Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Evaluating the relative impact of positive and negative encounters with police: a randomized experiment

  • Published:
Journal of Experimental Criminology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Objectives

Examines the influence of positive, negative, and neutral police behavior during traffic stops on citizen perceptions of police.

Methods

Participants were randomly assigned to view a video clip of a simulated traffic stop in which the officer communicates with the driver in a positive (procedurally just), negative (procedurally unjust), or neutral manner. After viewing the video, participants completed a survey about their perceptions of police, including their level of trust in police, obligation to obey police orders, and willingness to cooperate with police.

Results

Observing positive interactions with police enhanced people’s self-reported willingness to cooperate with police, obligation to obey police and the law, and trust and confidence in police, whereas observing negative interactions undermined these outcomes. The effects of these interactions were much stronger for encounter-specific outcomes than for more general outcomes.

Conclusions

The results from this randomized experiment confirm that procedural justice can enhance people’s prosocial attitudes toward police, whereas procedural injustice can undermine these attitudes. While positive (procedurally just) interactions tend to have weaker effects than negative (procedurally unjust) interactions, this study finds little support for the notion that only negative experiences shape people’s views about the police.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For example, Manning (1996: 52) notes that the core technology of policing “remains people talking to people, officers trying to persuade people by various interactional strategies to comply with requests, threats, and commands….”

  2. Note that some studies have relied on the analysis of two-wave panel data to estimate the effects of procedural justice on a variety of outcomes (e.g., Beijersbergen et al. 2015; Murphy 2005; Tyler 2006). The use of panel data typically offers stronger internal validity than correlational studies based on cross-sectional data, but weaker internal validity than studies relying on well-executed experimental or quasi-experimental designs (Worden and McLean 2016).

  3. There is also a growing body of experimental research on the effects of procedural justice interventions outside of criminology. For instance, Wenzel (2006) randomly allocated taxpayers to receive one of three reminder letters: a standard letter (which served as the control condition), and two others containing different elements of procedural justice. The reminder letters that incorporated procedural justice principles generated greater levels of tax compliance. Several field experiments have also tested the effects of procedural justice interventions on the attitudes, intentions and behaviors of employees in organizations (e.g., Hunton and Beeler 1997; Schaubroeck et al. 1994).

  4. Communication accommodation theory (CAT) posits that people subconsciously modify their speech patterns to match those of others with whom they are speaking. This communication accommodation, if well calibrated, can generate a number of benefits, including increased trust. However, overaccommodation can be perceived as disingenuous or artificial and can decrease trust (Lowrey et al. 2016).

  5. Confidence in police was measured using questions on “police responsiveness to community concerns and whether police were dealing with problems that really concerned residents. There were also questions about ‘how good a job’ police were doing in preventing crime, keeping order and helping victims” (Skogan 2006: 107).

  6. In Tyler’s (2006) process-based model of regulation, obligation to obey is treated as the principal measure of institutional legitimacy. Thus, legitimacy is said to mediate the relationship between procedural justice and outcomes like cooperation and compliance. However, recent theoretical challenges call into question the meaning and measurement of legitimacy as specified by Tyler. For instance, Tankebe (2013) has articulated a model in which legitimacy is comprised of procedural justice, distributive justice, effectiveness, and lawfulness, and obligation to obey is treated as an outcome that is influenced by legitimacy. Given that the meaning and measurement of institutional legitimacy is currently under debate, we do not incorporate legitimacy as a construct in our model. Instead we focus on the major construct used to measure it in the work of Tyler and his colleagues: obligation to obey. Obligation to obey serves as a mediator between procedural justice and outcomes like cooperation and compliance in both Tyler’s and Tankebe’s models and therefore its effects are not part of the current debate.

  7. In addition to the three procedural justice conditions that are the focus of this study, the larger research project included additional experimental conditions that varied the demographic characteristics of the driver. These results are not reported here. The present study relies only on survey data from the 266 respondents who viewed a video featuring a teenage white male driver. Based on preliminary power analyses, we estimated that a minimum sample size of 159 would be necessary to detect a medium-sized effect (f = .25) with a power of .80 and an α level of .05 (Cohen 1992). Our achieved sample size of 266 is likely sufficient for detecting medium and large effects, but insufficient to detect small effects.

  8. Our appraisals of model fit are informed by the following considerations. For the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), Browne and Cudeck (1993) conclude that values of .06 to .08 constitute acceptable fit, while values of .01 to .06 constitute “close fit.” Hu and Bentler (1999) also treat a RMSEA value of .06 as the upper threshold for close fit. For the Confirmatory Fit Index (CFI) and the Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI), Hu and Bentler (1999) suggest that values of .95 or greater indicate close fit. For the Weighted Root Mean Square Residual (WRMR), simulation evidence suggests that values below 1 are indicative of good fit (Yu 2002).

  9. We estimated composite reliabilities using coefficient omega (Ω), which is based on the ratio of the true score variance to the total variance (McDonald 1999; Raykov 1997). Omega values for the encounter-specific outcomes were as follows: cooperation (Ω = .898), obligation (Ω = .940), and trust and confidence (Ω = .941). Omega values for the general outcomes were as follows: cooperation (Ω = .865), obligation (Ω = .838), and trust and confidence (Ω = .884).

  10. Participants were assigned to groups based on a randomization algorithm in Qualtrics that was not susceptible to intentional or unintentional manipulation. We do not have a ready explanation for the differences in group composition. Randomization is premised on the law of large numbers and sometimes fails in small samples. The most likely possibility in this case is that the differences between groups in the number of males compared to females/intersex resulted from having a relatively small sample.

  11. Respondents were allowed to mark more than one racial group when asked about their racial identity, and 6.7 % of the sample did so. Multiracial respondents who selected white and one or more other races were alternatively coded as either white or non-white, and regression models were run using both configurations. The coefficients and significance levels were virtually the same regardless of how these respondents were classified. The results presented here are based on the former classification (multiracial respondents who marked white as one of their racial identities were coded as white).

  12. Since these variables were only included as covariates to account for differences between groups rather than for substantive reasons, the coefficients are not reported. Of the 18 coefficients for percent male (six outcomes × three contrasts), only two were statistically significant. In both cases, male respondents were found to have greater levels of general trust in police than female and intersex respondents.

  13. Four models were used to generate the estimates reported in Table 3: one with encounter-specific outcomes and negative as the reference category; one with encounter-specific outcomes and neutral as the reference category; one with global outcomes and negative as the reference category; and one with global outcomes and neutral as the reference category. All four models fit the data well, with RMSEA values ranging from .051 to .052, CFI ranging from .990 to .996, TLI ranging from .986 to .994, and WRMR ranging from .525 to .685.

  14. Our Bayesian regression analysis relies on iterative Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) algorithms to “obtain an approximation to the posterior distributions of the parameters from which the estimates are obtained” (Muthén 2010: 8). The estimates in Table 4 are partially standardized regression coefficients derived from the medians of the posterior distributions. The asterisks associated with the Bayesian estimates summarize the one-tailed p values based on the posterior distributions.

  15. Here, we use the term “proximate outcomes” to refer to phenomena that are near in time and scope to the encounter being evaluated. We use the term “distal outcomes” to refer to phenomena that are more distant in time and scope from the encounter being evaluated. The encounter-specific outcomes measured here are examples of proximate outcomes, whereas the global outcomes measured here are examples of distal outcomes. The procedural justice literature is replete with numerous other examples of proximate and distal outcomes.

  16. An anonymous reviewer noted that, because this was a vicarious encounter, participants may have been uncertain about whether to respond to the encounter-specific items from their own personal perspective or from what they viewed as the driver’s likely perspective. Due to this uncertainty, the reviewer suggested that some participants may have answered the encounter-specific items from the perspective of the driver but the general items from their own perspective. We intended for respondents to answer the encounter-specific items from their own perspective (not from the perspective of the driver), and took steps to encourage this approach when designing the experiment and survey instrument. For instance, we carefully considered the placement of these items in the survey, as well as the survey instructions to the respondent. For example, the instructions for the encounter-specific items read: “Thinking specifically about the police officer in the video, please indicate the extent to which you agree or disagree with the following statements.” The goal here was to focus respondents on their own personal evaluation of the police officer, and not on the driver’s likely perspective. In addition, we placed the encounter-specific items immediately after the items for the manipulation check, which focused on the respondents’ assessment of the police officer’s behavior (for example: “The officer was respectful.”).

References

  • Asparouhov, T., & Bengt, M. (2010). Bayesian analysis of latent variable models using Mplus (version 4). Retrieved from http://www.statmodel.com/download/BayesAdvantages18.pdf.

  • Augustyn, M. B. (2016). Updating perceptions of (in)justice. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 53, 255–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bagozzi, R. P., & Yi, Y. (1989). On the use of structural equation models in experimental designs. Journal of Marketing Research, 26, 271–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barkworth, J. M., & Murphy, K. (2015). Procedural justice policing and citizen compliance behaviour: the importance of emotion. Psychology, Crime & Law, 21, 254–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beijersbergen, K. A., Dirkzwager, A. J. E., Eichelsheim, V. I., Van der Laan, P. H., & Nieuwbeerta, P. (2015). Procedural justice, anger, and prisoners’ misconduct. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 42, 196–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradford, B., Jonathan, J., & Stanko, E. (2009). Contact and confidence: revisiting the impact of public encounters with the police. Policing and Society, 19, 20–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brandl, S., Frank, J., Worden, R., & Bynum, T. (1994). Global and specific attitudes toward the police: disentangling the relationship. Justice Quarterly, 11, 119–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In B. A. Kenneth & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136–162). Newbury Park: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiStefano, C., & Morgan, G. B. (2014). A comparison of diagonal weighted least squares robust estimation techniques for ordinal data. Structural Equation Modeling, 21, 425–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engel, R. S. (2005). Citizens’ perceptions of distributive and procedural injustice during traffic stops with police. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 42, 445–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epp, C. R., Maynard-Moody, S., & Haider-Markel, D. P. (2014). Pulled over: how police stops define race and citizenship. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

  • Flora, D. B., & Curran, P. J. (2004). An empirical evaluation of alternative methods of estimation for confirmatory factor analysis with ordinal data. Psychological Methods, 9, 466–491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, J., Smith, B., & Novak, K. (2005). The basis of citizens’ attitudes toward the police. Police Quarterly, 8, 206–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, C., Maguire, E. R., Mastrofski, S. D., & Reisig, M. (2001). The public image of the police. Alexandria: International Association of Chiefs of Police.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gau, J. (2014). Procedural justice and police legitimacy: a test of measurement and structure. American Journal of Criminal Justice, 39, 187–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gau, J., & Brunson, R. (2010). Procedural justice and order maintenance policing: a study of inner-city young men’s perceptions of police legitimacy. Justice Quarterly, 27, 255–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gau, J. M., Corsaro, N., Stewart, E., & Brunson, R. K. (2012). Examining macro-level impacts on procedural justice and police legitimacy. Journal of Criminal Justice, 40, 333–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, A., Fagan, J., & Kiss, A. (2007). An analysis of the New York City Police Department’s “stop-and-frisk” policy in the context of claims of racial bias. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 102, 813–823.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hancock, G. R. (2004). Experimental, quasi-experimental, and nonexperimental design and analysis with latent variables. In D. Kaplan (Ed.), The SAGE handbook of quantitative methodology for the social sciences. Thousand Oaks: SAGE Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indices in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunton, J. E., & Beeler, J. D. (1997). Effects of user participation in systems development: a longitudinal field experiment. MIS Quarterly, 21, 359–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hurst, Y. G., & Frank, J. (2000). How kids view cops: the nature of juvenile attitudes toward the police. Journal of Criminal Justice, 28, 189–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jesilow, P., Meyer, J., & Namazzi, N. (1995). Public attitudes toward the police. American Journal of Police, 14, 67–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D., Maguire, E. R., & Kuhns, J. B. (2014). Public perceptions of the legitimacy of the law and legal authorities: evidence from the Caribbean. Law and Society Review, 48, 947–978.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liang, X., & Yang, Y. (2014). An evaluation of WLSMV and Bayesian methods for confirmatory factor analysis with categorical indicators. International Journal of Quantitative Research in Education, 2, 17–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lind, E. A., & Tyler, T. R. (1988). The social psychology of procedural justice. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lowrey, B. V., Maguire, E. R., & Bennett, R. R. (2016). Testing the effects of procedural justice and overaccommodation in traffic stops: a randomized experiment. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 43, 1430–1449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacQueen, S., & Bradford, B. (2015). Enhancing public trust and police legitimacy during road traffic encounters: results from a randomised controlled trial in Scotland. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 11, 419–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maguire, E. R., & Johnson, D. (2010). Measuring public perceptions of the police. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies and Management, 33, 703–730.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manning, P. K. (1996). Information technology in the police context: the “sailor” phone. Information Systems Research, 7, 52–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mazerolle, L., Antrobus, E., Bennett, S., & Tyler, T. R. (2013). Shaping citizen perceptions of police legitimacy: a randomized field trial of procedural justice. Criminology, 51, 33–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mazerolle, L., Bennett, S., Antrobus, E., & Eggins, E. (2012). Procedural justice, routine encounters and citizen perceptions of police: main findings from the Queensland Community Engagement Trial (QCET). Journal of Experimental Criminology, 8, 343–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDonald, R. P. (1999). Test theory: A unified treatment. Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, K. (2005). Regulating more effectively: the relationship between procedural justice, legitimacy, and tax non-compliance. Journal of Law and Society, 32, 562–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, K., Mazerolle, L., & Bennett, S. (2014). Promoting trust in police: findings from a randomized experimental field trial of procedural justice policing. Policing and Society, 24, 405–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muthén, B. (2010). Bayesian analysis in Mplus: A brief introduction. (http://www.statmodel.com).

  • Muthén, B., et al. (1997). Robust inference using weighted-least squares and quadratic estimating equations in latent variable modeling with categorical and continuous outcomes. Unpublished paper [available at www.statmodel.com].

  • Myhill, A., & Bradford, B. (2012). Can police enhance public confidence by improving quality of service? Results from two surveys in England and Wales. Policing and Society, 22, 397–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peeters, G., & Czapinski, J. (1990). Positive-negative asymmetry in evaluations: the distinction between affective and informational negativity effects. European Review of Social Psychology, 1(1), 33–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • President’s Task Force on 21st Century Policing. (2015). Final Report of the President’s Task Force on 21st Century Policing. Washington, DC: Office of Community Oriented Policing Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raykov, T. (1997). Estimation of composite reliability for congeneric measures. Applied Psychological Measurement, 21, 173–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. P., Schuck, A. M., Costello, S. K., Hawkins, D. F., & Ring, M. K. (2005). Attitudes toward the police: the effects of direct and vicarious experience. Police Quarterly, 8(3), 343–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, D. W., Kahn, J. H., Spoth, R., & Altmaier, E. M. (1998). Analyzing data from experimental studies: a latent variable structural equation modeling approach. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 45(1), 18–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sahin, N. M. (2014). Legitimacy, procedural justice and police-citizen encounters: A randomized controlled trial of the impact of procedural justice on citizen perceptions of the police during traffic stops in Turkey (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Newark: Rutgers University.

  • Schaubroeck, J., May, D. R., & Brown, F. W. (1994). Procedural justice explanations and employee reactions to economic hardship: a field experiment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79(3), 455–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sklansky, D.A. (2011). The persistent pull of police professionalism. New Perspectives in Policing. Retrieved from https://ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/nij/232676.pdf.

  • Skogan, W. G. (2006). Asymmetry in the impact of encounters with police. Policing and Society, 16, 99–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skogan, W. G. (2012). Assessing asymmetry: the life course of a research project. Policing and Society, 22, 270–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sunshine, J., & Tyler, T. R. (2003). The role of procedural justice and legitimacy in shaping public support for policing. Law and Society Review, 37, 513–548.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tankebe, J. (2009). Public cooperation with the police in Ghana: does procedural fairness matter? Criminology, 47, 1265–1293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tankebe, J. (2013). Viewing things differently: the dimensions of public perceptions of police legitimacy. Criminology, 51, 103–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: the mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 67–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, T. J., Turner, K. B., Esbensen, F., & Winfree, L. T., Jr. (2001). Coppin’ an attitude: attitudinal differences among juveniles toward police. Journal of Criminal Justice, 29, 295–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thibaut, J. W., & Walker, L. (1975). Procedural justice: A psychological analysis. Hillsdale: L. Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tuch, S. A., & Weitzer, R. (1997). Trends: racial differences in attitudes toward the police. Public Opinion Quarterly, 61, 642–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T. R. (1989). The psychology of procedural justice: a test of the group-value model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 830–838.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T. R. (2005). Policing in black and white: ethnic group differences in trust and confidence in the police. Police Quarterly, 8, 322–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T. R. (2006). Why people obey the law. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T. R., & Blader, S. L. (2003). The group engagement model: procedural justice, social identity, and cooperative behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7, 349–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T. R., & Fagan, J. (2008). Legitimacy and cooperation: why do people help the police fight crime in their communities? Ohio State Journal of Criminal Law, 6, 231–275.

  • Tyler, T. R., & Huo, Y. (2002). Trust in the law: Encouraging public cooperation with the police and courts. New York: Russell Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T. R., Goff, P. A., & MacCoun, R. J. (2015). The impact of psychological science on policing in the United States: procedural justice, legitimacy and effective law enforcement. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 16, 75–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T. R., & Lind, E. A. (1992). A relational model of authority in groups. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 115–292). San Diego: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Prooijen, J. W., van den Bos, K., & Wilke, H. A. M. (2004). Group belongingness and procedural justice: social inclusion and exclusion by peers affects the psychology of voice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 66–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webb, V. J., & Marshall, C. E. (1995). The relative importance of race and ethnicity on citizen attitudes toward the police. American Journal of Police, 14, 45–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weitzer, R., & Brunson, R. K. (2009). Strategic responses to the police among inner-city youth. The Sociological Quarterly, 50(2), 235–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wenzel, M. (2006). A letter from the tax office: compliance effects of informational and interpersonal justice. Social Justice Research, 19, 345–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Worden, R., & McLean, S. (2016). Research on police legitimacy: The state of the art. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies and Management.

  • Yu, C. (2002). Evaluating cutoff criteria of model fit indices for latent variable models with binary and continuous outcomes. Doctoral dissertation. Los Angeles, CA: UCLA.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Edward R. Maguire.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. This article does not contain any studies with animals performed by any of the authors.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Maguire, E.R., Lowrey, B.V. & Johnson, D. Evaluating the relative impact of positive and negative encounters with police: a randomized experiment. J Exp Criminol 13, 367–391 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11292-016-9276-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11292-016-9276-9

Keywords

Navigation