Skip to main content

Meta-Morphosis: Kinesis and Semiosis in Language Concerning a Theory of Enunciation

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Diagrams and Gestures

Part of the book series: Lecture Notes in Morphogenesis ((LECTMORPH))

  • 177 Accesses

Abstract

My observations in this article will be restricted to the theory of enunciation developed by Antoine Culioli (1924–2018), one of the most eminent figures in French linguistics, whose thinking has not acquired the international renown due to it—this despite the fact that English-speaking linguists, as well as those of other languages, have entered into dialogue with Harris, Chomsky, Langacker and Talmy, to name but a few.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    See Culioli’s publications listed in the bibliography. Also of note are works in English, Cognition and representation in linguistic theory, edited by Michel Liddle (John Benjamins Publishing Company, Amsterdam/Philadelphia 1995); and in Italian, L’arco e la freccia: scritti scelti, translated and edited by Francesco La Mantia with a preface by Tullio di Mauro (Bologna, Il Mulino, 2014).

  2. 2.

    See Jean Ladrière, “Système, épistémologie”, Encyclopoedia Universalis. http://www.universalis-edu.com/encyclopedie/systeme-epistemologie/ (accessed 7 May 2015).

  3. 3.

    “We can therefore say that, with texts, we are dealing with (mental) representations that are fixed (materialized and stabilized) through the intermediary of signs, for which we shall provide a meta-representation, so that we may manipulate them—manipulate them with a view to searching for consistencies and rules of good formation, in order to arrive at a calculation” [19: 12].

  4. 4.

    It may be useful to refer to the Lexique élémentaire de la Théorie des Opérations Prédicatives et Énonciatives d’Antoine Culioli compiled by de la Mantia [36].

  5. 5.

    Epilinguistics, which I am renaming epilanguage, refers to an unconscious, permanent activity, corresponding to a process in which forms are generated by variation-selection, in a similar way (but for language) to the epigenetic theories described by Courrèges, Changeux and Danchin (see Jean-Pierre Changeux and Antoine Danchin, “Apprendre par stabilisation sélective de synapses en cours de développement”, L’unité de l’homme 2. Le cerveau humain, Seuil, Paris, 1974). The neuronal model, known as the Darwinian model, of epigenesis through selective stabilization, has been extended, in conjectural fashion, to mental activity in general; it is based on a dynamic and interactive pattern of variation-selection, with a diversity generator and a selection system determined by the environment.

  6. 6.

    The article first appeared in Cahiers pour l’analyse, 9, 1968, Paris, Seuil.

  7. 7.

    See La Mantia [36, 35].

  8. 8.

    Sigmund Freud, “Remarques sur un cas de névrose obsessionnelle (L’homme aux rats)” [Notes upon a case of obsessional neurosis], Cinq psychanalyses (Paris: P.U.F., 1954) pp. 199–261.

  9. 9.

    My examples are in the spirit of those analysed by Culioli but are not reproduced literally.

  10. 10.

    “The word is accepted, for example, to describe the area within the atom where its charge is concentrated, and within a cell, the area where its genetic material is concentrated and preserved: always in accordance with a demand for invariance, each time specific and locatable according to appropriate procedures. In the language of algebraists, a kernel acquires the meaning of invariant” (Desanti 1999: 138–140).

  11. 11.

    My hypothesis may be compared with that of Per Aage Brandt on the dual status, semiotic and cognitive, of the diagram: “The meaning of the diagram consists, so to speak, in a mental graph that is identical—at least partially—to the graph that we occasionally draw or sketch physically. It might therefore be said that the diagram is an indicator of the cognitive mechanism, which schematizes graphically while a thought is developing” [7]. Brandt’s perspective is semiotic, while Culioli’s work belongs to formal and empirical linguistics.

  12. 12.

    “Autour d’un objet” (unpublished), TOPE seminar, 17 March 2003.

  13. 13.

    Gilles Châtelet quotes this “axiom” of Hegel [Wissenschaft der Logik]: “Negativity is the rotation point (Wendungspunkt) of the movement of the concept” (op. cit., p. 30).

  14. 14.

    Oral seminar, 22/03/2006.

  15. 15.

    I would point out, in relation to this, and echoing what has been said of the role of schematic form, that the history of science reports cases of mathematical prefiguration, visual figures giving access, after the event, to mathematical realities that were unknown at the time when these graphic productions appeared in texts (see Peiffer, already cited, also [39]).

  16. 16.

    Culioli was then alluding to Taoist philosophy and to the idea that artists seek to grasp the intimate form of their subject, its internal structuring principle (the li in Chinese).

  17. 17.

    Idem, p. 30. [English edition, Introduction, p. 8.].

  18. 18.

    Oral seminar at the École Normale Supérieure, Paris.

  19. 19.

    The work of Charles Malamoud focuses mainly on Vedic ritual, based on his reading of the relevant philological and anthropological treatises. He attended courses given by Émile Benveniste from 1956 to 1962 and was a regular participant in Culioli’s seminars.

  20. 20.

    These two references are taken up and commented upon in a 2012 interview [24].

  21. 21.

    The author continues as follows: “In a more general sense, the texts are an update of the elementary and fundamental questions suggested to, or rather imposed upon, the authors by their self-attributed task of providing an explanation for ritual acts: What does it mean to begin? What does it mean to proceed to the next step? How is one to understand that the same act can be both single and multiple? What is meant by ‘too much’ and ‘not enough’? What is the relationship between parts and wholes? What does it mean to measure? How are repetition and difference to be understood? How does one tell creative reiteration apart from harmful redundancy? […] It is tempting to think that Vedic theoreticians considered rites to be the systems of acts where such concepts operated with the greatest clarity and purity and that it was for this reason that they devoted so much ingenuity and pertinacity to their analysis and glorification” [41, 180].

  22. 22.

    Gustave Guillaume, a French linguist who had a highly unusual career, at the margins of university institutions. He was the founder of “psychomecanics”, whose purpose was to analyse the operations at the root of linguistic signs—from language in its virtual state to the effectiveness of speech—in terms of cinetisms. A trend developed around his body of work, which was immense, although only a small part has been published. There are very few followers of Guillaume’s theory today but, recently, there has been a resurgence of interest in it, from a cognitive and philosophical point of view.

  23. 23.

    “A symbol incorporates a habit, and is indispensable to the application to any intellectual habit, at least. Moreover, Symbols offer the means of thinking about thoughts in ways in which we could not otherwise think of them” [43: 414].

  24. 24.

    “And in the case of the concessive, it is precisely a kind of counter to this: you do everything that you can, you make all the efforts you want, the fact remains that it does not prevent it coming to nothing; whereas, normally, your efforts should lead you to” [16: 177].

  25. 25.

    Some of these are presented in Ducard 2020.

  26. 26.

    Lawrence Barsalou is Professor of Psychology at the University of Glasgow, carrying out research at the Institute of Neuroscience and Psychology.

  27. 27.

    “From this perspective, a category is typically simulated in diverse situations. Depending on the situation currently relevant, a different situated simulation is produced. […] One proposal is that the brain is a situation-processing architecture whose primary function is to capture and later simulate situated conceptualizations” [6: 15–16].

  28. 28.

    Barsalou’s model is along similar lines to one of the hypotheses put forward by Per Aage Brandt and Ulf Cronquist “That diagrammatic signs are the active components of the abstract mental figuration in working memory that accompany perception and deliver the schematizing ideas which invest and connect our categories and construe proper descriptive, narrative patterns of inferential thought and programs of sensorimotor and social action” [8, abstract].

  29. 29.

    In his theoretical model of brain functioning, the neurobiologist Edelman [30, 31] distinguishes primary consciousness, involving a “scene” in response to objects and events and connected by memory to a former experience associated with values and awareness of a superior order. This implies semantic aptitudes, with which certain primates are endowed, enabling a symbolic activity of representation to take place, made possible by language, and thus creating the potential for a consciousness of consciousness.

  30. 30.

    Let us remind ourselves of this simple definition of the image in mathematics: “In mathematics, we say that y is the image of x through the function f if y = f(x). By extension, the set of y elements for which there is an antecedent in E are called the image of a part E through an f function. For each y of the image set we can find an element x of the definition set, such as y = f(x)”. (https://www.techno-science.net/definition/10532.html).

  31. 31.

    “In mathematics, given two non-empty sets E, F and a map, we call any element x of E such as f(x) = y the antecedent (by f) of an element y of F”. (https://www.techno-science.net/definition/10523.html).

References

  1. Alunni, C.: Des Enjeux du mobile à L’Enchantement du virtuel–et retour. In: Alunni, C., Paoletti, C. (éds.) Gilles Châtelet, L’enchantement du virtuel. Mathématiques, physique, philosophie. Éditions Rue d’Ulm (2010)

    Google Scholar 

  2. Armstrong, D.F., Stokoe, W.C., Wilcox, S.E.: Gesture and the Nature of Language. Cambridge University Press (1995)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  3. Bachelard, G.: La formation de l’esprit scientifique, Paris, Vrin (1970)

    Google Scholar 

  4. Bachelard, G.: The Formation of the Scientific Mind. Introduced, translated and annotated by Jones, M.M. Clinamen Press, Manchester (2002)

    Google Scholar 

  5. Barsalou, L.W., Santos, A., Simmons, W.K., Wilson, C.D.: Language and simulation in conceptual processing. In: De Vega, M., Glenberg, A.M., Graesser, A.C. (eds.) Symbols, Embodiment, and Meaning, pp. 245–283. Oxford University Press, Oxford (2008)

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  6. Barsalou, L.W.: Situated conceptualization: theory and applications. In: Coello, Y., Fischer, M.H. (eds.) Foundations of embodied cognition: perceptual and emotional embodiment. pp. 11–37, Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group (2016)

    Google Scholar 

  7. Brandt, P.A.: La pensée graphique.–Pour une sémiotique des diagrammes. Visible No. 9, Visualisation et mathématisation, Maria Giulia Dondero et Sémir Badir dir., Limoges, PULIM (2012)

    Google Scholar 

  8. Brandt, P.A., Cronquist, U.: Diagrams and mental figuration. A semio-cognitive analysis. Semiotica, N° 229 (2019). En ligne: https://www-degruyter-com.ezproxy.u-pec.fr/view/journals/semi/2019/229/article-p253.xml?tab_body=abstract

  9. Châtelet, G.: Les enjeux du mobile. Mathématiques, physique, philosophie, Paris, coll. Des travaux, Seuil (1993)

    Google Scholar 

  10. Châtelet, G.: Figuring Space: Philosophy, Mathematics and Physics. Translated by Shore, R., Zagha, M. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht, Boston, London (2000)

    Google Scholar 

  11. Corballis, M.C.: From Hand to Mouth, the Origins of Language. Princeton University Press, Princeton and Oxford (2002)

    Google Scholar 

  12. Corballis, M.C.: Evolution of language as gestural system. Marges linguistiques–N°11, mai 2006, M.C.M.S. éditeur, pp. 218–229 (2006). Disponible en ligne: http://www.marges-linguistiques.com

  13. Culioli, A.: Pour une linguistique de l’énonciation. Tours et détours, Tome IV, Limoges, Lambert-Lucas (2018)

    Google Scholar 

  14. Culioli, A.: Pour une théorie des opérations énonciatives, T. 3, HDL, Ophrys (1999b)

    Google Scholar 

  15. Culioli, A.: Nouvelles variations sur la linguistique. In: Vivre le sens, Paris, Seuil, pp. 113–145 (2008)

    Google Scholar 

  16. Culioli, A.: Notes du séminaire de DEA 1983–1984, Université de Poitiers (1985)

    Google Scholar 

  17. Culioli, A.: Variations Linguistiques, Klincksieck, Paris (2002)

    Google Scholar 

  18. Culioli, A.: Gestes mentaux et réseaux symboliques : à la recherche des traces enfouies dans l’entrelacs du langage. In: Faits de langue, n°3, Paris, Ophrys, pp. 7–31 (2011)

    Google Scholar 

  19. Culioli, A.: Pour une théorie des opérations énonciatives, T. 1, HDL, Ophrys (1990)

    Google Scholar 

  20. Culioli, A., Normand, C.: Onze rencontres sur le langage et les langues, HDL, Ophrys (2005)

    Google Scholar 

  21. Culioli, A.: Table ronde. Intervention d’Antoine Culioli. In: Lüdi, G., Zuber C.-A. (éds.) Contributions à l’École d’été de la Société Suisse de Linguistique, Sion, 6–10 Sep. 1993, ARBA 3 (1995). Disponible en ligne: https://emono.unibas.ch/catalog/book/33

  22. Culioli, A.: Rapport sur un rapport. In: Joly, A. (éd.) La psychomécanique et les théories de l’énonciation. Presses Universitaires de Lille, pp. 37–47 (1980)

    Google Scholar 

  23. Culioli, A.: Pour une théorie des opérations énonciatives, T. 2, HDL, Ophrys (1999a)

    Google Scholar 

  24. Culioli A., Ducard D.: Un témoin étonné du langage. In Espace théorique du langage. Des parallèles floues, Cl. Normand, E. Sofia dir., Bruxelles, academia, pp. 129–172 (2012)

    Google Scholar 

  25. Dagognet, F.: Écriture et iconographie, Paris, Librairie philosophique J. Vrin (1973)

    Google Scholar 

  26. Desanti, J.T.: Réflexions sur le temps. Variations philosophiques 1, Paris, Grasset (1992)

    Google Scholar 

  27. Ducard, D., Normand, C. (eds.) Antoine Culioli, un homme dans le langage, Paris, HDL, Ophrys (2006)

    Google Scholar 

  28. Ducard, D.: Le graphe du geste mental dans la théorie énonciative d’A. Culioli. In: Les linguistes et leurs graphiques, Cahiers Parisiens/Parisian Notebooks, vol. 5, pp. 555–576. The University of Chicago Center in Paris (2009)

    Google Scholar 

  29. Ducard, D.: La formalisation dans la théorie des opérations énonciatives: formes, formules, schémas. In: Dossiers d'HEL, SHESL, Écriture(s) et représentations du langage et des langues, vol. 9, pp. 113–122 (2016). Disponible en ligne: http://htl.linguist.univ-paris-diderot.fr/hel/dossiers/numero9ethal-01304863

  30. Edelman, G.M., Tononi G.: A Universe of Consciousness. How Matter Becomes Imagination. Basic Books (2000)

    Google Scholar 

  31. Edelman G.M.: Wider than the Sky. The Phenomenal Gift of Consciousness. Yale University Press (2004)

    Google Scholar 

  32. Imbert, Cl.: Les Stoïciens et leur logique, Paris, Vrin (1978)

    Google Scholar 

  33. Jousse, M.: L’Anthropologie du geste. Gallimard, Paris (1974)

    Google Scholar 

  34. Jousse, M.: L’Anthropologie du geste, II La manducation de la parole, Paris, Gallimard (1975)

    Google Scholar 

  35. La Mantia, F.: Et la structure en came? Notes pour une diagrammatologie énonciative. In: La Mantia, F. (éd.) Pour se faire langage. Lexique de base de la théorie des opérations prédicatives et énonciatives d’Antoine Culioli. Louvain-La Neuve, Éditions Academia (2020)

    Google Scholar 

  36. La Mantia, F.: Pour se faire langage. Lexique de base de la Théorie des Opérations Prédicatives et Énonciatives d’Antoine Culioli, Louvain-La Neuve, Éditions Academia (2020)

    Google Scholar 

  37. La Mantia, F.: From Topology to Quasi-Topology. The complexity of the notional domain. In: La Mantia F., Licata, I., Perconti, P. (eds.) Language in complexity. The emerging meaning. Berlin, Springer (2017)

    Google Scholar 

  38. Langer, S.: Philosophy in a New Key, A Study of the Symbolism of Reason, Rite and Art. Harvard University Press, Cambridge (1942, 1979)

    Google Scholar 

  39. Lenoir, T. (ed.): Inscribing Science. Scientific Texts and the Materiality of Communication. Stanford University Press. (1998)

    Google Scholar 

  40. Les Œuvres de Maître Tchouang, traduction de Jean Levi, L’Encyclopédie des nuisances (2006)

    Google Scholar 

  41. Malamoud, C.: Cooking the World: Ritual and Thought in Ancient India. Translated by David White, Bombay, Calcutta, pp. 169–180. Madras, Delhi, Oxford University Press (1996). “Paths of the Knife: Carving up the Victim in Vedic Sacrifice”

    Google Scholar 

  42. Peirce, C.S.: Prolegomena for an apology to pragmatism. The Monist, vol. 16, no. 4, pp. 492–546. Oxford University Press (Oct. 1906). Published by Oxford University Press: https://www.jstor.org/stable/27899680

  43. Peirce, C.S.: Collected papers of Charles sanders Peirce. In: Hartshorne, C., Weiss, P. (eds.) The simplest mathematics, vol. IV. The Belknaf Press of Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts (1960–67)

    Google Scholar 

  44. Pfeiffer, J.: Rôles des figures dans la production et la transmission des mathématiques. In: Images des mathématiques 2006, Paris, CNRS éd. (2006)

    Google Scholar 

  45. Varela, F.J., Thompson, E., Rosch, E.: The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, USA (1991)

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dominique Ducard .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2023 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Ducard, D. (2023). Meta-Morphosis: Kinesis and Semiosis in Language Concerning a Theory of Enunciation. In: La Mantia, F., Alunni, C., Zalamea, F. (eds) Diagrams and Gestures. Lecture Notes in Morphogenesis. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-29111-1_11

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics