Skip to main content
Log in

Age-related differences in responses to affective vs. rational ads for hedonic vs. utilitarian products

  • Published:
Marketing Letters Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This research investigated the moderating role of product category type (hedonic vs. utilitarian) on age-related differences in responses to affective vs. rational ads. An experiment showed that elderly consumers (age 65 plus) had more favorable attitudes toward affective (vs. rational) ads, regardless of product category type. In contrast, young adult consumers (age 18–25) favored affective ads only for hedonic products. They favored rational ads for utilitarian products. Results of the experiment imply that, to explain age-related differences in decision making, researchers must take into account age-related differences in motivational states apart from apparent shortfalls in cognition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Blanchard-Fields, F. (1986). Reasoning on social dilemmas varying in emotional saliency: An adult developmental perspective. Psychology and Aging, 1, 325–333 (March).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carstensen, L. L. (1992). Social and emotional patterns in adulthood: Support for socioemotional selectivity theory. Psychology and Aging, 7, 331–338, (September).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carstensen, L. L., & Turk-Charles, S. (1994). The salience of emotion across the adult life span. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 239–264 (June).

    Google Scholar 

  • Drolet, A., & Aaker, J. (2002). Off-target? Changing cognitively-based attitudes. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12, 59–68 (January).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drolet, A., & Luce, M. F. (2004). The rationalizing effects of cognitive load on response to emotional tradeoff difficulty. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(1), 63–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fung, H. H., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Sending memorable messages to the old: Age differences in preferences and memory for advertisements. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 163–178 (July).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isaacowitz, D. M., Turk-Charles, S., & Carstensen, L. L. (2000). Emotion and cognition. In F. Craik & T. Salthouse (Eds.), Handbook of aging and cognition (2nd ed., pp. 593–631). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G. (1998). Cognitive–emotional integration in adulthood. In K. Warner Schaie & M. P. Lawton (Eds.), Annual review of gerontology and geriatrics, vol. 17: Focus on emotion and adult development (pp. 206–237). New York, NY: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G., & Blanchard-Fields, F. (1982). Cognitive ageing and psychological growth. Aging and society, 2, 183–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G., DeVoe,M., & Bulka, D. (1989). Speaking about feelings: Conceptions of emotion across the life span. Psychology and Aging, 4, 425–437 (March).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Law, S., Hawkins, S. A., & Craik, F. I. M. (1998). Repetition-induced belief in the elderly: Rehabilitating age-related memory deficits. Journal of Consumer Research, 25, 91–107 (September).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pham, M. T. (1998). Representativeness, relevance and the use of feelings in decision making. Journal of Consumer Research, 25, 144–159, September.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poon, L. W. (1985). Differences in human memory with aging: Nature, causes and clinical implications. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Shaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (pp. 427–455). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratchford, B. T. (1987). New insights about the FCB grid. Journal of Advertising Research, 27, 24–38, September.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rousseau, G. K., & Rogers, W. A. (2002). Effects of processing style and age on schema acquisition. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57, 11–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skurnik, I., Yoon, C., Park, D. C., & Schwarz, N. (2005). How warnings about false claims become recommendations. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(4), 713–724.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • United Nations Population Information Network (2007). www.un.org/popin.

  • Williams, P., & Aaker, J. (2002). Can mixed emotions peacefully co-exist? Journal of Consumer Research, 28, 636–649 (March).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, P., & Drolet, A. (2005). Age-related differences in responses to emotional advertisements. Journal of Consumer Research, 32, 343–354 (December).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yoon, C. (1997). Age differences in consumers’ processing strategies: An investigation of moderating influences. Journal of Consumer Research, 24, 329–342 (December).

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Aimee Drolet.

Appendix

Appendix

Table 1 Ad stimuli examples

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Drolet, A., Williams, P. & Lau-Gesk, L. Age-related differences in responses to affective vs. rational ads for hedonic vs. utilitarian products. Market Lett 18, 211–221 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-007-9016-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-007-9016-z

Keywords

Navigation