Skip to main content

Young Children’s Ideas About Earth and Space Science Concepts

  • Chapter
Research in Early Childhood Science Education

Abstract

This chapter focuses on young children’s conceptual understanding of earth and space science concepts. Most studies in the domain of earth science have targeted young children’s understanding of some meteorological phenomena including the formation of rain and clouds, evaporation and water cycle, wind, and thunder and lightning. There is a lack of research that addressed young children’s conceptual understanding of earth’s materials and the pattern of changes in these materials over time. A relatively large number of studies have targeted young children’s understanding of space science concepts. Most of these studies have focused on children’s understanding of the shape of the earth, day and night cycle, seasons, and lunar concepts. Several directions for further research have identified. Future studies should focus on investigating (1) young children’s understanding of properties of rocks and soils and the pattern of changes in the earth’s materials over time, (2) influence of second-order constrains on young children’s understanding of related astronomical phenomena, (3) the role of specific cognitive and affective characteristics that facilitate young children’s understanding of earth and space science concepts, (4) the developmental trajectories of conceptual understanding of earth and space science concepts in young children, (5) children’s early science learning experience at home, (6) possible gender differences in understanding earth and space science concepts and whether early gender socialization leads children to develop different levels of expertise in the domains of science, and (7) possible constraints and contributions of culture on children’s understandings of earth and space science concepts.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 139.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Alexander, J. M., Johnson, K. E., & Kelly, K. (2012). Longitudinal analysis of the relations between opportunities to learn about science and the development of interests related to science. Science Education, 96(5), 763–786.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aron, R. H., Francek, M. A., Nelson, B. D., & Biasrd, W. J. (1994). Atmospheric misconceptions. The Science Teacher, 61(1), 30–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atwood, V. A., & Atwood, K. (1995). Preservice elementary teachers’ conceptions of what causes night and day. School Science and Mathematics, 95(6), 290–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atwood, R. K., & Atwood, V. A. (1996). Preservice elementary teachers’ conceptions of the causes of the seasons. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 33(5), 553–563.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ausubel, D. P. (1963). The psychology of meaningful verbal learning. New York: Grune & Stratton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ausubel, D. P. (1968). Educational psychology: A cognitive view. San Francisco: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, J. M., & Slater, T. F. (2003). A review of astronomy education research. Astronomy Education Review, 2(2), 20–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bar, V. (1989). Children’s views about the water cycle. Science Education, 73(4), 481–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bar, V., & Galili, I. (1994). Stages of children’s views about evaporation. International Journal of Science Education, 16(2), 157–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnett, M., & Morran, J. (2002). Addressing children’s alternative frameworks of the Moon’s phases and eclipses. International Journal of Science Education, 24(8), 859–879.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baxter, J. (1989). Children’s understanding of familiar astronomical events. International Journal of Science Education, 11(special issue), 502–513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blown, E. J., & Bryce, T. G. K. (2006). Knowledge restructuring in the development of children’s cosmologies. International Journal of Science Education, 28(12), 1411–1462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blown, E. J., & Bryce, T. G. K. (2013). Thought-experiments about gravity in the history of science and in research into children’s thinking. Science and Education, 1–63. doi: 10.1007/s11191-012-9548-3.

  • Broadstock, M. J. (1992). Elementary students’ alternative conceptions about earth systems phenomena in Taiwan, Republic of China. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Ohio State University, Columbus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryce, T. G. K., & Blown, E. J. (2006). Cultural mediation of children’s cosmologies: A longitudinal study of the astronomy concepts of Chinese and New Zealand children. International Journal of Science Education, 28(10), 1113–1160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryce, T. G. K., & Blown, E. J. (2007). Gender effects in children’s development and education. International Journal of Science Education, 29(13), 1655–1678.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Callanan, M. A., Jipson, J. L., & Soennichsen, M. S. (2002). Maps, globes, and videos: Parent–child conversations about representational objects. In S. G. Paris (Ed.), Perspectives on object-centered learning in museums (pp. 261–283). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diakidoy, A., Vosniadou, S., & Hawks, S. (1997). Conceptual change in astronomy: Models of the earth and of the day/night cycle in American-Indian children. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 12(2), 159–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doğru, M., & Şeker, F. (2012). The effect of science activities on concept acquisition of age 5–6 children groups-the effect of science activities on concept acquisition of age 5–6 children groups. Educational Sciences: Theory and Practice, 12(4), 3011–3024.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donaldson, M. J. (1973). Development of understanding of selected science phenomena in young children. Unpublished dissertation, The University of Tennessee.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dove, J. (1998). Alternative conceptions about weather. School Science Review, 79(289), 65–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Driver, R., Guesne, E., & Tiberghien, A. (1985). Children’s ideas and the learning of science. In R. Driver, E. Guesne, & A. Tiberghien (Eds.), Children’s ideas in science (pp. 1–9). Philadelphia: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunlop, J. (2000). How children observe the universe. Publication of Astronomical Society of Australia, 17, 194–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frède, V., et al. (2011). The acquisition of scientific knowledge: The influence of methods of questioning and analysis on the interpretation of children’s conceptions of the earth. Infant and Child Development, 20(6), 432–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, N. K. (2007). Preschoolers’ perceptions of gender appropriate toys and their parents’ beliefs about genderized behaviors: Miscommunication, mixed messages, or hidden truths? Early Childhood Education Journal, 34(5), 357–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hannust, T., & Kikas, E. (2007). Children’s knowledge of astronomy and its change in the course of learning. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 22, 89–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hannust, T., & Kikas, E. (2010). Young children’s acquisition of knowledge about the Earth: A longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 164–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haupt, G. W. (1950). First grade concepts of the moon, part 2 by interview. Science Education, 34(4), 224–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, B. K., Goodhew, A., Heit, E., & Gillian, J. (2003). The role of diverse instruction in conceptual change. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 253–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, S. M., Trundle, K. C., & Saçkes, M. (2010). Using a planetarium software program to promote conceptual change with young children. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 19(2), 165–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inagaki, K. (1992). Piagetian and post-Piagetian conceptions of development and their implications for science education in early childhood. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 7, 115–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inbody, D. (1964). Children’s understandings of natural phenomena. Science Education, 47(3), 270–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kallery, M. (2011). Astronomical concepts and events awareness for young children. International Journal of Science Education, 33(2), 341–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kavanagh, C., Agan, L., & Sneider, C. (2005). Learning about phases of the moon and eclipses: A guide for teachers and curriculum developers. The Astronomy Education Review, 4(1), 19–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kikas, E. (1998). The impact of teaching on students’ definitions and explanations of astronomical phenomena. Learning and Instruction, 8(5), 439–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Küçüközer, H., & Bostan, A. (2010). Ideas of kindergarten students on the day-night cycles, the seasons and the moon phases. Journal of Theory and Practice in Education, 6(2), 267–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Küçüközer, H., Korkusuz, M. E., Küçüközer, H. A., & Yürümezoğlu, K. (2009). The effect of 3d computer modeling and observation-based instruction on the conceptual change regarding basic concepts of astronomy in elementary school students. Astronomy Education Review, 8(1), 010104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lelliott, A., & Rollnick, M. (2010). Big ideas: A review of astronomy education research from 1975–2008. International Journal of Science Education, 32(13), 1771–1799.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miner, J. T. (1992). An early childhood study of the water cycle. Unpublished master thesis, University of Nevada, Las Vegas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moyle, R. (1980). Weather. Learning in science project working paper (Vol. 21). Hamilton: University of Waikato.

    Google Scholar 

  • Munn, M. M. (1974). Children’s conceptions of selected astronomical and meteorological phenomena. Unpublished master of art thesis, The Ohio State University, Columbus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, B. D., Aron, R. H., & Francek, M. A. (1992). Clarification of selected misconceptions in physical geography. Journal of Geography, 91(2), 76–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nobes, G., Martin, A. E., & Panagiotaki, G. (2005). The development of scientific knowledge of the earth. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 47–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nobes, G., et al. (2003). Children’s understanding of the earth in a multicultural community: Mental models or fragments of knowledge? Developmental Science, 6(1), 72–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nobes, G., & Panagiotaki, G. (2007). Adults’ representations of the earth: Implications for children’s acquisition of scientific concepts. British Journal of Psychology, 98, 645–665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nussbaum, J. (1985). The earth as a cosmic body. In R. Driver, E. Guesne, & A. Tiberghien (Eds.), Children’s ideas in science (pp. 170–192). Philadelphia: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oakes, M. (1947). Children’s explanations of natural phenomena. New York: Teacher College, Columbia University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osborne, J., Black, P. J., Wadsworth, P., & Meadows, J. (1994). SPACE research report: The earth in space. Liverpool: University of Liverpool.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osborne, R. J., & Cosgrove, M. M. (1983). Children’s conceptions of the changes of state of water. Journal of Research in Science Education, 20(9), 825–838.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panagiotaki, G., Nobes, G., & Banerjee, R. (2006). Is the world round or flat? Children’s understanding of the earth. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3(2), 124–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Panagiotaki, G., Nobes, G., & Potton, A. (2009). Mental models and other misconceptions in children’s understanding of the earth. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 52–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papadimitriou, V., & Londridou, P. (2001). A cross-age study of pupils’ conceptions concerning the movement of air masses in the troposphere. Paper presented at the IOSTE symposium in Southern Europe, Paralimni. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 466 368).

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1972a). Child’s conceptions of the world (J. A. Tomlinson, Trans.). Lanham: Littlefield Adams. (Original work published 1928).

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1972b). The child conception of physical causality (M. Gabain, Trans.). Totowa: Littlefield, Adams & Co. (Original work published 1930).

    Google Scholar 

  • Plummer, J. D. (2009). A cross-age study of children’s knowledge of apparent celestial motion. International Journal of Science Education, 31(12), 1571–1605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plummer, J. D., & Krajcik, J. (2010). Building a learning progression for celestial motion: Elementary levels from an earth-based perspective. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 47(7), 768–787.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plummer, J. D., Wasko, K., & Slagle, C. (2011). Children learning to explain daily celestial motion: Understanding astronomy across moving frames of reference. International Journal of Science Education, 33(14), 1963–1992.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roald, I., & Mikalsen, O. (2001). Configuration and dynamics of the earth-sun-moon system: An investigation into conceptions of deaf and hearing pupils. International Journal of Science Education, 23, 423–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russel, T., & Watt, D. (1990). Evaporation and condensation (Primary SPACE project research report). Liverpool: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, T., Bell, D., Longden, K., & McGuigan, L. (1993). Rocks, soil and weather (Primary SPACE project research report). Liverpool: Liverpool University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saçkes, M. (2014). Parents who want their PreK children to have science learning experiences are outliers. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(2), 132–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saçkes, M., Flevares, L. M., & Trundle, K. C. (2010). Four– to six-year-old children’s conceptions of the mechanism of rainfall. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25(4), 536–546.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saçkes, M., Trundle, K. C., Bell, R. L., & O’Connell, A. A. (2011). The influence of early science experience in kindergarten on children’s immediate and later science achievement: Evidence from the early childhood longitudinal study. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 48(2), 217–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saçkes, M., Trundle, K. C., & Krissek, L. (2011). The impact of a summer institute on inservice early childhood teachers’ knowledge of earth and space science concepts. The Science Educator, 20(1), 23–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Samarapungavan, A., Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. (1996). Mental models of the earth, sun and moon: Indian children’s cosmologies. Cognitive Development, 11, 491–521.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schoon, K. J. (1989). Misconceptions in the earth sciences: A cross-age study. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Associations for Research in Science Teaching, San Francisco.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoultz, J., Saljo, R., & Wyndhamn, J. (2001). Heavenly talk: Discourse, artifacts, and children’s understanding of elementary astronomy. Human Development, 44, 103–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sharp, J. G. (1996). Children’s astronomical beliefs: A preliminary study of year 6 children in southwest England. International Journal of Science Education, 18, 685–712.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sharp, J. G. (1999). Young children’s ideas about the earth in space. International Journal of Early Years Education, 7(2), 159–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegal, M., Butterwort, G., & Newcombe, P. A. (2004). Culture and children’s cosmology. Developmental Science, 7(3), 308–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simpkins, S. D., Davis-Kean, P. E., & Eccles, J. S. (2006). Math and science motivation: A longitudinal examination of the links between choices and belief. Developmental Psychology, 42(1), 70–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spiropoulou, D., Kostopoulos, D., & Jacovides, C. P. (1999). Greek children’s alternative conceptions on weather and climate. School Science Review, 81(294), 55–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stahly, L. L., Krockover, G. H., & Shepardson, D. P. (1999). Third grade students’ ideas about the lunar phases. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 36(2), 159–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Straatemeier, M., van der Maas, H. L. J., & Jansen, B. R. J. (2008). Children’s knowledge of the earth: A new methodological and statistical approach. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 100(4), 276–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tao, Y., Oliver, M., & Venville, G. (2012). Long-term outcomes of early childhood science education: Insights from a cross-national comparative case study on conceptual understanding of science. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 10(6), 1269–1302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tenenbaum, H. R., Snow, C. E., Roach, K., & Kurland, B. (2005). Talking and reading science: Longitudinal data on sex differences in mother–child conversations in low-income families. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 26, 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tracy, D. M. (1987). Toys, spatial ability, and science and mathematics achievement: Are they related? Sex Roles, 17, 115–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trundle, K. C., Atwood, R. K., & Christopher, J. E. (2002). Preservice elementary teachers’ conceptions of moon phases before and after instruction. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39(7), 633–658.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trundle, K. C., Atwood, R. K., & Christopher, J. E. (2007). Fourth grade elementary students’ conceptions of standards-based lunar concepts. International Journal of Science Education, 29(5), 595–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trundle, K. C., & Saçkes, M. (2012). Science and early education. In R. C. Pianta, W. S. Barnett, L. M. Justice, & S. M. Sheridan (Eds.), Handbook of early childhood education (pp. 240–258). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trundle, K. C., Saçkes, M., Smith, M. M., & Miller, H. L. (2012, September). Preschoolers’ ideas about day and night and objects in the sky. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the International Congress on Early Childhood Education. Adana, September 12–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tytler, R. (2000). A comparison of year 1 and year 6 students’ conceptions of evaporation and condensation: Dimensions of conceptual progression. International Journal of Science Education, 22(5), 447–467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tytler, R., & Peterson, S. (2004). Young children learning about evaporation: A longitudinal perspective. Canadian Journal of Science, Mathematics, and Technology Education, 4(1), 111–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valanides, N., Gritsi, F., Kampeza, M., & Ravanis. (2000). Changing pre-school children’s conceptions of the day/night cycle. International Journal of Early Years Education, 8(1), 27–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Venville, G., Wilhelm, J., & Louisell, R. (2012). A complex dynamic systems view of young children’s knowledge about the Moon. Research in Science Education, 42(4), 729–752.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S. (1994). Universal and culture-specific properties of children’s mental models of the earth. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 412–430). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1992). Mental models of the earth: A study of conceptual change in childhood. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 535–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1994). Mental models of the day/night cycle. Cognitive Science, 18(1), 123–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., Skopeliti, I., & Ikospentaki, K. (2004). Modes of knowing and ways of reasoning in elementary astronomy. Cognitive Development, 19(2), 203–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., Skopeliti, I., & Ikospentaki, K. (2005). Reconsidering the role of artifacts in reasoning: Children’s understanding of the globe as a model of the earth. Learning and Instruction, 15, 331–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilhelm, J. (2009). A case study of three children’s original interpretations of the moon’s changing appearance. School Science and Mathematics, 109(5), 258–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Za’rour, G. I. (1976). Interpretation of natural phenomena by Lebanese school children. Science Education, 60(2), 277–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mesut Saçkes .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Saçkes, M. (2015). Young Children’s Ideas About Earth and Space Science Concepts. In: Cabe Trundle, K., Saçkes, M. (eds) Research in Early Childhood Science Education. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-9505-0_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics