Skip to main content

Strategy-Specific Information Access in Knowledge Acquisition from Hypertext

  • Chapter
Discourse, Tools and Reasoning

Part of the book series: NATO ASI Series ((NATO ASI F,volume 160))

Abstract

A central issue in learning from hypertext is the nonlinearity of presenting information. Nonlinearity, however, does not have a facultative effect on learning per se. The essential point is rather how learners make use of this nonlinearity, that is, how they interact with hypertexts, which strategies they use, and what kind of knowledge they construct. This chapter is concerned with differences between successful and unsuccessful strategies in learning about a complex subject matter from hypertext. In an empirical study, subjects had to answer a series of questions with the help of a hypertext. Cognitive processing was analyzed through the think-aloud method. A group of successful learners and a group of unsuccessful learners were identified by their scores in a comprehension test presented after learning. The results indicated that the two groups used different strategies, which were reflected by different patterns of information access. The successful learners tried to construct a mental model of the subject domain, which was then successively elaborated. They combined model construction, model analysis, and symbol processing in a flexible way, and tried to solve inconsistencies by elaborating the respective model structure. In contrast, unsuccessful learners tried to get by with a symbol processing strategy and tackled the construction of a mental model only when the former strategy could no longer be applied. Inconsistencies were solved by ad hoc repairs of procedures rather than by systematic elaboration of the model structure. The respective strategies can be related to a global and a local orientation of knowledge acquisition. Instructional implications for the selecting and sequencing of learning tasks, as well as for the display of information in the design of learning environments with hypertext, are pointed out.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 169.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Armbruster, B. B., & Armstrong, J. O. (1993). Locating information in text: A focus on children in the elementary grades. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 1.(2), 139–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory: An introduction. International Journal of Psycholog., 2.(3/4), 96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, L. (1985). How do we know when we don’t understand? Standards for evaluating text comprehension. In D. L. Forrest-Pressley, G. E. MacKinnon, & T. G. Waller (Eds.), Metacognition, cognition, and human performance. Vol. I: Theoretical perspective. (pp. 155–205). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. S., Burton, R. R., & de Kleer, J. (1982). Knowledge engineering and pedagogical techniques in SOPHIE I, II, and HI. In D. Sleeman & J. S. Brown (Eds.), Intelligent tutoring system. (pp. 227–282). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. S., Collins, A., & Duguid, P. (1989). Situated cognition and the culture of learning. Educational Researcher, 18(1., 32–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, P. J. (1990). Assessing the quality of hypertext documents. In A. Rizk, N. Streite, & J. André (Eds.), Hypertext: Concepts, systems, and application. (pp. 1 – 12). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (1986). Cognitive science and science education. American Psychologist, 4., 1123–1130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carmel, E., Crawford, S. F., & Chen, H. (1992). Browsing in hypertext: A cognitive study. IEEE-Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics, 2.(5), 865–884.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chafe, W. L. (1979). The flow of thought and the flow of language. In T. Givón (Ed.), Syntax and semantics, Vol. XII: Discourse and synta. (pp. 159–181). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H., Glaser, R., & Rees, E. (1982). Expertise in problem solving. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligenc. (Vol. I, pp. 7–75). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conklin, J. (1987). Hypertext: An introduction and survey. IEEE Computer, 2.(9), 17–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Young, L. (1990). Linking considered harmful. In A. Rizk, N. Streitz, & J. André (Eds.), Hypertext: Concepts, systems, and application. (pp. 238–249). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • diSessa, A. (1983). Phenomenology and the evolution of intuition. In D. Gentner & A. L. Stevens (Eds.), Mental model. (pp. 15–33). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Furnas, G. W. (1986, April). Generalized fisheye views. CHI Proceeding., 16–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garner, R., & Reis, R. (1981). Monitoring and resolving comprehension obstacles: An investigation of spontaneous text lookbacks among upper-grade good and poor comprehenders. Reading Research Quarterly, 2., 569–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garrod, S. C. (1985). Incremental pragmatic interpretation versus occasional inferencing during fluent reading. In G. Rickheit & H. Strohner (Eds.), Inferences in text processin. (pp. 161–181). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., Wilkinson, A. C., & Epstein, W. (1982). The illusion of knowing: Failure in the self-assessment of comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 1., 597–602.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gloor, P. A., & Streitz, N. A. (Eds.). (1990). Hypertext und hypermedia. Berlin: Springer.

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, S. H. (1990). Using protocol analyses and drawings to study mental model construction during hypertext navigation. International Journal of Human Computer Interactio., (4), 359–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, J. T., & Mosenthal, P. (1987). Literacy as multidimensional: Locating information and reading comprehension. Educational Psychologis., 2.(3&4), 279–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, J. T., Britten, T., & Barker, K. G. (1991, April). Effects of document and strategy type on cognitive components on search. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association Convention, Boston, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hacker, W. (1986). Arbeitspsychologie. Psychische Regulation von Arbeitstätigkeiten. Berlin: VEB Deutscher Verlag der Wissenschaften.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models. Towards a cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1987). The comprehension of discourse and mental models. In M. Nagao (Ed.), Language and artificial intelligenc. (pp. 253–261). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D. H., & Grabinger, R. S. (1990). Problems and issues in designing hypertext/hypermedia for learning. In D. H. Jonassen & H. Mandl (Eds.), Designing hypermedia for learnin. (pp. 3–25). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D. H., & Mandl, H. (Eds.). (1990). Designing hypermedia for learning. NATO A51 Series F, Vol. 67. Berlin: Springer.

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D. H., & Wang, S. (1993). Acquiring structural knowledge from semantically structured hypertext. Journal of Computer-Based Instructio., 2.(1), 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kobasigawa, A. (1993). Children’s retrieval skills for school learning. The Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 2., 259–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1981). The medium and the message in mental imagery: A theory. Psychological Review, 8., 46–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucarella, D. (1990). A model for hypertext-based information retrieval. In A. Rizk, N. Streitz, & J. André (Eds.), Hypertext: Concepts, systems, and application. (pp. 81–94). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marton, F., & Säljö, R. (1976). On qualitative differences in learning: II. Outcome as a function of the learner’s conception of the task. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 4., 115–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E. (1989). Models for understanding. Review of Educational Researc., 5.(1), 43–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McAleese, R. (Ed.). (1989). Hypertext: Theory into practice. Worcester, England: Billing & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • McAleese, R., & Green, C. (Eds.). (1990). Hypertext: State of the art. Oxford, England: Intellect.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGrath, D. (1992). Hypertext, CAI, paper or program control: Do learners benefit from choices? Journal of Research on Computing in Education, 2.(4), 513–532.

    Google Scholar 

  • Melton, R. F. (1978). Resolution of conflicting claims concerning the effect of behavioral objectives on student learning. Review of Educational Research, 4., 291–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, B. J. F. (1975). The organization of prose and its effects on memory. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mohageg, M. F. (1992). The influence of hypertext linking structures on the efficacy of information retrieval. Human-factors, 3.(3), 351–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morrow, D. G., Greenspan, S. L., & Bower, G. H. (1987). Accessibility and situation models in narrative comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 2., 165–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Donnell, A. (1993). Searching for information in knowledge maps and texts. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 1.(2), 222–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, S. E. (1978). Fundamental aspects of cognitive representation. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorizatio. (pp. 259–303). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pichert, J. W., & Anderson, R. C. (1977). Taking different perspectives on a story. Journal of Educational Psychology, 6., 309–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1990). A theory of graph comprehension. In R. Freedle (Ed.), Artificial intelligence and the future of testin. (pp. 73–126). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizk, A., Streitz, N., & André, J. (Eds.). (1990). Hypertext: Concepts, systems, and. applications. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Samarapungavan, A., & Beishuizen, J. (1992). Hypermedia and knowledge acquisition from non-linear expository text. In B. van Hout-Wolters & W. Schnotz (Eds.), Text comprehension and learning from tex. (pp. 53–69). Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saxer, K. H., & Gloor, P. A. (1990). Navigation im Hyperraum: Fisheye views in HyperCard. In P. A. Gloor & N. A. Streitz (Eds.), Hypertext und Hypermedi. (pp. 190–204). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. B., & Weiss, S. (1988). Hypertext. Communications of the ACM., 3.(1), 816–819.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spiro, R. J., Feltovich, R. L., Jacobson, M. J., & Coulson, R. L. (1991). Cognitive flexibility, constructivisM., and hypertext: Random access instruction for advanced knowledge acquisition in ill-structured domains. Educational Technology, 3.(5), 24–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Dijk, T. A., & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of discourse comprehension. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1990). Across-cultural investigation of children’s conceptions about the earth, the sun, and the moon: Greek and American data. In H. Mandl, E. de Corte, S. N. Bennett, & H. F. Friedrich (Eds.), Learning & instruction: European research in an international contex., Vol 11:2 Analysis of complex skills and complex knowledge domain. (pp. 605–629). Oxford, England: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, B. Y., & Frederiksen, J. R. (1990). Causal model progressions as a foundation for intelligent learning environments. Artificial Intelligence, 4., 99–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yankelovich, N., Landow, G., & Cody, D. (1987). Creating hypermedia materials for English literature students. ACM SIGCUE Outlook, 19., 12–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1997 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Schnotz, W. (1997). Strategy-Specific Information Access in Knowledge Acquisition from Hypertext. In: Resnick, L.B., Säljö, R., Pontecorvo, C., Burge, B. (eds) Discourse, Tools and Reasoning. NATO ASI Series, vol 160. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-03362-3_15

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-03362-3_15

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-642-08337-2

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-662-03362-3

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics