Skip to main content

Implicit and Explicit Attitudes toward Abrahamic Religions. Comparison of Direct and Indirect Assessment

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Xenosophia and Religion. Biographical and Statistical Paths for a Culture of Welcome

Abstract

In this chapter, we consider the problem that many measures of religiosity and prejudice are at risk to be affected by socially desirable responding. Indirect measurement procedures such as the Implicit Association Test (IAT) provide an opportunity to assess also pre-reflective, impulsive parts of attitudes avoiding the risk of socially desirable responding. Hence, three Single-Category IATs (SC-IATs) have been used in the Bielefeld Study on Xenosophia and Religion in order to assess implicit attitudes toward the three Abrahamic religions Judaism, Christianity, and Islam among a subsample of n = 272 persons. Participants on average displayed a slightly positive implicit attitude toward Christianity while Judaism and Islam were evaluated almost completely neutrally. While there are significant implicit-explicit correlations for attitudes toward Christianity and Islam, there is no such association for Judaism; maybe because Judaism has been a less salient category among the predominantly Christian, but partly also Muslim participants. While some measures of religiosity (centrality of religiosity and the religious schemata ttt and ftr) showed to be correlated with social desirability, the religious schema xenos showed no association with social desirable responding, but turned out to be a significant predictor of lower Islamophobic prejudice both on the explicit and implicit level of cognition. Consequences and limitations of the described findings are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 79.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Please note that, from the perspective of religious studies, of course several stimuli could refer to more than one of the three Abrahamic religions: E.g., Moses is a prominent figure not only in the Jewish tradition, but in the other two traditions as well, Jesus is also a recognized prophet in Islam, and, at least in German, the term kosher can also be used to describe the Muslim way of food preparation. But for measures like SC-IATs aiming at assessing impulsive, automaticed reactions, it is primarily important to choose stimuli which are spontaneously associated with a certain target. Hence, they should be familiar, rather short, and stereotypical for the respective target.

  2. 2.

    This is a clear weakness of our study: If we had been able to include a Jewish subgroup in our sample, our analyses would have provided the opportunity to detect group-specific patterns. However, as our review of IAT studies on inter-religious attitudes and prejudice has shown, the focus throughout the last decade has been clearly on the relation between Christians and Muslims (Henry & Hardin, 2006; Park et al., 2007; Rowatt et al., 2005; Wigboldus et al., 2006) while the relation of these two groups to Jews and vice versa has been studied only sparsely (e.g. Rudman et al., 1999). Hence, future studies should lay similar emphasis on implicit attitudes in both directions: attitudes toward Jews and attitudes of Jews toward the other religions.

