Skip to main content

Psychology Instead of Ethics? Why Psychological Research Is Important but Cannot Replace Ethics

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Dual-Process Theories in Moral Psychology

Abstract

Recent research in cognitive and moral psychology suggests that our judgments and decisions are primarily driven by intuitions and that giving reasons is a matter of posthoc rationalization or even confabulation – thus challenging the ethical self-conception held by common sense and many philosophers. Do these empirical findings prompt us to abandon the belief that we act and decide on the basis of reasons? I will point to our everyday practice and use the heuristic approach of decision making to argue that they do not. We have, at least, two good reasons to answer the question ‘Psychology instead of Ethics?´ in the negative. The heuristic approach is not only compatible with a rationalist position; it specifies the underlying rules of moral judgments. Combining my arguments from common sense reasoning with the heuristic approach allows me to reinterpret the empirical findings as being about application and systematic errors (bias) of otherwise adaptive heuristics. My proposal for a reconciliation of the philosophical and psychological position will lead to the conclusion that normative and psychological questions are mutually dependent. While not sufficient on their own, both positions are necessary for an informed picture about our reasoning abilities as well as for our ethical self-conception.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Blair, R.J.R. (1995). A cognitive developmental approach to morality: investigating the psychopath. Cognition 57, 1–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. (1997). Moral reasoning and the child with psychopathic tendencies. Personality and Individual Differences 22, 731–739.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandom, R. (1994). Making it explicit. Reasoning, representing, and discursive commitment. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • van den Bos, W., & Güroglu, B. (2009). The role of ventral medial prefrontal cortex in social decision making. The Journal of Neuroscience 29(24), 7631–7632.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, P.W., Holygak, K.J., Nisbett, R. E., & Oliver, M. (1986). Pragmatic versus syntactic approaches to training deductive reasoning. Cognitive Psychology 18, 293–328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darley J.M., & Batson, C.D. (1973). From Jerusalem to Jerichow: A Study of situational and dispositional variables in helping behavior. Journal of personality and Social Psychology 27(1), 100–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, D. (1980). Essays on actions and events. Oxford Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doris, J. M. (1998). Lack of character. Personality and moral behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eskine, K.J., Kacinik, N.A., & Prinz, J.J. (2011). A bad taste in the mouth: Gustatory disgust influences moral judgment. Psychological Science 22(3), 295–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, M. S., & LeDoux, J. E. (1978). The integrated mind. New York: Plenum Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G. (2008). Moral intuitions= Fast and frugal heuristics? In W. Sinnott-Armstrong (Ed.), Moral psychology. Volume 2: The cognitive science of morality: Intuition and diversity (pp. 1–26). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J.D., Sommerville, R.B., Nystrom, L.E., Darley, J.M., & Cohen, J.D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science 293, 2105–2108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D. (2007). Why are VMPFC patients more utilitarian?: A dual-process theory of moral judgment explains. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11(8), 322–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Reviews 108(4), 814–834.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harenski, C.L., & Hamann, S. (2005). Neural correlates of regulating negative emotions related to moral violations. Neuroimage 30, 313–324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heekeren, H.R., Wartenburger, I., Schmidt, H., Schwintowski, H.-P., & Villringer, A. (2003). An FMRI study of simple ethical decision making. Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuropsychology 14(9), 1215–1219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horberg, E.J., Oveis, C., Keltner, D., & Cohan, A.B. (2009). Disgust and the moralization of purity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 97(6), 963–976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horberg, E.J., Oveis, C., & Keltner, D. (2011). Emotions as moral amplifiers: An appraisal tendency approach to the influences of distinct emotions upon moral judgment. Emotion Review 3(3), 237–244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huebner, B., Dwyer, S., & Hauser, M. (2008). The role of emotion in moral psychology. Trends in Cognitive Science 13(1), 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D., & Bloom, P. (2012). Disgusting smells cause decreased liking of gay men. Emotion 12(1), 1–5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones A., & Fitness, J. (2008). Moral hypervigilance: The influence of disgust sensitivity in the moral domain. Emotion 8(5), 613–627.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joyce, R. (2008). What neuroscience can (and cannot) contribute to metaethics. In W. Sinnott-Armstrong (Ed.), Moral Psychology Volume 3: The Neuroscience of Morality: Emotion, Brain Disorders, and Development (pp. 371–394). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2003). Maps of bounded rationality: Psychology for behavioral economics. The American Economic Review 93(5), 1449–1475.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ladwig, B. (2003). Autonomie als Antwortfähigkeit. Unpublished conference paper, talk on the 5th GAP Conference in Bielefeld: http://www.gap5.de/proceedings/pdf/547-559_ladwig.pdf. Accessed 31 Jan 2014.

