Skip to main content
Log in

How humans combine simultaneous proprioceptive and visual position information

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

To enable us to study how humans combine simultaneously present visual and proprioceptive position information, we had subjects perform a matching task. Seated at a table, they placed their left hand under the table concealing it from their gaze. They then had to match the proprioceptively perceived position of the left hand using only proprioceptive, only visual or both proprioceptive and visual information. We analysed the variance of the indicated positions in the various conditions. We compared the results with the predictions of a model in which simultaneously present visual and proprioceptive position information about the same object is integrated in the most effective way. The results are in disagreement with the model: the variance of the condition with both visual and proprioceptive information is smaller than expected from the variances of the other conditions. This means that the available information was integrated in a highly effective way. Furthermore, the results suggest that additional information was used. This information might have been visual information about body parts other than the fingertip or it might have been visual information about the environment.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Beers RJ van, Sittig AC, Denier van der Gon JJ (1995) The effectiveness of the integration of visual and proprioceptive information in man (abstract). Eur J Neurosci [Suppl]8:196

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowe A, Keessen W, Kuus WAW, Van Vliet REC (1985) Non-visual assessment of finger-tip positions in a horizontal plane (abstract). J Physiol (Lond) 366:94P

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowe A, Keessen W, Kuus W, Van Vliet R, Zegeling A (1987) Proprioceptive accuracy in two dimensions. Percept Mot Skills 64:831–846

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher RA (1966) The design of experiments, 8th edn. Oliver and Boyd, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher RA (1970) Statistical methods for research workers. Oliver and Boyd, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Gandevia SC, Burke D (1992) Does the nervous system depend on kinesthetic information to control natural limb movements? Behav Brain Sci 15:614–632

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon J, Ghilardi MF, Ghez C (1994) Accuracy of planar reaching movements. I. Independence of direction and extent variability. Exp Brain Res 99:97–111

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Graaf JB de, Sittig AC, Denier van der Gon JJ (1994) Misdirections in slow, goal-directed arm movements are not primarily visually based. Exp Brain Res 99:464–472

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregory RL (1959) Eye movements and the stability of the visual world. Nature 182:1214–1216

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton CR (1964) Intermanual transfer of adaptation to prisms. Am J Psychol 77:457–462

    Google Scholar 

  • Helms Tillery SI, Flanders M, Soechting JF (1991) A coordinate system for the synthesis of visual and kinesthetic information. J Neurosci 11:770–778

    Google Scholar 

  • Helms Tillery SI, Flanders M, Soechting JF (1994) Errors in kinesthetic transformations for hand apposition. Neuroreport 6:177–181

    Google Scholar 

  • Howard IP (1982) Human visual orientation. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod M (1988) The neural and behavioural organization of goal-directed movements. Clarendon, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod M (1991) The interaction of visual and proprioceptive cues in controlling reaching movements. In: Humphrey DR, Freund HJ (ed) Motor control: concepts and issues. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeka JJ, Lackner JR (1994) Fingertip contact influences human postural control. Exp Brain Res 100:495–502

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan S (1970) Ocular pursuit movement as a function of visual and proprioceptive stimuli. Vis Res 10:775–780

    Google Scholar 

  • Lackner JR, DiZio P (1994) Rapid adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectory. J Neurophysiol 72:299–313

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews PBC (1988) Proprioceptors and their contribution to somatosensory mapping: complex messages require complex processing. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 66:430–438

    Google Scholar 

  • Paillard J, Brouchon M (1968) Active and passive movements in the calibration of position sense. In: Freedman SJ (ed) The neuropsychology of spatially oriented behavior. Dorsey, Illinois, pp 37–55

    Google Scholar 

  • Paillard J, Brouchon M (1974) A proprioceptive contribution to the spatial encoding of position cues for ballistic movements. Brain Res 71:273–284

    Google Scholar 

  • Prablanc C, Jeannerod M, Tzavaras A (1975a) Independent and interdependent processes in prism adaptation. In: Vital-Durand F, Jeannerod M (ed) Aspects of neural plasticity. INSERM, Paris, pp 139–152

    Google Scholar 

  • Prablanc C, Tzavaras A, Jeannerod M (1975b) Adaptation of the two arms to opposite prism displacements. Q J Exp Psychol 27:667–671

    Google Scholar 

  • Prablanc C, Echallier JF, Komilis E, Jeannerod M (1979) Optimal response of eye and hand motor systems in pointing at a visual target. I. Spatio-temporal characteristics of eye and hand movements and their relationships when varying the amount of visual information. Biol Cybern 35:113–124

    Google Scholar 

  • Rossetti Y, Meckler C, Prablanc C (1994) Is there an optimal arm posture? Deterioration of finger localization precision and comfort sensation in extreme arm-joint postures. Exp Brain Res 99:131–136

    Google Scholar 

  • Rossetti Y, Desmurget M, Prablanc C (1995) Vectorial coding of movement: vision, proprioception, or both? J Neurophysiol 74:457–463

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott SH, Loeb GE (1994) The computation of position sense from spindles in mono- and multiarticular muscles. J Neurosci 14:7529–7540

    Google Scholar 

  • Sittig AC, Denier van der Gon JJ, Gielen CCAM (1985) Separate control of arm position and velocity demonstrated by vibration of muscle tendon in man. Exp Brain Res 60:445–453

    Google Scholar 

  • Slinger RT, Horsley V (1906) Upon the orientation of points in space by the muscular, arthrodial, and tactile senses of the upper limbs in normal individuals and in blind persons. Brain 29:1–27

    Google Scholar 

  • Soechting JF, Flanders M (1989) Sensorimotor representations for pointing to targets in three-dimensional space. J Neurophysiol 62:582–594

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor JR (1982) Introduction to error analysis. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Wann JP, Ibrahim SF (1992) Does limb proprioception drift? Exp Brain Res 91:162–166

    CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

van Beers, R.J., Sittig, A.C. & van der Gon Denier, J.J. How humans combine simultaneous proprioceptive and visual position information. Exp Brain Res 111, 253–261 (1996). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00227302

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00227302

Key words

Navigation