References

  • Allport, G. W., & Ross, J. M. (1967). Personal religious orientation and prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 432–443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Altemeyer, B., & Hunsberger, B. (1992). Authoritarianism, religious fundamentalism, quest, and prejudice. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 2, 113–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Banse, R., Seise, J., & Zerbes, N. (2001). Implicit attitudes towards homosexuality: Reliability, validity, and controllability of the IAT. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 145–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bassett, R. L., Thrower, J., Barclay, J., Powers, C., Smith, A., Tindall, M., Tiuch, K., & Monroe, J. (2005). One effort to measure implicit attitudes toward spirituality and religion. Journal of Psychology and Christianity, 24, 210–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Batson, C. D., Naifeh, S. J., & Pate, S. (1978). Social desirability, religious orientation, and racial prejudice. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 17, 31–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Batson, C. D., Flink, C. H., Schoenrade, P. A., Fultz, J., & Pych, V. (1986). Religious orientation and overt versus covert racial prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 175–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Batson, C. D., Schoenrade, P., & Ventis, W. L. (1993). Religion and the individual: A social-psychological perspective. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brendl, C. M., Markman, A. B., & Messner, C. (2001). How do indirect measures of evaluation work? Evaluating the inference of prejudice in the implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 760–773.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burris, C. T., & Navara, G. S. (2002). Morality play—or playing morality? Intrinsic religious orientation and socially desirable responding. Self and Identity, 1, 67–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R. B., & Warburton, F. W. (1967). Objective personality and motivation tests. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clobert, M., Saroglou, V., Kwang-Kuo, H., & Soong, W. L. (2014). East Asian religious tolerance—a myth or a reality? Empirical investigations of religious prejudice in East Asian societies. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45, 1515–1533.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clobert, M., Saroglou, V., & Kwang-Kuo, H. (2015). Buddhist concepts as implicitly reducing prejudice and increasing prosociality. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41, 513–525.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 349–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J. (2002). The implicit association test as a tool for studying dysfunctional associations in psychopathology: Strengths and limitations. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 33, 115–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (1986). Prejudice, discrimination, and racism: Historical trends and contemporary approaches. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination, and racism (pp. 1–34). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duck, R. J., & Hunsberger, B. (1999). Religious orientation and prejudice: The role of religious proscription, right-wing authoritarianism, and social desirability. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 9, 157–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duriez, B. (2004). A research note on the relation between religiosity and racism: The importance of the way in which religious contents are being processed. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 14, 177–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1975). Manual of the Eysenck personality questionnaire (adult and junior). London: Hodder and Stoughton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and uses. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gillings, V., & Joseph, S. (1996). Religiosity and social desirability: Impression management and self-deceptive positivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 1047–1050.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Poehlmann, A. T., Uhlmann, E. L., & Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and using the implicit association test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 17–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gschwendner, T., Hofmann, W., & Schmitt, M. (2006). Synergistic moderator effects of situation and person factors of awareness and adjustment on the consistency of implicit and explicit attitudes. Journal of Individual Differences, 27, 47–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henry, P. J., & Hardin, C. D. (2006). The contact hypothesis revisited. Status bias in the reduction of implicit prejudice in the United States and Lebanon. Psychological Science, 17, 862–868.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hills, P., Francis, L. J., Argyle, M., & Jackson, C. J. (2004). Primary personality trait correlates of religious practice and orientation. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 61–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Himmelfarb, S., & Lickteig, C. (1982). Social desirability and the randomized response technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 710–717.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., Le, H., & Schmitt, M. (2005). A meta-analysis on the correlation between the implicit association test and explicit self-report measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1369–1385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holtgraves, T. (2004). Social desirability and self-reports: Testing models of socially desirable responding. Personality and Social Psychological Bulletin, 30, 161–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huber, S., & Huber, O. W. (2012). The centrality of religiosity scale (CRS). Religions, 3, 710–724.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hundleby, J., Pawlik, K., & Cattell, R. B. (1965). Personality factors in objective test devices. San Diego: Knapp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunsberger, B., & Platonow, E. (1986). Religion and helping charitable causes. Journal of Psychology, 120, 517–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E., & Sigall, H. (1971). The bogus pipeline: A new paradigm for measuring affect and attitude. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 349–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karpinski, A., & Steinman, R. B. (2006). The single category implicit association test as a measure of implicit social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 16–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, C., Hood, R. W., Silver, C. F., Keller, B., & Streib, H. (2016). Is “spirituality” nothing but “religion”? An indirect measurement approach. In H. Streib & R. W. Hood (Eds.), Semantics and psychology of spirituality. A cross-cultural analysis (pp. 71–85). Cham: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • LaBouff, J. P., Rowatt, W. C., Johnson, M. K., Thedford, M., & Tsang, J. A. (2010). Development and initial validation of an implicit measure of religiousness-spirituality. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 49, 439–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leak, G. K., & Fish, S. B. (1989). Religious orientation, impression management, and self-deception: Toward a clarification of the link between religion and social desirability. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 28, 355–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, C. A. (1999). Is the relationship between religiosity and personality “contaminated” by social desirability as assessed by the lie scale? A methodological reply to Michael W. Eysenck (1998). Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 2, 105–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maltby, J. (1999). Religious orientation and Eysenck’s personality dimensions: The use of the amended religious orientation scale to examine the relationship between religiosity, psychoticism, neuroticism and extraversion. Personality and Individual Differences, 26, 79–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meier, B. P., Hauser, D. J., Robinson, M. D., Friesen, C. K., & Schjeldahl, K. (2007). What’s “up” with God? Vertical space as a representation of the divine. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 699–710.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, R. J., Hood, R. W., & Watson, P. J. (1989). A second look at religious orientation, social desirability, and prejudice. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 27, 81–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Park, J., Felix, K., & Lee, G. (2007). Implicit attitudes toward Arab-Muslims and the moderating effects of social information. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 29, 35–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paulhus, D. L. (1984). Two-component models of socially desirable responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 598–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paulhus, D. L. (1998). The Paulhus deception scales: BIDR version 7. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paulhus, D. L. (2002). Socially desirable responding: The evolution of a construct. In H. Braun, D. N. Jackson, & D. E. Wiley (Eds.), The role of constructs in psychological and educational measurement (pp. 67–88). Hillsdale/NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pearson, P. R., & Francis, L. J. (1989). The dual nature of the Eysenckian lie scales: Are religious adolescents more truthful? Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 1041–1048.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pettigrew, T. F., & Meertens, R. W. (1995). Subtle and blatant prejudice in Western Europe. European Journal of Social Psychology, 25, 57–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, W. M. (1982). Development of reliable and valid short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38, 119–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, D. T., Gibson-Beverly, G., & Schwartz, J. P. (2004). Sorority and fraternity membership and religious behaviors: Relation to gender attitudes. Sex Roles, 50, 871–877.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roese, N. J., & Jamieson, D. W. (1993). Twenty years of bogus pipeline research: A critical review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 363–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowatt, W. C., & Franklin, L. M. (2004). Christian orthodoxy, religious fundamentalism, and right-wing authoritarianism as predictors of implicit racial prejudice. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 14, 125–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowatt, W. C., & Kirkpatrick, L. A. (2002). Two dimensions of attachment to God and their relation to affect, religiosity, and personality constructs. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 41, 637–651.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowatt, W. C., & Schmitt, D. P. (2003). Associations between religious orientation and varieties of sexual experience. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 42, 455–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowatt, W. C., Franklin, L. M., & Cotton, M. (2005). Patterns and personality correlates of implicit and explicit attitudes toward Christians and Muslims. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 44, 29–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowatt, W. C., Tsang, J. A., Kelly, J., LaMartina, B., McCullers, M., & McKinley, A. (2006). Associations between religious personality dimensions and implicit homosexual prejudice. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 45, 397–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rudman, L. A., Greenwald, A. G., Mellott, D. S., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1999). Measuring the automatic components of prejudice: Flexibility and generality of the implicit association test. Social Cognition, 17, 437–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saroglou, V., & Galand, P. (2004). Identities, values, and religion: A study among Muslim, other immigrant, and native Belgian young adults after the 9/11 attacks. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 4, 97–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schnell, R., Hill, P. B., & Esser, E. (1999). Methoden der empirischen Sozialforschung [Methods of empirical socio-scientific research]. München: Oldenbourg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S. H. (2006). Basic human values: An overview. http://segr-did2.fmag.unict.it/allegati/convegno%207-8-10-05/schwartzpaper.pdf.

  • Schwartz, S. H. (2012). An overview of the Schwartz theory of basic values. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 2. https://doi.org/10.9707/2307-0919.1116.

  • Sedikides, C., & Gebauer, J. E. (2010). Religiosity as self-enhancement: A meta-analysis of the relation between socially desirable responding and religiosity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, 17–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shariff, A. F., Cohen, A. B., & Norenzayan, A. (2008). The Devil’s advocate: Secular arguments diminish both implicit and explicit religious belief. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 8, 417–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shen, M. J., Yelderman, L. A., Haggard, M. C., & Rowatt, W. C. (2013). Disentangling the belief in God and cognitive rigidity/flexibility components of religiosity to predict racial and value-violating prejudice: A post-critical belief scale analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 54, 389–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. R., & DeCoster, J. (2000). Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology: Conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 108–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spilka, B., Kojetin, B., & McIntosh, D. (1985). Forms and measures of personal faith: Questions, correlates and distnctions. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 24, 437–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stocké, V. (2003). Deutsche Kurzskala zur Erfassung des Bedürfnisses nach sozialer Anerkennung [Brief German scale for the assessment of the need for social recognition]. In A. Glöckner-Rist (Ed.), ZUMA-Informationssystem. Elektronisches Handbuch sozialwissenschaftlicher Erhebungsinstrumente. Mannheim: Zentrum für Umfragen, Methoden und Analysen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stocké, V. (2004). Entstehungsbedingungen von Antwortverzerrungen durch soziale Erwünschtheit: Ein Vergleich der Prognosen der Rational-Choice-Theorie und des Modells der Frame-Selektion [Conditions of the occurence of biased responses due to social desirability: A comparison of the prognoses of rational choice theory and of the model of frame selection]. Zeitschrift für Soziologie, 33, 303–320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 220–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Streib, H., & Gennerich, C. (2011). Jugend und Religion: Bestandsaufnahmen, Analysen und Fallstudien zur Religiosität Jugendlicher [Youth and religion: State of research, analyses, and case studies about the religiosity of adolescents]. Juventa: Weinheim.

    Google Scholar 

  • Streib, H., & Klein, C. (2013). Atheists, agnostics, and apostates. In K. I. Pargament, J. J. Exline, & J. W. Jones (Eds.), APA handbooks in psychology: APA handbook of psychology, religion, and spirituality (Vol. 1, pp. 713–728). Washington: APA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Streib, H., Hood, R. W., & Klein, C. (2010). The religious schema scale: Construction and initial validation of a quantitative measure for religious styles. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 20, 151–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, J. B. (1961). What do attitude scales measure: The problem of social desirability. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62, 386–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trimble, D. E. (1997). The religious orientation scale: Review and meta-analysis of social desirability effects. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 57, 970–986.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsang, J. A., & Rowatt, W. C. (2007). The relationship between religious orientation, right-wing authoritarianism, and implicit sexual prejudice. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 17, 99–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ventis, W. L., Ball, C. T., & Viggiano, C. (2010). A Christian humanist implicit association test: Validity and test-retest reliability. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 2, 181–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Viswesvaran, C. (1999). Meta-analyses of fakability estimates: Implications for personality measurement. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 59, 197–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, P. J., Hood, R. W., & Morris, R. J. (1985). Dimensions of religiosity and empathy. Journal of Psychology and Christianity, 4, 73–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, P. J., Morris, R. J., Foster, J. E., & Hood, R. W. (1986). Religiosity and social desirability. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 25, 215–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wenger, J. L., & Yarbrough, T. D. (2005). Religious individuals: Evaluating their intrinsic and extrinsic motivations at the implicit level of awareness. The Journal of Social Psychology, 145, 5–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitley, B. E., & Kite, M. E. (2010). The psychology of prejudice and discrimination. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wigboldus, D. H. J., Holland, R. W., & Van Knippenberg, A. (2006). Single target implicit associations. Radboud University Nijmegen: Unpublished manuscript.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiggins, J. S. (1959). Interrelations among MMPI measures of dissimulation under standard and social desirability instructions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 23, 419–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Constantin Klein .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Klein, C., Bullik, R., Streib, H. (2018). Implicit and Explicit Attitudes toward Abrahamic Religions. Comparison of Direct and Indirect Assessment. In: Streib, H., Klein, C. (eds) Xenosophia and Religion. Biographical and Statistical Paths for a Culture of Welcome. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-74564-0_8

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-74564-0_8

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-74563-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-74564-0

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics