Skip to main content

The Importance and Functions of Theory

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Methodology for Early Childhood Education and Care Research

Part of the book series: SpringerBriefs in Education ((BRIEFSEDUCAT))

  • 209 Accesses

Abstract

In this chapter, the decisiveness of theory for scientific knowledge building is argued, and different functions served by theory are discussed. In light of this discussion, practices such as triangulation and the mixing of theories are criticized. The important distinction between term and concept is made and exemplified with reference to learning theories. The theoretical nature of research questions is exemplified, and the difference between process and product studies is explained. The localization of a study is discussed in terms of the difference between context and contextualization. Ontology and epistemology are introduced through etymology, and examples of their relationship from the research field of early childhood education are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 49.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    It is quite common that references are misplaced. Something that has perhaps become more common, not only in popular-scientific texts but also in scientific fora, is to almost consistently place references at the end of sentences. This often leads to an unfortunate lack of clarity regarding where to locate claims, which in turn serves to hide one’s own contributions behind those of other researchers. For instance, “In this study I have shown how children change their participation in the practice of… when teachers do…” (Name, 2017, 2020). Writing in this way becomes confusing; the sentence starts by stating what the contribution of the present study is, but ends with references to other researchers’ work. Thus, it not only hides the contributions of the present study but also transfers the study’s findings to the work of other researchers.

  2. 2.

    Variation theory (Marton, 2015; Marton & Tsui, 2004) builds on the premises that (a) meaning springs from difference, not similarity; (b) learning can be conceptualized as increased differentiation and reintegration; and (c) in order to facilitate learners’ discernment of something, it has to vary (while other things are kept invariant). To give a classic example, if everything that exists were blue, people would not be able to discern blue (as blue only exists in contrast to other colors); nor would they have a concept of color (as this category presumes more than one color). In order for us to discern something as blue, there also has to be non-blue.

    Cognitive psychology actually exists in two major traditions, the European and the American. The European, which I allude to here, fundamentally builds on the major contributions of Jean Piaget (e.g., 1970) and has an interest in the development of cognition (while the American is the so-called information-processing tradition, in which models of human thinking, memory (e.g., Atkinson et al., 1996), etc. are built on the analogy of the information processor (the computer).

    Cultural-historical theory (Daniels et al., 2007; Luria, 1976; Vygotsky, 1987) (aka sociocultural theory) builds on the founding works of Lev Vygotsky and Alexandr Luria, and subsequent development, with a focus on human development as a process informed by biology, culture, and society. Central to this theory is how human inventions (cultural tools and artefacts) come to mediate people’s perception and thinking.

  3. 3.

    When defining terms conceptually, it is crucial not to include the term itself in the (alleged) definition. For example, do not write “Intersubjectivity is defined as how participants intersubjectively…” or “Learning in this study is defined as learning to…”. If the term to be conceptualized (theoretically specified)—in these examples “intersubjectivity” and “learning”—is included in the definition, the reasoning becomes entirely circular and no definition is provided. To define a concept means to explicate it in other terms.

  4. 4.

    Even though the reference here is to 1978, it should be recognized that this is a collated volume of separate texts that were written by Vygotsky in the 1930s. The particular text cited here is from 1931.

References

  • Atkinson, R. L., Atkinson, R. C., Smith, E. E., Bem, D. J., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1996). Hilgard’s introduction to psychology (12th ed.). Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnhart, R. K. (Ed.). (2004). Chambers dictionary of etymology. Chambers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, G. (2000). Steps to an ecology of mind. University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1972).

    Google Scholar 

  • Beazley, H., Bessell, S., Ennew, J., & Waterson, R. (2009). The right to be properly researched: Research with children in a messy, real world. Children’s Geographies, 7(4), 365–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U. (2005). Making human beings human: Bioecological perspectives on human development. Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1990). Acts of meaning. Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chaiklin, S., & Lave, J. (Eds.). (1996). Understanding practice: Perspectives on activity and context. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. The Belknap Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daniels, H., Cole, M., & Wertsch, J. V. (Eds.). (2007). The Cambridge companion to Vygotsky. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dohn, N. B. (2021). Units of analysis in learning research: Transparency, fit for purpose and purposeful fit. Learning, Culture and Social Interaction, 31, 1–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lcsi.2020.100426

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Einarsdóttir, J. (2007). Research with children: Methodological and ethical challenges. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 15(2), 197–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ekdahl, A.-L. (2021). Different learning possibilities from the same activity—Swedish preschool teachers’ enactment of a number relation activity. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 65(4), 601–614. https://doi.org/10.1080/00313831.2020.1739131

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fleer, M. (2014). A digital turn: Post-developmental methodologies for researching with young children. In M. Fleer & A. Ridgway (Eds.), Visual methodologies and digital tools for researching with young children (International perspectives on early childhood education and development, 10) (pp. 3–11). Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fleer, M., & Ridgway, A. (Eds.). (2014). Visual methodologies and digital tools for researching with young children. Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garvey, C. (1990). Play (enlarged edition). Harvard University Press. (Original work published 1977).

    Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J. G., Collins, A. M., & Resnick, L. B. (1996). Cognition and learning. In D. C. Berliner & R. C. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 15–46). Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanson, N. R. (1981). Patterns of discovery: An inquiry into the conceptual foundations of science. Cambridge University Press. (Original work published 1958)

    Google Scholar 

  • Hedegaard, M. (2008). Developing a dialectic approach to researching children’s development. In M. Hedegaard, M. Fleer, J. Bang, & P. Hviid (Eds.), Studying children: A cultural-historical approach (pp. 30–45). Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hedegaard, M. (2012). Analyzing children’s learning and development in everyday settings from a cultural-historical wholeness approach. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 19, 127–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hedegaard, M., Aronsson, K., Højholt, C., & Skjær Ulvik, O. (Eds.). (2012). Children, childhood, and everyday life: Children’s perspectives. Information Age.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katona, G. (1940). Organizing and memorizing: Studies in the psychology of learning and teaching. Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Köhler, W. (1947). Gestalt psychology. Liveright.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lagerlöf, P. (2015). Musical make-believe playing: Three preschoolers collaboratively initiating play ‘in-between.’ Early Years, 35(3), 303–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Linell, P. (2014). Interactivities, intersubjectivities and language: On dialogism and phenomenology. Language and Dialogue, 4(2), 165–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lloyd, G. E. R. (1992). Polarity and analogy: Two types of argumentation in early Greek thought. Hackett. (Original work published 1966).

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1976). Cognitive development: Its cultural and social foundations. (M. Lopez-Morillas & L. Solotaroff, Trans.). Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marton, F., & Tsui, A. B. M. (Eds.). (2004). Classroom discourse and the space of learning. Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marton, F. (2015). Necessary conditions of learning. Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mauthner, M. (1997). Methodological aspects of collecting data from children: Lessons from three research projects. Children & Society, 11, 16–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nilsen, M., Lundin, M., Wallerstedt, C., & Pramling, N. (2021). Evolving and re-mediated activities when preschool children play analogue and digital memory games. Early Years, 41(2–3), 232–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D. R. (2003). Psychological theory and educational reform: How school remakes mind and society. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmér, H., & Björklund, C. (2020). Framing mathematics teaching with narratives: The ambiguity of Goldilocks. In M. Carlsen, I. Erfjord, & P. S. Hundeland (Eds.), Mathematics education in the early years: Results from the POEM4 Conference, 2018 (pp. 249–262). Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, M. A., White, E. J., Besley, T., Locke, K., Redder, B., Novak, R., Gibbons, A., O’Neill, J. Tesar, M., & Sturm, S. (2020). Video ethics in educational research involving children: Literature review and critical discussion. Educational Philosophy and Theory. https://doi.org/10.1080/00131857.2020.1717920.

  • Piaget, J. (1970). Piaget’s theory. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 703–732). Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1926). The language and thought of the child (M. Warden, Trans.). Harcourt, Brace. (Original work published 1923).

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1951). The child’s conception of the world (J. Tomlinson & A. Tomlinson, Trans.). Littlefield Adams. (Original work published 1926).

    Google Scholar 

  • Pramling, I. (1996). Understanding and empowering the child as a learner. In D. R. Olson & N. Torrance (Eds.), The handbook of education and human development: New models of learning, teaching and schooling (pp. 565–592). Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pramling, N. (2006a). Minding metaphors: Using figurative language in learning to represent (Göteborg Studies in Educational Sciences, 238). Acta Universitatis Gothoburgensis. Open Access: http://hdl.handle.net/2077/16798.

  • Pramling, N. (2006b). ‘The clouds are alive because they fly in the air as if they were birds’: A re-analysis of what children say and mean in clinical interviews in the work of Jean Piaget. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 21(4), 453–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pramling, N., & Säljö, R. (2015). The clinical interview: The child as a partner in conversations vs. the child as an object of research. In S. Robson & S. F. Quinn (Eds.), International handbook of young children’s thinking and understanding (pp. 87–95). Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pramling, N., Wallerstedt, C., Lagerlöf, P., Björklund, C., Kultti, A., Palmér, H., Magnusson, M., Thulin, S., Jonsson, A., & Pramling Samuelsson, I. (2019). Play-responsive teaching in early childhood education. Springer. Open Access: https://link.springer.com/book/10.1007%2F978-3-030-15958-0.

  • Reddy, M. J. (1993). The conduit metaphor: A case of frame conflict in our language about language. In A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and thought (2nd ed., pp. 164–201). Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rommetveit, R. (1974). On message structure: A framework for the study of language and communication. Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roncancio-Moreno, M., & Bronco, A. U. (2017). Developmental trajectories of the self in children during the transition from preschool to elementary school. Learning, Culture and Social Interaction, 14, 38–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Säljö, R. (2009). Learning, theories of learning, and units of analysis in research. Educational Psychologist, 44(3), 202–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schär, R. G. (2021). An argumentative analysis of the emergence of issues in adult-children discussions. John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Schoultz, J., Säljö, R., & Wyndhamn, J. (2001). Heavenly talk: Discourse, artifacts, and children’s understanding of elementary astronomy. Human Development, 44, 103–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scribner, S., & Cole, M. (1973). Cognitive consequences of formal and informal education. Science, 182(4112), 553–559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Semino, E. (2008). Metaphor in discourse. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1958). Teaching machines. Science, 128(3330), 969–977.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sommer, D., Pramling Samuelsson, I., & Hundeide, K. (2010). Child perspectives and children’s perspectives in theory and practice (International perspectives on early childhood education and development, 2). Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2005a). General introduction: Developmental science in the making: The role of Heinz Werner. In J. Valsiner (Ed.), Heinz Werner and developmental science (pp. 1–17). Kluwer Academic/Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2005b). General synthesis: Recurring agendas: Integration of developmental science. In J. Valsiner (Ed.), Heinz Werner and developmental science (pp. 391–424). Kluwer Academic/Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Oers, B. (1998). From context to contextualizing. Learning and Instruction, 8(6), 473–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes . In M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner & E. Souberman, (Eds.). Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky, Volume 1: Problems of general psychology, including the volume. Thinking and speech (R. W. Rieber & A. S. Carton, Eds., N. Minick, Trans.). Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1997). The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky, Volume 4: The history of the development of higher mental functions (M. J. Hall, Trans., R. W. Rieber, Ed.). Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1998). The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky, Volume 5: Child psychology (R. W. Rieber, Ed.; M. J. Hall, Trans.). Plenum, M. J. Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallerstedt, C., Pramling, N., & Säljö, R. (2014). Learning to discern and account: The trajectory of a listening skill in an institutional setting. Psychology of Music, 42(3), 366–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H. (1937). Process and achievement–A basic problem of education and developmental psychology. Harvard Educational Review, 7(3), 353–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertheimer, M. (1959). Productive thinking (Enlarged ed.). Greenwood Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V. (1994). The primacy of mediated action in sociocultural studies. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 1(4), 202–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V. (1998). Mind as action. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V. (2007). Mediation. In H. Daniels, M. Cole, & J. V. Wertsch (Eds.), The Cambridge companion to Vygotsky (pp. 178–192). Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. V., del Río, P., & Alvarez, A. (Eds.). (1995). Sociocultural studies of mind. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Niklas Pramling .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2023 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Pramling, N. (2023). The Importance and Functions of Theory. In: Methodology for Early Childhood Education and Care Research. SpringerBriefs in Education. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-24174-1_3

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-24174-1_3

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-031-24173-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-031-24174-1

  • eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics