Skip to main content
Log in

The impact of response format on relations among intentions, attitudes, and social norms

  • Published:
Marketing Letters Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This research investigates the response consistency error which may be induced by certain common methodological practices in tests of predictive models such as the theory of reasoned action (Ajzen and Fishbein 1980). Specifically, the physical presence of a questionnaire was expected to provide respondents an inducement and basis for maintaining consistent responses. In addition, the practice of presenting all measures of a given construct together seemed likely to heighten correlations among constructs, resulting in heightened predictions. Two experiments were conducted in which respondents completed measures of the theory of reasoned action for a variety of behaviors using either a paper or computer-administered questionnaire with items in standard or random order. The results of these studies indicate that, contrary to expectations, the degree of attitude-intention consistency across behaviors may be attentuated in standard paper administrations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ajzen, Icek, and MartinFishbein. (1980). Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bagozzi, Richard P. (1982). “A Field Investigation of Causal Relations Among Cognitions, Affect, Intentions, and Behavior,” Journal of Marketing Research 19 (November), 562–584.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beckwith, Neil E., and U. VictorKubilius. (1978). “Empirical Evidence of Halo Effects in Store Image Research by Estimating True Locations,” Advances in Consumer Research 5, 485–493.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beckwith, Neil E., and Donald R.Lehmann. (1975). “The Importance of Halo Effects in Multi-Attribute Attitude Models,” Journal of Marketing Research 12 (August), 265–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beckwith, Neil E., Harold H.Kassarjian, and Donald R.Lehmann. (1978). “Halo Effects in Marketing Research: Review and Prognosis,” Advances in Consumer Research 5, 465–467.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chan, Florentius, JosephKarbowski, Richard A.Monty, and Lawrence C.Perlmuter. (1986). “Performance as a Source of Perceived Control,” Motivation and Emotion 10 (1), 59–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, William H. (1981). “Ubiquitous Halo,” Psychological Bulletin 90 (2), 218–244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., T. M.Lenn, and E. A.Effrein. (1984). “Spontaneous Attitude Formation,” Social Cognition 2, 217–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, Jack M., and John G.LynchJr. (1988). “Self-Generated Validity and Other Effects of Measurement on Belief, Attitude, Intention and Behavior,” Journal of Applied Psychology 73 (3), 421–435.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laroche, Michel. (1978). “Four Methodological Problems in Multi-Attribute Attitude Models,” Advances in Consumer Research 5, 175–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liefeld, John P. (1988). “Response Effects in Computer Administered Questioning,” Journal of Marketing Research 25 (November), 405–409.

    Google Scholar 

  • O'Brien, Terry, and ValerieDugdale. (1978). “Questionnaire Administration by Computer,” Journal of the Market Research Society 20(4), 228–237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheppard, Blair H., JonHartwick, and Paul R.Warshaw. (1988). “The Theory of Reasoned Action: A Meta-Analysis of Past Research with Recommendations for Modifications and Future Research,” Journal of Consumer Research 15 (December), 325–343.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tesser, Abraham. (1978). “Self-Generated Attitude Change.” In L.Berkowitz (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, New York: Academic Press. 289–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1920). “A Constant Error in Psychological Ratings,” Journal of Applied Psychology 4, 25–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tourangeau, Roger, and Kenneth A.Rasinski. (1988). “Cognitive Processes Underlying Context Effects in Attitude Measurement,” Psychological Bulletin 103(3), 299–314.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, F. L. (1907). “A Statistical Study of Literary Merit,” Archives of Psychology 1 (7).

  • Wilkie, William L., John M.McCann, and David J.Reibstein. (1973). “Halo Effects in Brand Belief Measurement: Implications for Attitude Model Development,” Advances in Consumer Research 1, 280–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, Timothy D., Dana S.Dunn, DoloresKraft, and Douglas J.Lisle. (1989). “Introspection, Attitude Change, and Attitude-Behavior Consistency: The Disruptive Effects of Explaining Why We Feel the Way We Do.” In L.Berkowitz (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 287–343.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Ellen, P.S., Madden, T.J. The impact of response format on relations among intentions, attitudes, and social norms. Market Lett 1, 161–170 (1990). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00435299

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00435299

Key words

Navigation