subjective mood ratings, we conducted two repeated measures ANOVAs with genotype (S′/S′ vs. L′/L′) as the between-subjects factor and treatment (TRP+ vs. TRP−) as the within-subjects factors for the subjective mood ratings. To investigate the effects of treatment and the effects of stress separately, we included time (t 0 vs. t 5) in the first analysis and stress (pre-stress vs. post-stress) in the second analysis as within-subjects factor. For the affective go/no-go task, valence (positive vs. negative) and shift (shift blocks vs. non-shift blocks) were included as within-subjects factors. For the emotional related free recall, valence (negative vs. neutral vs. positive) was included as a within-subjects variable. Greenhouse–Geisser corrected p values are reported when the sphericity assumption is not met. All statistics are evaluated at a two-tailed significance level of 5%. Post hoc analyses for significant interactions were assessed by paired and unpaired t tests to aid interpretation; corrections for multiple comparisons were not carried out.

Results

Plasma amino acids (total TRP and TRP/LNAA ratio)

For TRP concentrations a significant treatment × time interaction was found [F(1, 32) = 224.87, p < 0.001], reflecting a decrease from t 0 to t 4.5 by 79% after TRP– and an increase from t 0 to t 4.5 by 68% after TRP+ administration. Analysis of the plasma TRP/LNAA ratio revealed a significant treatment × time interaction [F(1, 32) = 269.59, p < 0.001] with a 77% decline in plasma TRP/LNAA after TRP− and an increase from t 0 to t 4.5 by 62% after TRP+ (see Fig. 1). No other main or interaction effects were found including genotype.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Plasma total TRP concentrations and TRP/LNAA ratio for the TRP− and TRP+ conditions. For TRP concentrations, we found a significant decrease from t 0 to t 4.5 after TRP− [t(34) = 24.56, p < 0.001] and a significant increase from t 0 to t 4.5 by 68% after TRP+ [t(34) = 7.58, p < 0.001]. For the TRP/LNAA ratio, we found a significant decrease from t 0 to t 4.5 after TRP− [t(34) = 23.07, p < 0.001] and a significant increase from t 0 to t 4.5 by 68% after TRP+ [t(34) = 7.63, p < 0.001]

Cortisol

Repeated measures analysis of variance only revealed a main effect of time [F(3, 30) = 4.02, p = 0.016] showing a slight but significant decline in cortisol concentrations t +20 to t +30 (from 2.4 ± 0.2 to 2.2 ± 0.2 nmol/L).

Vegetative side effects

No significant main or interaction effects were found.

Subjective mood ratings

Data for the subjective mood rating scales are presented in Table 3.

Table 3 Subjective mood ratings

POMS

In the first analysis, multivariate analysis revealed a main effect of time [F(5, 28) = 12.52, p < 0.001] with higher anger and fatigue scores and lower tension scores at t 5. Further, univariate analysis of the POMS revealed a treatment × time interaction of the vigor subscale [F(1, 32) = 7.28, p = 0.011] and fatigue subscale [F(1, 32) = 5.35, p = 0.027] reflecting a stronger decrease in vigor ratings following TRP− than TRP+ [t(34) = 2.74, p = 0.01] and a stronger increase in fatigue ratings following TRP− than TRP+ [t(34) = 2.32, p = 0.026] (see Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mean (SE) vigor (POMS) and positive affect (PANAS) ratings showing a significant stronger decrease in vigor ratings following TRP− than TRP+ [t(34) = 2.74, p = 0.01] and a significant decrease in positive affect ratings following TRP− but not TRP+ [t(34) = 2.57, p = 0.015]

In the second analysis, multivariate analysis showed a main effect of stress [F(5, 28) = 3.81, p = 0.009] and a significant stress × genotype interaction [F(5, 28) = 3.96, p = 0.008] (see Fig. 3). Further, univariate analysis showed that the main effect of stress originated from the subscale anger [F(1, 32) = 12.39, p = 0.001] and fatigue [F(1, 32) = 4.51, p = 0.041], reflecting an increase in anger post-stress compared to pre-stress and a decrease in fatigue post-stress compared to pre-stress. The stress × genotype interaction originated from the depression subscale [F(1, 32) = 8.24, p = 0.007] and the vigor subscale [F(1, 32) = 13.12, p = 0.001]. This interaction was explored with a one-way multivariate analysis including the vigor and depression subscale for each genotype, indicating an increase in depressive mood after stress in the S′/S′ group [F(1, 16) = 5.38, p = 0.034] only and an increase in vigor after stress in the L′/L′ group [F(1, 15) = 12.61, p = 0.003] only (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Mean (SE) depression and vigor ratings pre-stress and post-stress for both genotypes collapsed over treatment showing a significant increase in vigor post-stress for the L′/L′ genotype [F(1, 15) = 12.61, p = 0.003] and a significant increase in depressive mood for the S′/S′ genotype [F(1, 16) = 5.38, p = 0.034]

PANAS

In the first analysis, we found a treatment × time interaction [F(1, 32) = 6.94, p = 0.013] on the positive scale, indicating a decrease in positive mood regardless of genotype following TRP− [t(34) = 2.57, p = 0.015] but not following TRP+ [t(34) < 1, ns] (see Fig. 2). No effects were found for the negative scale of the PANAS.

In the second analysis, only a significant effect of stress [F(1, 32) = 4.28, p = 0.047] was found on the negative scale, reflecting higher negative mood scores post-stress compared to pre-stress regardless of treatment and genotype.

Affective go/no-go

Mean values and standard deviations are presented in Table 4.

Table 4 AGNG and memory data

Reaction time

Analysis of reaction time (RT) data revealed a main effect of stress [F(1, 32) = 5.73, p = 0.023], indicating longer RTs post-stress compared to pre-stress, and a main effect of Shift [F(1, 32) = 11.98, p = 0.002] with faster RTs for shift blocks than non-shift blocks. This shift effect was qualified by a treatment × shift interaction [F(1, 32) = 5.5, p = 0.025], indicating faster RTs for shift trials than non-shift trials in the TRP+ condition only [t(34) = 4.31, p < 0.001] and a valence × shift interaction [F(1, 32) = 4.71, p = 0.038], indicating longer RTs for sad non-shift trials compared to sad shift trials [t(34) = 3.88, p < 0.001]. Analysis also revealed a valence × genotype interaction [F(1, 32) = 8.62, p = 0.006], reflecting significant longer RTs for sad words compared to happy words for the S′/S′ genotype group only [t(17) = 3.11, p = 0.006] (Fig. 4).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Mean RTs (SE) for happy and sad target words for both genotypes collapsed over stress showing significant longer RTs for sad words compared to happy words for the S′/S′ genotype group only [t(17) = 3.11, p = 0.006]

Errors

Analysis of error data revealed a main effect of treatment [F(1, 32) = 4.68, p = 0.038], indicating significantly more errors following TRP− than TRP+; a main effect of stress [F(1, 32) = 4.55, p = 0.041] with fewer errors post-stress than pre-stress and a main effect of shift [F(1, 32) = 21.99, p < 0.001] with more errors for shift than non-shift blocks. Furthermore, we found a treatment × genotype × valence × shift interaction [F(1, 32) = 5.27, p = 0.028], which was explored by a three-way (treatment, valence, shift) ANOVA for each genotype revealing a treatment × valence × shift interaction for the S′/S′ genotype only [F(1, 16) = 12.24, p = 0.003]. To break down this interaction, a two-way (treatment, valence) ANOVA for the S′/S′ genotype was run for each shift condition showing a treatment × valence interaction for the shift condition only [F(1, 16) = 9.19, p = 0.008], revealing more errors following TRP− than TRP+ during happy shift blocks [t(17) = 2.15, p = 0.047].

Further, a treatment × stress × valence × shift interaction [F(1, 32) = 13.01, p = 0.001] was found reflecting a treatment × stress × valence interaction for the non-shift condition only [F(1, 33) = 8.45, p = 0.006]. This interaction was further explored by a two-way (treatment, stress) ANOVA for happy words and sad words yielding a treatment × stress interaction for happy words only [F(1, 33) = 12.62, p = 0.001], showing a decrease in errors pre-stress to post-stress following TRP− [t(34) = 2.52, p = 0.017] but no effect following TRP+ [t(34) = 1.67, p = 0.10].

Omissions

Analysis of omission data revealed a significant treatment × valence × shift interaction [F(1, 32) = 4.66, p = 0.038]. A two-way (treatment, valence) ANOVA was run for shift and non-shift blocks revealing a marginally significant treatment × valence interaction for the shift blocks only [F(1, 33) = 3.98, p = 0.054], indicating more omissions for happy shift trials following TRP+ compared to TRP−. However, this difference did not approach significance [t(34) = 1.78, p = 0.085]. There were no effects of genotype or stress.

Emotionally related free recall

Mean values and standard deviations are presented in Table 4.

Immediate recall

Analysis of immediate recall data revealed a main effect of valence [F(2, 64) = 11.38, p < 0.001] and a significant genotype × treatment × valence interaction [F(2, 64) = 4.17, p = 0.02]. This interaction was explored by a two-way ANOVA (treatment, genotype) for positive, negative, and neutral words, revealing a marginally significant treatment × genotype interaction for positive words only [F(1, 32) = 3.48, p = 0.071]. This interaction, which is visualized in Fig. 5, reflected that individuals with the S′/S′ genotype recalled fewer positive words following TRP− than TRP+ [t(17) = 2.5, p = 0.023].

Fig. 5
figure 5

Mean number (SE) of immediately recalled positive words for both genotypes following TRP+ and TRP− collapsed over stress showing that individuals with the S′/S′ genotype recalled fewer positive words following TRP− than TRP+ [t(17) = 2.5, p = 0.023]

Delayed recall

An analysis of the delayed recall data revealed a main effect of stress [F(1, 32) = 14.57, p = 0.001] reflecting that fewer words were recalled post-stress than pre-stress, and a main effect of valence [F(2, 64) = 54.17, p < 0.001] showing that more positive words were recalled than negative and neutral words. Further, we found a significant treatment × stress × valence interaction [F(2, 64) = 3.28, p = 0.049]. This interaction was further explored by a two-way (treatment, stress) ANOVA for positive, negative, and neutral words, revealing a treatment × stress interaction for neutral words only [F(1, 33) = 7.45, p = 0.01], indicating that more neutral words were recalled pre-stress compared post-stress following TRP+ [t(34) = 4.37, p = 0.000] but not TRP− [t(34) < 1, ns]. However, it should be noted that there was already a difference between treatments pre-stress [t(34) = 2.2, p = 0.034] with fewer neutral words being recalled following TRP− than TRP+.

Discussion

The goal of the present study was to assess mood, memory, and attention in individuals with different 5-HTTLPR genotypes following ATD before and after the exposure to uncontrollable stress.

ATD lowered the plasma TRP/LNAA by 77%, which is comparable with previous studies using the same CP ATD method (Evers et al. 2005) or the classic ATD mixture (Van der Does 2001). Previous studies using the classic ATD mixture have reported an increase in plasma TRP of up to 200% in the balanced (TRP+) condition (for a review see Fusar-Poli et al. 2006), whereas the CP ATD method has been found to be more effective in maintaining a stable TRP+ condition (Evers et al. 2005; Firk and Markus 2008). In the present study, the TRP/LNAA ratio also increased by 62% in the TRP+ condition suggesting that the TRP+ condition may also be psychoactive due to an increase in 5-HT synthesis. However, this increase in TRP/LNAA ratio is probably insufficient to affect behavior (Markus 2008).

ATD resulted in a significant reduction of positive mood in all subjects independent of 5-HTTLPR genotype. This finding was surprising since most studies only found mood-lowering effects of ATD in individuals at risk for depression such as depressive patients in remission, individuals with a positive family history of depression (for a recent review see Ruhe et al. 2007), or in healthy s allele carries as compared to l carriers (Neumeister et al. 2002, 2006). However, few recent studies including the triallelic 5-HTTLPR polymorphism also revealed negative mood effects of ATD in l/l carriers if they were patients (Neumeister et al. 2006) or women (Walderhaug et al. 2007). Although our participants reported no family history of depression during screening, it may indeed be possible that some of these answers were instead based on lack of information, which may also explain this surprising effect. However, even if one or two young adult participants were actually FH+, it is unlikely that this has confounded our findings given the sample size of the current study.

Even though the s allele is suggested as a vulnerability factor for the negative effects of ATD on mood, it seems that this is more complex and brain 5-HT manipulation may particularly alter mood in s allele carriers when the serotonergic system is actually needed in the face of acute stress as 5-HT plays a role in regulating HPA axis activity, which is important for stress coping (e.g., Porter et al. 2004). Therefore, we predicted that s allele carriers are especially vulnerable to ATD under acute stress exposure. For this purpose, the MPA task was used in the present study as an uncontrollable stress situation since it has frequently been found to induce stress as indicated by mood deterioration and increases in cortisol and electrophysiological arousal (e.g., Markus et al. 2002; Peters et al. 1998). Analysis indeed revealed a slight stress-induced increase in depression scores exclusively in S′/S′ genotypes, whereas in L′/L′ genotypes stress even increased positive feelings of vigor. These findings seem to support previous assumptions of 5-HTTLPR s allele as a vulnerability factor in the development of depression and suggest that L′/L′ carriers are less susceptible to the negative effects of stress than S′/S′ genotypes. As already described in the “Introduction”, 5-HT plays an important role in stress coping which may explain why l allele carriers are more resistant to the negative effects of stress compared to s allele carriers. This involvement of 5-HT in stress coping is supported by complex interactions between 5-HT and the neuroendocrine stress system (Porter et al. 2004). Acute stress increases 5-HT neurotransmission (Davis et al. 1995; De Kloet et al. 1982, 1983), which promotes stress adaptation by mediating negative feedback control of cortisol on the HPA axis (Nuller and Ostroumova 1980; Van Praag 2004). In accord with the current findings, s allele carriers are thought to be 5-HT vulnerable and to be susceptible to depression symptoms particularly in the face of stress (Caspi et al. 2003; Firk and Markus 2007; Van Praag 2004). Although hypothetically, stress and enhanced HPA activation, particularly in s allele carriers, may further undermine 5-HT function, which then may lead to reduced stress coping and subsequently result in negative mood (e.g., Dayan and Huys 2008; Firk and Markus 2007; Van Praag 2004). In further support, Gotlib et al. (2008) found that only girls homozygous for the s allele showed a cortisol response following acute stress exposure, which also supports the hypothesis that 5-HTTLPR affects HPA activity and hence, stress reactivity.

Although the affective effects of acute stress exposure appear to be influenced by 5-HTTLPR, there was no additional effect of ATD. Assuming that S′/S′ genotypes have a greater serotonergic vulnerability than L′/L′ genotypes, and that sufficient 5-HT function is a biological basis for stress coping, we expected that ATD would further increase the negative effect of stress exposure in S′/S′ genotypes. This absence of interaction between ATD, stress, and 5-HTTLPR on depression scores might well be explained by the apparent modest effect of the current acute stress task. Although the MPA stress task also affected mood in the current study, cortisol responses were not increased following stress exposure, suggesting that the MPA task, in the present study, was only a brief and mild stressor. On the contrary, Gotlib et al. (2008) found a 5-HTTLPR-mediated change in cortisol responses by including a more severe and sustained stress procedure. Therefore, ATD may only mediate stress-induced affective changes in s allele carriers under highly emotionally (depression) relevant stressful conditions that require enhanced activation of 5-HT-involved adaptation mechanisms. Although in the current study design, stress may be confounded with fatigue effects over time, subjective mood ratings showed that fatigue decreases post-stress compared to pre-stress, suggesting that stress was not meaningfully confounded by fatigue over time in the current study.

Based on previous studies reporting memory (e.g., Rinck and Becker 2005) and attention bias (e.g., Murphy et al. 1999) toward negatively toned material in depressive patients, we expected a negative attention and memory bias for S′/S′ genotypes especially following ATD and stress exposure. On the AGNG task, we expected faster RTs when responding to sad words; however, slower RTs were found for sad words compared to happy words for S′/S′ genotypes. One possible explanation for this is that S′/S′ genotypes might have a pre-clinical baseline disposition to avoid negative information to counteract heightened amygdala responsivity to negative information (Hariri et al. 2005; Heinz et al. 2005; Pezawas et al. 2005). In support of this theory, avoidance behavior is generally recognized as a mediating factor in anxiety disorders and in the onset of depression (Tull and Gratz 2008). Another possible explanation might be that S′/S′ genotypes spend more time on the elaboration of negative material which might increase their vulnerability to depression. In support of this, healthy individuals without any vulnerability to depression actively avoid elaboration of negative stimuli while depressed patients fail to do so (Deveney and Deldin 2004). This is also consistent with a recently described neuro-computational model by Dayan and Huys (2008). In their model, 5-HT modulates behavioral inhibition as a Pavlovian control process suppressing negative representations that may lead to aversive states and consequences. By reducing brain 5-HT through ATD, this control process will fail and subsequently will give free rein to negative thoughts and actions. Further, in terms of this model, ATD might decrease suppression of negative thoughts particularly in S′/S′ genotypes due to serotonergic vulnerability that is caused by higher 5-HT levels throughout the lifespan development as a function of less sufficient 5-HT reuptake (Dayan and Huys 2008). However, in the present study, ATD did not affect RTs, and increased the number of errors during happy shift blocks only in S′/S′ genotypes, suggesting a slightly increased ATD vulnerability for S′/S′ genotypes in line with our expectations. Previous findings from ATD studies in healthy subjects were inconsistent: some showed a negative attention bias following ATD (Murphy et al. 2002), others reported a bias toward positive material following ATD (Hayward et al. 2005) and others did not find a differential effect of ATD on emotional processing (Roiser et al. 2007; Rubinsztein et al. 2001).

On the memory task, ATD impaired immediate recall of positive words in S′/S′ genotypes. This indeed resembles the mood-congruent memory bias that has been reported for depressed patients reflecting impaired memory for positive information (Matt et al. 1992). Memory performance following ATD has been investigated in previous studies reporting impaired recall of positive words following ATD in healthy subjects (Klaassen et al. 2002) and impaired recall in healthy S′/S′ genotypes independent of word valence (Roiser et al. 2007). Thus, ATD impairs memory performance and in the present study even induces a depressive-like memory pattern in S′/S′ carriers following ATD, supporting greater vulnerability of S′/S′ carriers to depression.

To conclude, polymorphisms of the 5-HTTLPR differentially affects responses to mild stress and ATD, suggesting greater vulnerability of S′/S′ carriers to serotonergic manipulations and supporting increased depression vulnerability. The absence of interaction between ATD, stress, and 5-HTTLPR, possibly due to the modest effect of the current acute stress task, merits further research using a stronger and a more sustained stress procedure.