  • Larrick, R. P. (2004). Debiasing. In D.J. Koehler & N. Harvey (Eds.), Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making (pp. 316–33). Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazar, N., & Zhong, C.-B. (2010). Do green products make us better people? Psychological Science 21(4), 494–498.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mercier, H., & Sperber, D. (2011). Why do humans reason? Arguments for an argumentative theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34, 57–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moll, J., Oliveira-Souza, R., Esliner, P.J., Bramati, I. E., Mourao-Miranda, J., Andreiuolo, P. A., & Pessoa, L. (2002a). The neural correlates of moral sensitivity. A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of basic and moral emotions. The Journal of Neuroscience 22(7), 2730–2736.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moll, J., Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I.E., & Grafman, J. (2002b). Functional networks in emotional moral and nonmoral social judgments. NeuroImage 16, 696–703.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mussweiler, T., Strack, F., & Pfeiffer, T. (2000). Overcoming the inevitable anchoring effect: Considering the opposite compensates for selective accessibility. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 26(9), 1142–1150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newstead, S. (2001) Introspection-A new look? The Psychologist 14(1), 34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R.E, & Wilson, T. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. In Psychological Review 84(3), 231–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, J. W., & Bettman, J.R. (2004). Walking with the scarecrow: The information-processing approach to decision research. In D.J. Koehler & N. Harvey (Eds.), Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making (pp. 110–132). Oxford, U.K.: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, J. (2006). The emotional basis of moral judgment. Philosophical Explorations 9(1), 29–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pronin, E., Wegner, D.M., McCarthy, K., & Rodriguez, S. (2006). Everyday magical powers: The role of apparent mental causation in the overestimation of personal influence. Journal of personality and Social Psychology 91(2), 218–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schnall, S., Benton, J., & Harvey, S. (2008a). With a clean conscience. Cleanliness reduces the severity of moral judgments. Psychological Science 19(12), 1219–1222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schnall, S., Haidt, J., & Jordan, A.H. (2008b). Disgust as embodied moral emotion. Personaity and Social Psychology Bulletin 34(8), 1096–1109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. (2000). Mind, language and society: Philosophy in the real world. London: Phoenix.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sellars, W. (1997). Empiricism and the philosophy of mind (ed. by Brandom, R.) Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sie, M., & Wouters, A. (2010). The BCN challenge to compatibilist free will and personal responsibility. Neuroethics 3(2), 121–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sommer, M., Rothmayr, C., Döhnel, K., Meinhardt, J., Schwerdtner, J., Sodian, B., & Hajak, G. (2010). How should I decide? The neural correlates of everyday moral reasoning. Neuropsychologia 48, 2018–2016.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sunstein, C. (2005). Moral heuristics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (4), 531–573.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science 185(4157), 1124–1131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valdesolo P., & de Steno, D. (2006). Manipulations of emotional context shape moral judgment. Psychological Science 17(6), 476–477.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waldmann, M., Nagel, J., & Wiegmann, A. (2012). Moral judgment. In K.J. Holyoak & R.G. Morrison (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning (pp. 364–389). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D.M., & Wheatly, T. (1999). Apparent mental causation: Sources of the experience of will. American Psychologist 54, 480–491.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheatly, T., & Haidt, J. (2005). Hypnotic disgust makes moral judgments more severe. Psychological Science 16(10), 780–784.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T., & Brekke, N. (1994). Mental contamination and mental correction: Unwanted influences on judgments and evaluations. Psychological Bulletin 116 (1), 117–142.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Janett Triskiel .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer Fachmedien Wiesbaden

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Triskiel, J. (2016). Psychology Instead of Ethics? Why Psychological Research Is Important but Cannot Replace Ethics. In: Brand, C. (eds) Dual-Process Theories in Moral Psychology. Springer VS, Wiesbaden. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-658-12053-5_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics