Skip to main content

The Stream of Consciousness: Literary Psychology as the First Uniquely American Phenomenology in the Works of William James and His Swedenborgian and Transcendentalist Milieu

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover Art, Literature, and Passions of the Skies

Part of the book series: Analecta Husserliana ((ANHU,volume 112))

Abstract

Husserlians will possibly be shocked at the idea that there might have been a uniquely American phenomenology before Husserl and before the first flourishing of that movement in the US in the late 1950s (Tymieniecka 1989). Theirs is principally a Continental understanding of the subject and we are grateful that they are its foremost champions. A case can be made, however, for a uniquely American phenomenological tradition beginning in the early nineteenth century with the dramatic and charismatic discourse of an aspiring Swedenborgian minister, Sampson Reed, whose “Oration on Genius” at Harvard Class Day in 1821 captivated the young Ralph Waldo Emerson who was sitting in the audience. “Native gold,” he called it, and it became later known, according to Perry Miller, the American intellectual historian, as “the first salvo of the Transcendentalist movement in New England.”

The manner in which the Swedenborgian and transcendentalist impulse can be understood as literary psychology can be seen in the writings of both Emerson and Hawthorne, and certainly in Melville, but also in the work of James John Garth Wilkinson, English homeopath and translator of Swedenborg’s scientific and medical writings, and close friend of Thomas Carlyle and also Henry James Sr.. Wilkinson had published a book of poems that contained in an appendix “an infallible method for automatic speaking, writing, and drawing,” which Emerson drew from, and which the psychologist Saul Rosenzweig later demonstrated was most likely the origin of the James brothers’ steam of consciousness technique (Rosenzweig, Contemporary Psychology III:250–257, 1958).

