Skip to main content
Log in

Picture-word interference increases with target-set size

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In the picture-word interference task the naming of a picture is hampered by the presence of a distractor word that is to be ignored. Two main components of this interference effect can be distinguished: an interference effect induced by an unrelated distractor word in comparison with a nonword control, and an additional interference effect that is due to a semantic similarity between target and distractor (calledsemantic interference). We examine whether the size of these two interference effects is affected by the number of different target pictures in an experiment. The results show that both interference effects increase with the size of the target set. This finding has two implications. First, at an empirical level, the use of a relatively small number of target pictures may account for remarkably small, or even nonsignificant, picture-word interference effects in a number of previous studies. Second, at a theoretical level, the present finding is in accordance with a name-retrieval account of picture-word interference.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bakan, P., & Alperson, B. (1967). Pronounceability, attensity, and interference in the color-word test.American Journal of Psychology, 80, 416–420.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, W. (1915). Practice in associating color names with colors.Psychological Review, 22, 45–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burke, D. M., MacKay, D. G., Worthley, J. S., Wade, E. (1991). On the tip of the tongue: What causes word finding failures in young and older adults?Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 542–579.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalrymple-Alford, E. C., & Budayr, B. (1966). Examination of some aspects of the Stroop color-word test.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 23, 1211–1214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dell, G. S., & O'Seaghdha, P. G. (1992). Stages of lexical access in language production.Cognition, 42, 287–314.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dyer, F. N. (1973). The Stroop phenomenon and its use in the study of perceptual, cognitive, and response processes.Memory & Cognition, 1, 106–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, A. W., & Young, A. W. (1988).Human cognitive neuropsychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974) Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in an nonsearch task.Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, C. W., Coles, M. G. H., Morris, L. R., & O'Hara, W. P. (1985). An electromyographic examination of response competition.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 23, 165–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, L. A., Shor, R. E., & Steinman, R. J. (1971). Semantic gradients and interference in naming color, spatial direction, and numerosity.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 91, 59–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraisse, P. (1964). Le temps de réaction verbale: Dénomination et lecture [Verbal reaction times: Naming and reading].Année psychologique, 64, 21–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glaser, W. R., & Düngelhoff, F.-J. (1984). The time course of picture-word interference.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 640–654.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glaser, W. R., & Glaser, M. O. (1989). Context effects on Stroop-like word and picture processing.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 13–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gohlson, B., & Hohle, R. H., (1968). Choice reaction times to hues printed in conflicting hue names and nonsense words.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76, 413–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golden, C. J., (1974). Effect of differing number of colors on the Stroop color and word test.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 39, 550.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hock, H. S., & Egeth, H. E. (1970). Verbal interference with encoding in a perceptual classification task.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83, 229–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jescheniak, J. D., & Levelt, W. J. M. (in press). Word frequency effects in speech production: retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological formJournal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition.

  • Keele, S. W. (1972). Attention demands of memory retrieval.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93, 245–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, G. S. (1964). Semantic power measured through the interference of words with color-naming.American Journal of Psychology, 77, 576–588.

    Google Scholar 

  • La Heij, W. (1988). Components of Stroop-like interference in picture naming.Memory & Cognition, 16, 400–410.

    Google Scholar 

  • La Heij, W., de Bruyn, E., Elens, E., Hartsuiker, R., Helaha, D., & van Schelven, L. (1990). Orthographic facilitation and categorical interference in a word-translation variant of the Stroop task.Canadian Journal of Psychology, 44, 76–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • La Heij, W., Dirkx, J., & Kramer, P. (1990). Categorical interference and associative priming in picture naming.British Journal of Psychology, 81, 511–525.

    Google Scholar 

  • La Heij, W., Starreveld, P. A., & Steehouwer, L. C. (1993). Semantic interference and orthographic facilitation in definition naming.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition.19, 352–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • La Heij, W., van der Heijden, A. H. C., & Schreuder, R. (1985). Stroop-like interference in word-naming tasks.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 62–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • La Heij, W., & Vermeij, M. (1987). Reading versus naming: The effect of target set size on contextual interference and facilitation. Perception & Psychophysics,41, 355–366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1992). Accessing words in speech production: Stages, processes and representations.Cognition, 42, 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupker, S. J. (1979). The semantic nature of response competition in the picture-word interference task.Memory & Cognition, 7, 485–495.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClain, L. (1983). Effects of response type and set size on Stroop color-word performance.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 56, 735–743.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review.Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J. (1969). Interaction of information in word recognition.Psychological Review, 76, 165–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neill, W. T. (1977). Inhibitory and facilitatory processes in selective attention.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3, 444–450.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, O. (1980).Informationsselektion und Handlungssteuerung [Selection of information and control of action]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Bochum, Germany.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, O. (1986). Facilitative and inhibitory effects of “semantic relatedness” (Report No. 111/1986). Bielefeld, Federal Republic of Germany: University of Bielefeld, Research Group on “Perception and Action”.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, O, & Kautz, L. (1982).Semantische Förderung und semantische Interferenz im Benennungsexperiment [Semantic facilitation and semantic interference in a naming experiment]. Report nr. 23/1982, University of Bochum, Germany.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oldfield, R. C., & Wingfield, A. (1965). Response latencies in naming objects.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17, 273–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, R. W. (1978). Sources of color-word interference in the Stroop color-naming task.Perception & Psychophysics, 23, 413–419.

    Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, R. W., & Fober, G. W. (1985). Repeated-stimulus superiority and inferiority effects in the identification of letters and digits.Perception & Psychophysics, 38, 125–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ray, C. (1974). The manipulation of color response times in a colorword interference task.Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 101–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K, & Springer, C. J. (1986). Graphemic and semantic similarity effects in the picture-word interference task.British Journal of Psychology, 77, 207–222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riddoch, M. J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1987). Visual object processing in optic aphasia: A case of semantic access agnosia.Cognitive Neuropsychology, 4, 131–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roelofs, A. (1992a). A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking.Cognition, 42, 107–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roelofs, A. (1992b).Lemma retrieval in speaking. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Catholic University of Nijmegen, The Netherlands.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roelofs, A. (1993). Testing a non-decompositional theory of lemma retrieval in speaking: retrieval of verbs.Cognition, 47, 59–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosinski, R. R. (1977). Picture-word interference is semantically based.Child Development, 48, 643–647.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schriefers, H., Meyer, A. S., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1990). Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production: Picture-word interference studies.Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 86–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H. K. (1977). Conceptual encoding and the locus of the Stroop effect.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 245–265.

    Google Scholar 

  • Starreveld, P. A., & La Heij, W. (in press). Semantic interference, orthographic facilitation and their interaction in naming tasks.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition.

  • Taylor, D. A. (1977). Time course of context effects.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 4, 404–426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tipper, S. P. (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37A, 571–590.

    Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, G., & Briggs, P. (1984). The development of word recognition processes.British Journal of Psychology, 75, 243–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vitkovitch, M., & Humphreys, G. W. (1991). Perseverant responding in speeded naming of pictures: It's in the links.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 664–680.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warren, C., & Morton, J. (1982). The effects of priming on picture recognition.British Journal of Psychology, 73, 117–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warren, R. E. (1974). Association, directionality, and stimulus encoding.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, 151–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, E. (1977). The effects of amount of information in the Stroop color word test.Perception & Psychophysics, 22, 463–470.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wingfield, A. (1968). Effects of frequency on identification and naming of objects.American Journal of Psychology, 81, 226–234.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Wido La Heij.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

La Heij, W., van den Hof, E. Picture-word interference increases with target-set size. Psychol. Res 58, 119–133 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00571100

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00571100

Keywords

Navigation