Skip to main content
Log in

Reduced Attention Towards Accomplishments Mediates the Effect of Self-Critical Rumination on Regret

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Background

Rumination is associated with counterfactual thinking (CFT) and regret, but underlying mechanisms in this association are uninvestigated. Rumination is characterized by attentional biases and focusing attention towards accomplishments versus lost opportunities influences CFT and regret. The goal of this study was to investigate the association between self-critical rumination and attention towards accomplishments and lost opportunities, and how this may underly the link between rumination and CFT and regret.

Methods

Hundred healthy female participants performed a risk-taking task while (a) attention towards accomplishments and lost opportunities, and (b) self-reported CFT and regret were measured.

Results

Analyses showed that participants with high (versus low) rumination tendencies focused less on accomplishments, and this mediated the association between rumination and regret.

Conclusion

These findings suggest that reduced attention towards accomplishments may be an underlying mechanism in the link between rumination and regret, and interventions could target this attentional bias for therapeutic benefit.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Data Availability

The data and analysis scripts are available online (https://osf.io/9yqub/).

Notes

  1. The sample size was determined based on simulations to test the hypothesized indirect mediation effect of self-critical rumination on CFT/regret via attentional deployment. In these simulations, the sample size was increased stepwise in increments of 10, starting from 10, until a power of 0.80 was achieved with alpha set to 0.05. The effect size for the first step of the mediation model, testing the effect of self-critical rumination on attentional deployment was estimated based on past research on rumination and attentional biases (Owens & Gibb, 2016; Sanchez-Lopez et al., 2019b; Yaroslavsky et al., 2019). The effect size for the second step in the mediation model, testing the effect of attentional deployment towards CFT/regret, was based on past research on attentional deployment and regret (Li et al., 2021; Liu et al., 2017, 2020).

  2. After this procedure, an additional task (relational responding task; De Houwer et al., 2015) was performed, in which implicit attitudes towards actual and ideal self-esteem were measured (Remue et al., 2014). The results of this section of the experiment fall outside of the scope of the current manuscript and are reported elsewhere. In addition, skin conductance and heart rate was measured throughout the protocol, but these indices were not used to inform about the current research question.

References

  • Allaert, J., De Raedt, R., Van Der Veen, F. M., Baeken, C., & Vanderhasselt, M. A. (2021a). Prefrontal tDCS attenuates counterfactual thinking in female individuals prone to self-critical rumination. Scientific Reports, 11(1), https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-90677-7.

  • Allaert, J., De Raedt, R., & Vanderhasselt, M. A. (2019). When choosing means losing: Regret enhances repetitive negative thinking in high brooders. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 85, 103850. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2019.103850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allaert, J., Erdogan, M., Sanchez-Lopez, A., Baeken, C., De Raedt, R., & Vanderhasselt, M. A. (2021b). Prefrontal tDCS attenuates Self-Referential Attentional Deployment: A mechanism underlying adaptive emotional reactivity to Social-Evaluative threat [Original Research]. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 15(439), https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.700557.

  • Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2014). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. arXiv preprint arXiv:1406.5823.

  • Bault, N., Wydoodt, P., & Coricelli, G. (2016). Different attentional patterns for regret and disappointment: An Eye-tracking study. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 29(2–3), 194–205.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Beike, D. R., Markman, K. D., & Karadogan, F. (2009). What we regret most are Lost Opportunities: A theory of Regret Intensity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(3), 385–397. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167208328329.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Benjamini, Y. (2010). Discovering the false discovery rate. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: series B (statistical methodology), 72(4), 405–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B (Methodological), 289–300. https://doi.org/10.2307/2346101.

  • Bernstein, E. E., Heeren, A., & McNally, R. J. (2017). Unpacking rumination and Executive Control: A Network Perspective. Clinical Psychological Science, 5(5), 816–826. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702617702717.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, E. E., Heeren, A., & McNally, R. J. (2019). Reexamining trait rumination as a system of repetitive negative thoughts: A network analysis. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 63, 21–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2018.12.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berridge, K. C., & Kringelbach, M. L. (2008). Affective neuroscience of pleasure: Reward in humans and animals. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 199(3), 457–480.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berryhill, M. E., & Martin, D. (2018). Cognitive Effects of Transcranial Direct current stimulation in healthy and clinical populations: An overview. The Journal of ECT, 34(3), e25–e35. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCT.0000000000000534.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brassen, S., Gamer, M., Peters, J., Gluth, S., & Buchel, C. (2012). Don’t look back in anger! Responsiveness to missed chances in successful and nonsuccessful aging [Article]. Science, 336(6081), 612–614. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1217516.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Broomhall, A. G., & Phillips, W. J. (2018). Self-referent upward counterfactuals and depression: Examining regret as a mediator. Cogent Psychology, 5(1), 1416884. https://doi.org/10.1080/23311908.2017.1416884.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broomhall, A. G., Phillips, W. J., Hine, D. W., & Loi, N. M. (2017). Upward counterfactual thinking and depression: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review 55(Supplement C), 56–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2017.04.010.

  • Buchanan, J., Summerville, A., Lehmann, J., & Reb, J. (2016). The regret elements Scale: Distinguishing the affective and cognitive components of regret. Judgment and Decision Making, 11(3), 275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. M. J. (2016). Counterfactual thought. Annual Review of Psychology, 67(1), 135–157. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033249.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, W. K., & Sedikides, C. (1999). Self-threat magnifies the self-serving Bias: A Meta-Analytic Integration. Review of General Psychology, 3(1), 23–43. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.3.1.23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, Y., & Lin, Y. C. (2016). Regret and psychological adjustment: An examination of the dual-route mediating effect of self-compassion and self-judgment.Bulletin of Educational Psychology.

  • Cnaan, A., Laird, N. M., & Slasor, P. (1997). Using the general linear mixed model to analyse unbalanced repeated measures and longitudinal data. Statistics in Medicine, 16(20), 2349–2380. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1097-0258(19971030)16:20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Contreras, A., Nieto, I., Valiente, C., Espinosa, R., & Vazquez, C. (2019). The study of psychopathology from the network analysis perspective: A systematic review. Psychotherapy and psychosomatics, 88(2), 71–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Connolly, T., & Zeelenberg, M. (2002). Regret in decision making. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(6), 212–216. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, N. V., & Weiss, R. (2012). Dichotomizing continuous variables in statistical analysis:A practice to avoid. Medical Decision Making, 32(2), 225–226. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989x12437605.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Raedt, R. (2020). Contributions from neuroscience to the practice of cognitive Behaviour Therapy: Translational psychological science in service of good practice. Behaviour Research And Therapy, 125, 103545. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2019.103545.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Raedt, R., & Koster, E. H. (2010). Understanding vulnerability for depression from a cognitive neuroscience perspective: A reappraisal of attentional factors and a new conceptual framework. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 10(1), 50–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeJong, H., Fox, E., & Stein, A. (2019). Does rumination mediate the relationship between attentional control and symptoms of depression? Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 63, 28–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2018.12.007.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dey, S., Joormann, J., Moulds, M. L., & Newell, B. R. (2018). The relative effects of abstract versus concrete rumination on the experience of post-decisional regret. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 108, 18–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2018.06.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Epstude, K., & Roese, N. J. (2011). When goal pursuit fails the functions of counterfactual thought in intention formation. Social Psychology, 42(1), 19–27. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000039.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferri, J., Schmidt, J., Hajcak, G., & Canli, T. (2013). Neural correlates of attentional deployment within unpleasant pictures. Neuroimage, 70, 268–277. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.030.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferri, J., Schmidt, J., Hajcak, G., & Canli, T. (2016). Emotion regulation and amygdala-precuneus connectivity: Focusing on attentional deployment [journal article]. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16(6), 991–1002. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-016-0447-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelfand, L. A., Mensinger, J. L., & Tenhave, T. (2009). Mediation analysis: A retrospective snapshot of practice and more recent directions. The Journal of general psychology, 136(2), 153–178.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of personality and social psychology, 85(2), 348.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 3–24). Guilford Press.

  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. Guilford Press.

  • Holas, P., Krejtz, I., Rusanowska, M., Rohnka, N., & Nezlek, J. B. (2019). Attention to negative words predicts daily rumination among people with clinical depression: Evidence from an eye tracking and daily diary study. Cognition and emotion, 33(6), 1277–1283. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2018.1541168.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hidaka, B. H. (2012). Depression as a disease of modernity: Explanations for increasing prevalence. Journal of Affective Disorders, 140(3), 205–214. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2011.12.036.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hur, J., Gaul, K., & Berenbaum, H. (2019). Different patterns of attention Bias in worry and rumination. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 43(4), 713–725. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-018-09993-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. P., & Whisman, M. A. (2013). Gender differences in rumination: A meta-analysis. Personality and individual differences, 55(4), 367–374. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.03.019.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jokisaari, M. (2004). Regrets and subjective well-being: A life course approach [journal article]. Journal of Adult Development, 11(4), 281–288. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JADE.0000044531.11605.d5. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org.

  • Jopling, E., Wilson, J., Burke, M., Tracy, A., & LeMoult, J. (2020). Positive interpretation bias across the psychiatric disorders.Cognitive Biases in Health and Psychiatric Disorders,99–117.

  • Kamijo, N., & Yukawa, S. (2018). The role of rumination and negative affect in meaning making following stressful experiences in a japanese sample. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2404.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kopf-Beck, J., & Fietz, J. (2021). Moving toward a process-oriented perspective in the personalized treatment of depression. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 271(3), 413–415. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-021-01249-9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H. W., De Lissnyder, E., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2011). Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: The impaired disengagement hypothesis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(1), 138–145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.08.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kress, L., & Aue, T. (2017). The link between optimism bias and attention bias: A neurocognitive perspective. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 80, 688–702. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.016.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest package: tests in linear mixed effects models.Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13).

  • Lambert, A., & Voot, N. (1993). A left visual field bias for semantic encoding of unattended words. Neuropsychologia, 31(1), 67–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(93)90081-A.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lenth, R. (2018). Emmeans: Estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means.R package version, 1(1).

  • Li, L., Liu, Z., Niu, H., Zheng, L., Cheng, X., Sun, P., Zhou, F. A., & Guo, X. (2018). Responsibility modulates the neural correlates of regret during the sequential risk-taking task. Experimental Brain Research, 236(3), 679–689. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-5165-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Li, S., Liu, S., Huang, P., Liu, S., Zhang, W., Guo, X., & Liu, Z. (2021). The modulation of attentional deployment on regret: An event-related potential study. Neuroreport, 32(7), 621–630. https://doi.org/10.1097/wnr.0000000000001640.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, Z., Cheng, X., Liu, S., Zhang, Z., Li, S., Li, L., & Guo, X. (2020). Attentional deployment training impacts neural responses to subsequent regret. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 157, 23–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.08.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, Z., Li, L., Zheng, L., Xu, M., Zhou, F. A., & Guo, X. (2017). Attentional deployment impacts neural response to regret. Scientific Reports, 7, 41374. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep41374.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mezulis, A. H., Abramson, L. Y., Hyde, J. S., & Hankin, B. L. (2004). Is there a universal positivity bias in attributions? A meta-analytic review of individual, developmental, and cultural differences in the self-serving attributional bias. Psychological bulletin, 130(5), 711.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mondino, M., Thiffault, F., & Fecteau, S. (2015). Does non-invasive brain stimulation applied over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex non-specifically influence mood and emotional processing in healthy individuals? Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience, 9, 399. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2015.00399.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Owens, M., & Gibb, B. E. (2016). Brooding rumination and attentional biases in currently non-depressed individuals: An eye-tracking study. Cognition and Emotion, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2016.1187116.

  • Quoidbach, J., Mikolajczak, M., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Positive interventions: An emotion regulation perspective. Psychological bulletin, 141(3), 655.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ray, R. D., Ochsner, K. N., Cooper, J. C., Robertson, E. R., Gabrieli, J. D., & Gross, J. J. (2005). Individual differences in trait rumination and the neural systems supporting cognitive reappraisal. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5(2), 156–168. https://doi.org/10.3758/cabn.5.2.156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rickerby, N., Krug, I., Fuller-Tyszkiewicz, M., Forte, E., Davenport, R., Chayadi, E., & Kiropoulos, L. (2022). Rumination across depression, anxiety, and eating disorders in adults: A meta-analytic review. Clinical psychology: Science and practice.

  • Roelofs, J., Muris, P., Huibers, M., Peeters, F., & Arntz, A. (2006). On the measurement of rumination: A psychometric evaluation of the ruminative response scale and the rumination on sadness scale in undergraduates. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 37(4), 299–313. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2006.03.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roese, N. J., & Epstude, K. (2017). The Functional Theory of Counterfactual Thinking: New Evidence, New Challenges, New Insights. In J. M. Olson (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 56, pp. 1–79). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2017.02.001

  • Roese, N. J., Epstude, K., Fessel, F., Morrison, M., Smallman, R., Summerville, A., Galinsky, A. D., & Segerstrom, S. (2009). Repetitive regret, Depression, and anxiety: Findings from a nationally Representative Survey. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28(6), 671–688. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2009.28.6.671.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanchez-Lopez, A., De Raedt, R., Puttevils, L., Koster, E. H. W., Baeken, C., & Vanderhasselt, M. A. (2020). Combined effects of tDCS over the left DLPFC and gaze-contingent training on attention mechanisms of emotion regulation in low-resilient individuals. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 110177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2020.110177.

  • Sanchez-Lopez, A., Everaert, J., Van Put, J., De Raedt, R., & Koster, E. H. W. (2019a). Eye-gaze contingent attention training (ECAT): Examining the causal role of attention regulation in reappraisal and rumination. Biological Psychology, 142, 116–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2019.01.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sanchez-Lopez, A., Koster, E. H. W., Van Put, J., & De Raedt, R. (2019b). Attentional disengagement from emotional information predicts future depression via changes in ruminative brooding: A five-month longitudinal eye-tracking study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 118, 30–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2019.03.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. (2000). Self-determination: The tyranny of freedom. American psychologist, 55(1), 79.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. (2004). The paradox of choice: Why more is less. Ecco

  • Slovic, P. (1966). Risk-taking in children: Age and sex differences. Child Development, 37(1), 169–176. https://doi.org/10.2307/1126437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smart, L. M., Peters, J. R., & Baer, R. A. (2016). Development and validation of a measure of self-critical rumination. Assessment, 23(3), 321–332. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191115573300.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Śmigasiewicz, K., Asanowicz, D., Westphal, N., & Verleger, R. (2014). Bias for the Left Visual Field in Rapid serial visual presentation: Effects of additional salient cues suggest a critical role of attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(2), 266–279. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00714.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. M., & Alloy, L. B. (2009). A roadmap to rumination: A review of the definition, assessment, and conceptualization of this multifaceted construct. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(2), 116–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2008.10.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smits, F. M., Schutter, D. J., van Honk, J., & Geuze, E. (2020). Does non-invasive brain stimulation modulate emotional stress reactivity? Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 15(1), 23–51. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa011.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Studer, B., Pedroni, A., & Rieskamp, J. (2013). Predicting risk-taking behavior from prefrontal resting-state activity and personality.PLoS ONE, 8(10), e76861.

  • Taylor, S. E., & Armor, D. A. (1996). Positive illusions and coping with adversity. Journal of personality, 64(4), 873–898.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., Kemeny, M. E., Reed, G. M., Bower, J. E., & Gruenewald, T. L. (2000). Psychological resources, positive illusions, and health. American psychologist, 55(1), 99.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tingley, D., Yamamoto, T., Hirose, K., Keele, L., & Imai, K. (2014). Mediation:R package for causal mediation analysis.

  • van Dijk, E., & Zeelenberg, M. (2005). On the psychology of ‘if only’: Regret and the comparison between factual and counterfactual outcomes. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 97(2), 152–160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.04.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Voyer, D., Voyer, S. D., & Tramonte, L. (2012). Free-viewing laterality tasks: A multilevel meta-analysis. Neuropsychology, 26(5), 551.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wadlinger, H. A., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2011). Fixing our focus: Training attention to regulate emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15(1), 75–102. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868310365565.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, E. R. (2009). Depressive rumination and co-morbidity: Evidence for brooding as a transdiagnostic process. Journal of rational-emotive & cognitive-behavior therapy, 27(3), 160–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Werner, K., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Emotion regulation and psychopathology:A conceptual framework.

  • Whitmer, A. J., Frank, M. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2012). Sensitivity to reward and punishment in major depressive disorder: Effects of rumination and of single versus multiple experiences. Cognition and emotion, 26(8), 1475–1485. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2012.682973.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yaroslavsky, I., Allard, E. S., & Sanchez-Lopez, A. (2019). Can’t look away: Attention control deficits predict rumination, depression symptoms and depressive affect in daily life. Journal of affective disorders, 245, 1061–1069.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yu, X., Chen, J., Zhao, S., & Yu, X. (2017). Impact of rumination and regret on depression among new employees in China. Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal, 45(9), 1499–1509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeelenberg, M., van Dijk, W. W., & Manstead, A. S. (1998). Reconsidering the relation between regret and responsibility. Organizational behavior and human decision processes, 74(3), 254–272.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, J. W., & Chen, S. (2016). Self-compassion promotes personal improvement from regret experiences via acceptance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42(2), 244–258.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This research was supported by the Research Foundation Flanders (G044019N, awarded to M-A. V. and R. D. R.).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

The experiment was designed by J. A. with guidance from M-A. V., R. D. R., and A. S-L. The data was collected by H. S. Statistical analyses were performed by J. A. The original manuscript was written by J.A. with support and feedback from H. S., M-A. V., R. D. R., and A.S-L.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jens Allaert.

Ethics declarations

Consent to Participate

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Ethical Approval

The study was conducted with the approval of Ghent University’s Medical Ethical Committee, and was in line with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic Supplementary Material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Supplementary Material 2

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Allaert, J., De Raedt, R., Sanchez-Lopez, A. et al. Reduced Attention Towards Accomplishments Mediates the Effect of Self-Critical Rumination on Regret. Cogn Ther Res 47, 399–411 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-023-10367-8

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-023-10367-8

Keywords

Navigation