William James, direct inheritor of the Swedenborgian and Transcendentalist literary legacy, had incorporated a chapter on the “Stream of Consciousness” in his monumental Principles of Psychology in 1890; James’s brand of phenomenology was the source of his data base in The Varieties of Religious Experience (902), and it figured in his definition of phenomenology which he argued with Charles Sanders Peirce about around James’s conception of radical empiricism in 1904. It also gives us a clue to the vitality of the James renaissance that was initiated almost single handedly by the phenomenologist John Wild of Harvard in the 1960s (Wild 1969).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Achterberg, J. 1989. Mind and medicine: The role of imagery in healing. Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research 83(2): 93–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aronson, J., E. Dietrich, and E. Way. 1992. Throwing the conscious baby out with the Cartesian bath water. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15(2): 202–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baars, Bernard J. 1988. A cognitive theory of consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, Bernard J. 1993. How does a serial, integrated and very limited stream of consciousness emerge from a nervous system that is mostly unconscious, distributed, parallel and of enormous capacity? In Experimental and theoretical studies of consciousness. Ciba Foundation Symposium, 174. BK: 4200; Conference presentation. Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, Bernard J., and Michael Fehling. 1992. Consciousness is associated with central as well as distributed processes. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15(2): 203–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benjamin, Lorna S. 1986. Operational definition and measurement of dynamics shown in the stream of free associations. Psychiatry 49(2): 104–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bogen, Joseph E., Bredo S. Johnsen, and Annti Revonsuo. 1992. Commentary on Daniel C. Dennett and Marcel Kinsbourne. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17(1): 175–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonanno, George A. 1993. Controlling one’s stream of thought through perceptual and reflective processing. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, David M., Suzzane Ball, and David Pape. 1991. An experimental investigation of thought suppression. Behaviour Research and Therapy 29(3): 253–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, David M., Emma Winton, and Lenka Thynn. 1993. A further experimental investigation of thought suppression. Behaviour Research and Therapy 31(2): 207–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cornock, Stroud. 1984. Implications of lateralisation of brain function for art education: A critical review. Educational Psychology 4(2): 139–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cox, Murray. 1992. Shakespeare comes to Broadmoor: The actors are come hither’: The performance of tragedy in a secure psychiatric hospital. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, Fergus I.M. 1989. On the making of episodes. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crook, John H. 1990. Complementary approaches to consciousness. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, Daniel C., and Marcel Kinsbourne. 1992. Time and the observer: The where and when of consciousness in the brain. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15(2): 183–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diaz, Jose-Luis. 1996. The stream revisited: A process model of phenomenological consciousness. Cambridge: MIT Press. 1996.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eiser, J.Richard. 1994. Attitudes, chaos and the connectionist mind. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emerson, R.W. 1884. “Demonology,” Lectures and biographical sketches. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farthing, G.William. 1992. The psychology of consciousness. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fizer, J. 1972. Ingarden’s pases, Bergson’s duree reelle, and William James’ stream: Metaphoric variants or mutually exclusive concepts on the theme of time. Dialectics and Humanism 2: 33–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flanagan, Owen. 1992. Consciousness reconsidered. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, John H. 1993. Young children’s understanding of thinking and consciousness. Current Directions in Psychological Science 2(2): 40–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, John H., Frances L. Green, and Eleanor R. Flavell. 1993. Children’s understanding of the stream of consciousness. Child Development 64(2): 387–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. 1904/1959. Freud’s psychoanalytic method. In Sigmund Freud: Collected papers, vol 1, 265–271. Repr. New York: Basic Books, 1959.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, Harry T. 1995. On the nature of consciousness: Cognitive, phenomenological, and transpersonal perspectives. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, William. 1890/1990. The stream of thought. In From sentience to symbols: Readings on consciousness, ed. John Pickering and Martin Skinner. 152–170. Repr. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalupahana, David J. 1987. The principles of Buddhist psychology, SUNY series in Buddhist studies. Albany: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kyora, Sabin. 1994. Eine andere Dimension: Jacques Lacan und Italo Svevo [Another dimension: Jacques Lacan and Italo Svevo]. Zeitschrift für Psychoanalytische Theorie und Praxis 9(1): 83–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lavy, Edith H., Van den Hout, and A. Marcel. 1990. Thought suppression induces intrusions. Behavioural Psychotherapy 18(4): 251–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mahoney, Michael J. 1991. Human change processes: The scientific foundations of psychotherapy. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meares, Russell. 1995. Episodic memory, trauma, and the narrative of self. Contemporary Psychoanalysis 31(4): 541–556.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milner, A.D. 1995. Cerebral correlates of visual awareness. Special issue: The biology and neuropsychology of consciousness. Neuropsychologia 33(9): 1117–1130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muris, Peter, Harald Merckelbach, and Peter F. de Jong. 1993. Verbalization and environmental cuing in thought suppression. Behaviour Research and Therapy 31(6): 609–612.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1985. Concerning the unity of consciousness: II. William James on personal conscious unity. Imagination, Cognition and Personality 5(1): 21–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1986. The six basic concepts of consciousness and William James’s stream of thought. Imagination, Cognition and Personality 6(4): 289–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1987. Gibson, James, and the temporal continuity of experience. Imagination, Cognition and Personality 7(4): 351–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1988a. Consciousness and commissurotomy: II. Some pertinencies for intact functioning. Journal of Mind and Behavior 9(4): 515–547.

    Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1988b. Is any state of consciousness self-intimating? Journal of Mind and Behavior 9(2): 167–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1988c. Sympathy, empathy, and the stream of consciousness. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 18(2): 169–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1989a. From visual sensations to the seen-now and the seen-from-here. Psychological Research 51(3): 87–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1989b. Understanding William James’s conception of consciousness with the help of Gerald E. Myers. Imagination, Cognition and Personality 8(4): 323–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1991. Introspecting and consciousness: A response to Howe. New Ideas in Psychology 9(1): 45–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1992a. Consciousness and commissurotomy: IV. Three hypothesized dimensions of deconnected left-hemispheric consciousness. Journal of Mind and Behavior 13(1): 37–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1992b. The stream of consciousness: II. William James’s specious present. Imagination, Cognition and Personality 12(4): 367–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1993a. An introduction to reflective seeing: I. Journal of Mind and Behavior 14(3): 235–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1993b. The stream of consciousness: III. Adolf Grunbaum’s nowness. Imagination, Cognition and Personality 13(1): 73–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1993c. Consciousness and commissurotomy: V. Concerning an hypothesis of normal dual consciousness. Journal of Mind and Behavior 14(2): 179–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1993d. The stream of consciousness: I. William James’s pulses. Imagination, Cognition and Personality 12(1): 3–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1994a. The stream of consciousness: VI. Relevancies from imagination, cognition and personality. Imagination, Cognition and Personality 14(1): 59–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1994b. An introduction to reflective seeing: II. Journal of Mind and Behavior 15(4): 351–374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1994c. The concept of consciousness-sub-5: The unitive meaning. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 24(4): 401–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1994d. The stream of consciousness: IV. Adolf Grunbaum’s atomicity. Imagination, Cognition and Personality 13(3): 229–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1994e. The stream of consciousness: V. William James’s change of view. Imagination, Cognition and Personality 13(4): 347–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1995a. Consciousness and commissurotomy: VI. Evidence for normal dual consciousness? Journal of Mind and Behavior 16(2): 181–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1995b. The stream of consciousness: VII. Further relevances from imagination, cognition and personality. Imagination, Cognition and Personality 14(2): 131–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, Thomas. 1995c. The stream of consciousness: VIII. James’s ejective consciousness (First Part). Imagination, Cognition and Personality 14(4): 333–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neimeyer, Robert A. 1993. Constructivist approaches to the measurement of meaning. In Constructivist assessment: A casebook, Counseling psychologist casebook series, vol. 2, ed. Greg J. Neimeyer, 58–103. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal, Paula M. 1994. The heart’s eye: Emotional influences in perception and attention. San Diego: Academic Press, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paranjpe, Anand Chintaman. 1988. On dealing with the stream of consciousness: A comparison of Husserl and Yoga. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennebaker, James W. 1989. Stream of consciousness and stress: Levels of thinking. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennebaker, James W., Jeanne A. Czajka, Russell Cropanzano, and B.C. Richards. 1990. Levels of thinking. Special issue: Centennial celebration of The principles of psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 16(4): 743–757.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pollio, H.R. 1909. The stream of consciousness since James. In Reflections on the principles of psychology: William James after a century, ed. M.G. Johnson and T.B. Henley, 271–294. Hilsdale: Lawrence Earlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pope, K.S., and J.L. Singer (eds.). 1978. The stream of consciousness: Scientific Investigations into the flow of human experience. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, Frank W. 1993. Dissociative disorders in children: Behavioral profiles and problems. Special issue: Clinical recognition of sexually abused children, Child Abuse and Neglect 17(1): 39–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenzweig, S. 1958. The Jameses’ stream of consciousness. Contemporary Psychology III: 250–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutkowski, Ryszard, and Teresa Gordon. 1995. The crisis in psychiatry and the protection of the civil rights of mentally ill patients. Psychiatria Polska 29(3 Suppl): 19–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schreiber, Evelyn. 1994. Stream-of-consciousness and Freud’s primary process: Comprehending Pinter’s Old Times. Literature and Psychology 40(1–2): 71–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharma, Arvind. 1984. Two techniques of sound meditation in yogic Hinduism and Mahayana Buddism: A comparison. International Journal of Eclectic Psychotherapy 3(1): 10–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharron, Avery. 1985. The mainstream of consciousness: An interactionist analysis of a phenomenological concept. Symbolic Interaction 8(1): 47–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, Jerome L. 1984. The private personality. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 10(1): 7–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, Jerome L. 1993. Experimental studies of ongoing conscious experience. Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steinberg, Edwin R.1982–83. The stream-of-consciousness technique in the novel: Simulating a stream of concurrencies. Imagination, Cognition and Personality 2(3): 241–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephen, Michele. 1989. Constructing sacred worlds and autonomous imagining in New Guinea. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strange, J.R. 1978. A search for the sources of the stream of consciousness. In The stream of consciousness: Scientific Investigations into the flow of human experience, ed. K.S. Pope and J.L. Singer, 10–30. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E.I. 1978. Asian interpretations: Transcending the stream of consciousness. In The stream of consciousness: Scientific Investigations into the flow of human experience, ed. K.S. Pope and J.L. Singer, 31–54. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E.I. 1984. William James on psychopathology: An archival memoir. In Essays in honor of Prof. Josef Brozek, Monographias de la Revista Historia de la Psychologia, ed. H. Carpentero and J.M. Peiro, 357–365. Valencia: University of Valencia, Spain.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E.I. 1985a. Psychotherapy, Harvard, and the American Society for Psychical Research, l884-l889. In Proceedings of the 28th Annual Convention of the Parapsychological Association 319–346. Tufts University, Medford, MA, 15 Aug l985.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E.I. 1985b. James Jackson Putnam’s fateful meeting with Freud: The l909 Clark University conference. Voices; The Art and Science of Psychotherapy 21(1): 78–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E.I. 1986. Research in Parapsychology 1985. Metchuen, NJ: Secaucus Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E.I. 1988a. The appearance of Swedenborg in the history of American psychology. In Swedenborg and his influence, ed. E.J. Brock et al., 155–178. Bryn Athyn: The General Church.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E.I. 1988b. On the first use of ‘Psychoanalysis’ at the Massachusetts General Hospital, 1903–1905. Journal of the History of Medicine and Allied Sciences 43(4): 447–471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E.I. l982. Louville Eugene Emerson: Psychotherapy, Harvard, and the early Boston scene. Harvard Medical Alumni Bulletin 56(2): 42–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E.I. 1988a. Ralph Waldo Emerson: The Swedenborgian and Transcendentalist connection. In Emanuel Swedenborg; The vision continues, ed. R. Larsen. 300th anniversary volume, 127–136. New York: The Swedenborg Foundation. Reprinted in Testimony to the Invisible, ed. J. Lawrence. San Francisco: J. Appleseed and Co., 1995.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tymienieka, A.T. 1989. The Theme: The History of American Phenomenology-in-Process. In Eugene Francis Kaelin & Calvin O. Schrag (eds.). American Phenomenology: Origins and development, Analecta Hussereliana, vol. 26, (ix–xxvii) Vermont: World Institute for Advanced Phenomenological Research and Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uleman, James S. 1989. Unintended thought. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varenne, Herve. 1989. A confusion of signs: The semiosis of anthropological Ireland. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Velmans, Max. 1992. Is consciousness integrated? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15(2): 229–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, Daniel M., David J. Schneider, Samuel R. Carter, and Teri L. White. 1987. Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 53(1): 5–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, Daniel M., David J. Schneider, Brian Knutson, and Sharon R. McMahon. 1991. Polluting the stream of consciousness: The effect of thought suppression on the mind’s environment. Cognitive Therapy and Research 15(2): 141–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wenzlaff, Richard M., Daniel M. Wegner, and David W. Roper. 1988. Depression and mental control: The resurgence of unwanted negative thoughts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 55(6): 882–892.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wild, John Daniel, 1969. The radical empiricism of William James. Garden City, N.Y.: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, James John Garth 1857a. Improvisations from the spirit. London/Manchester: W. White/Dunnill and Palmer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, James John Garth, 1857b. Improvisations from the spirit. London/Manchester: W. White/Dunnill and Palmer.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2012 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Taylor, E. (2012). The Stream of Consciousness: Literary Psychology as the First Uniquely American Phenomenology in the Works of William James and His Swedenborgian and Transcendentalist Milieu. In: Tymieniecka, A. (eds) Art, Literature, and Passions of the Skies. Analecta Husserliana, vol 112. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-4261-1_22

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics