Skip to main content

Fear Learning in Infancy: An Evolutionary Developmental Perspective

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Evolutionary Perspectives on Infancy

Part of the book series: Evolutionary Psychology ((EVOLPSYCH))

Abstract

Fear is ubiquitous among humans and has an evolved function to generate physiological and behavioral responses to threats that were adaptive for our ancestors. In this chapter, I outline the three main theories of fear acquisition—the traditional, nonassociative, and prepared-learning models. I then summarize my theory of fear learning in infants for recurrent threat—a perceptual template that orients infants to recurrent threats and a rapid and privileged associative-learning mechanism—and review the experimental literature that supports this model. The experimental evidence described here offers strong support not only for the prepared-learning model but also for the notion that infants’ fear mechanism drives them to detect recurrent threats such as snakes and spiders and then learn the appropriate emotional response to them. Limitations and future directions of fear learning research are also discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Askew, C., & Field, A. P. (2007). Vicarious learning and the development of fears in childhood. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 2616–2627.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Askew, C., & Field, A. P. (2008). The vicarious learning pathway to fear 40 years on. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 1249–1265.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Haim, Y. (2010). Research review: Attention bias modification (ABM): A novel treatment for anxiety disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(8), 859–870.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bertels, J., Bayard, C., Floccia, C., & Destrebecqz, A. (2018). Rapid detection of snakes modulates spatial orienting in infancy. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 42(4), 381–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bertels, J., Bourguignon, M., De Heering, A., Chetail, F., De Tiège, X., Cleeremans, A., & Destrebecqz, A. (2020). Snakes elicit specific neural responses in the human infant brain. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bosquet, M., & Egeland, B. (2006). The development and maintenance of anxiety symptoms from infancy through adolescence in a longitudinal sample. Development and Psychopathology, 18(2), 517–550.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brosch, T., & Sharma, D. (2005). The role of fear-relevant stimuli in visual search: A comparison of phylogenetic and ontogenetic stimuli. Emotion, 5(3), 360.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S., & Bartlett, E. (1978). Acquiring a single new word. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 15, 17–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark Barrett, H., & Broesch, J. (2012). Prepared social learning about dangerous animals in children. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(5), 499–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coelho, C. M., & Purkis, H. (2009). The origins of specific phobias: Influential theories and current perspectives. Review of General Psychology, 31, 335–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, L. B. (1972). Attention-getting and attention-holding processes of infant visual preferences. Child Development, 43, 869–879.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, M., & Mineka, S. (1990). Selective associations in the observational conditioning of fear in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 372–389.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Costello, E. G., & Angold, A. (1995). Epidemiology. In J. S. March (Ed.), Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents (pp. 109–122). Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeLoache, J. S., & LoBue, V. (2009). The narrow fellow in the grass: Human infants associate snakes and fear. Developmental Science, 12(1), 201–207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dollinger, S. J., O’Donnell, J. P., & Staley, A. A. (1984). Lightning-strike disaster: Effects on children’s fears and worries. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 1028–1038.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dubi, K., Rapee, R. M., Emerton, J. L., & Schniering, C. A. (2008). Maternal modeling and the acquisition of fear and avoidance in toddlers: Influence of stimulus preparedness and child temperament. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 499–512.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Egliston, K. A., & Rapee, R. M. (2007). Inhibition of fear acquisition in toddlers following positive modelling by their mothers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 1871–1882.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Esteves, F., & Öhman, A. (1993). Masking the face: Recognition of emotional facial expressions as a function of the parameters of backward masking. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 34, 1–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Esteves, F., Dimberg, U., & Öhman, A. (1994). Automatically elicited fear: Conditioned skin conductance responses to masked facial expressions. Cognition and Emotion, 8, 393–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Field, A. P., & Lester, K. J. (2010). Is there room for ‘development’ in developmental models of information processing biases to threat in children and adolescents? Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 13, 315–332.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flykt, A. (2006). Preparedness for action: Responding to the snake in the grass. The American Journal of Psychology, 119, 29–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, N. A., Hane, A. A., & Pine, D. S. (2007). Plasticity for affective neurocircuitry: How the environment affects gene expression. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(1), 1–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fredrikson, M., Annas, P., Fischer, H., & Wik, G. (1996). Gender and age differences in the prevalence of specific fears and phobias. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 33–39.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fredrikson, M., Annas, P., & Wik, G. (1997). Parental history, aversive exposure and the development of snake and spider phobia in women. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(1), 23–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia, J., & Koelling, R. A. (1966). Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychonomic Science, 4, 123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia, J., Kimeldorf, D. J., & Koelling, R. A. (1955). Conditioned aversion to saccharin resulting from exposure to gamma radiation. Science, 122, 157–158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gerull, F. C., & Rappe, R. M. (2002). Mother knows best: Effects of maternal modeling on the acquisition of fear and avoidance behavior in toddlers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 279–287.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Cauley, K. M., & Gordon, L. (1987). The eyes have it: Lexical and syntactic comprehension in a new paradigm. Journal of Child Language, 14(1), 23–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, C., & Hen, R. (2004). The developmental origins of anxiety. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(7), 545–552.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayakawa, S., Kawai, N., & Masataka, N. (2011). The influence of color on snake detection in visual search in human children. Scientific Reports, 1, 1–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • He, H., Kubo, K., & Kawai, N. (2014). Spiders do not evoke greater early posterior negativity in the event-related potential as snakes. Neuroreport, 25(13), 1049–1053.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoehl, S., & Pauen, S. (2017). Do infants associate spiders and snakes with fearful facial expressions? Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(3), 404–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunter, M., & Ames, E. (1988). A multifactor model of infant preferences for novel and familiar stimuli. In C. Rovee Collier & L. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in infancy research (pp. 69–93). Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isbell, L. A. (2006). Snakes as agents of evolutionary change in primate brains. Journal of Human Evolution, 51, 1–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. H., & Morton, J. (1991). Biology and cognitive development: The case of face recognition. Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H. D., & Morton, J. (1991). Newborns’ preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition, 40, 1–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joslin, J., Fletcher, H., & Emlen, J. (1964). A comparison of the responses to snakes of lab-and wild-reared rhesus monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 12(2–3), 348–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Fischer, J. (2004). Word learning in a domestic dog: Evidence for “fast mapping”. Science, 304, 1682–1683.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kats, L. B., Petranka, J. W., & Sih, A. (1988). Antipredator defenses and the persistence of amphibian larvae with fishes. Ecology, 69, 1865–1870.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kiesecker, J. M., Chivers, D. P., & Blaustein, A. R. (1996). The use of chemical cues in predator recognition by western toad tadpoles. Animal Behavior, 52, 1237–1245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kindt, M., & van den Hout, M. (2001). Selective attention and anxiety: A perspective on developmental issues and the causal status. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 23, 193–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • King, N. J., Eleonora, G., & Ollendick, T. H. (1998). Etiology of childhood phobias: Current status of Rachman’s three pathways model. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 297–309.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lipp, O. V. (2006). Of snakes and flowers: Does preferential detection of pictures of fear-relevant animals in visual search reflect on fear-relevance? Emotion, 6, 296–308.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V. (2009). More than just a face in the crowd: Detection of emotional facial expressions in young children and adults. Developmental Science, 12, 305–313.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V. (2010a). What’s so scary about needles and knives? Examining the role of experience in threat detection. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 80–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V. (2010b). And along came a spider: Superior detection of spiders in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 59–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V. (2014). Deconstructing the snake: The relative roles of perception, cognition, and emotion on threat detection. Emotion, 14(4), 701.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V. (2016). When is a face no longer a face? A problematic dichotomy in visual detection research. Emotion Review, 8, 250–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S. (2008). Detecting the snake in the grass: Attention to fear-relevant stimuli by adults and young children. Psychological Science, 19, 284–289.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S. (2010). Superior detection of threat-relevant stimuli in infancy. Developmental Science, 13, 221–228.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S. (2011). What so special about slithering serpents? Children and adults rapidly detect snakes based on their simple features. Visual Cognition, 19, 129–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V., & Rakison, D. H. (2013). What we fear most: A developmental advantage for threat-relevant stimuli. Developmental Review, 33(4), 285–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V., Rakison, D., & DeLoache, J. S. (2010). Threat perception across the lifespan: Evidence for multiple converging pathways. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 375–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V., Bloom Pickard, M., Sherman, K., Axford, C., & DeLoache, J. S. (2013). Young children’s interest in live animals. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31, 57–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V., Matthews, K., Harvey, T., & Stark, S. L. (2014). What accounts for the rapid detection of threat? Evidence for an advantage in perceptual and behavioral responding from eye movements. Emotion, 14(4), 816–823.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V., Buss, K. A., Taber-Thomas, B. C., & Pérez-Edgar, K. (2017). Developmental differences in infants’ attention to social and nonsocial threats. Infancy, 22(3), 403–415.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Madole, K. L., & Cohen, L. B. (1995). The role of object parts in infants’ attention to form-function correlations. Developmental Psychology, 31(4), 637–648.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marks, I. (2002). Innate and learned fears are at opposite ends of a continuum of associability. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 165–167.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marks, I. M., & Nesse, R. M. (1994). Fear and fitness: An evolutionary analyses of anxiety disorders. Ethology and Sociobiology, 15, 247–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masataka, N., Hayakawa, S., & Kawai, N. (2010). Human young children as well as adults demonstrate ‘superior’ rapid snake detection when typical striking posture is displayed by the snake. PLoS One, 5, e15122.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Matchett, G., & Davey, G. C. (1991). A test of a disease-avoidance model of animal phobias. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 29(1), 91–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Menzies, R. G., & Clarke, J. C. (1993). The etiology of childhood water phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 499–501.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Menzies, R. G., & Clarke, J. C. (1995). The etiology of phobias: A nonassociative account. Clinical Psychology Review, 15, 23–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mineka, S., & Zinbarg, R. (2006). A contemporary learning theory perspective on anxiety disorders: It’s not what you thought it was. American Psychologist, 61, 10–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mineka, S., Davidson, M., Cook, M., & Keir, R. (1984). Observational conditioning of snake fear in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 355–372.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., de Jong, P. J., & Ollendick, T. H. (2002). The etiology of specific fears and phobias in children: A critique of the non-associative account. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 185–195.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nesse, R. M. (1990). Evolutionary explanations of emotions. Human Nature, 1, 261–289.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • New, J., & German, T. C. (2015). Spiders at the cocktail party: An ancestral threat that surmounts inattentional blindness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36, 165–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A., & Dimberg, U. (1978). Facial expressions as conditioned stimuli for electrodermal responses: A case of “preparedness”? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(11), 1251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483–522.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A., Fredrikson, M., Hugdahl, K., & Rimmo, P. (1976). The premise of equipotentiality in human classical conditioning: Conditioned electrodermal responses to potentially phobic stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 105, 313–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology, General, 130, 466–478.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poulton, R., & Menzies, R. G. (2002). Non-associative fear-acquisition: A review of the evidence form retrospective and longitudinal research. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 127–149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S. J. (1977). The conditioning theory of fear acquisition: A critical examination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 15, 375–387.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S. J. (2002). Fears born and bred: Non-associative fear acquisition? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 121–126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rakison, D. H. (2004). Infants’ sensitivity to correlations between static and dynamic features in a category context. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 89(1), 1–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rakison, D. H. (2005a). Infant perception and cognition: An evolutionary perspective on early learning. In B. Elli & D. Bjorklund (Eds.), Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and child development (pp. 317–353). Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rakison, D. H. (2005b). A secret agent? How infants learn about the identity of objects in a causal scene. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 91(4), 271–296.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rakison, D. H. (2009). Does women’s greater fear of snakes and spiders originate in infancy? Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 438–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rakison, D. H. (2018). Do 5-month-old infants possess an evolved detection mechanism for snakes, sharks, and rodents? Journal of Cognition and Development, 19(4), 456–476.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rakison, D. H., & Derringer, J. L. (2008). Do infants possess an evolved spider-detection mechanism? Cognition, 107, 381–393.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rakison, D. H., & Lupyan, G. (2008). Developing object concepts in infancy: An associative learning perspective. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rakison, D. H., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2002). You go this way and I’ll go that way: Developmental changes in infants’ detection of correlations among static and dynamic features in motion events. Child Development, 73(3), 682–699.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roosevelt, F. D. (1933). FDR’s First Inaugural Address Declaring ‘War’ on the great depression. National Archives and Records Administration. Accessed December 04, 2021. https://www.archives.gov/education/lessons/fdr-inaugural

  • Rumbaugh, D. M. (1968). The learning and sensory capacities of the squirrel monkey in phylogenetic perspective. In L. A. Rosenblum & R. W. Cooper (Eds.), The squirrel monkey (pp. 256–317). Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiller, P. (1952). Innate constituents of complex responses. Psychological Review, 59, 177–191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. (1971). Phobias and preparedness. Behavior Therapy, 2, 307–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shibasaki, M., & Kawai, N. (2009). Rapid detection of snakes by Japanese Monkeys (Macaca fuscata): An evolutionarily predisposed visual system. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123, 131–135.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Slater, A., Mattock, A., Brown, E., Burnham, D., & Young, A. (1991). Visual processing of stimulus compounds in newborn infants. Perception, 20(1), 29–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thrasher, C., & LoBue, V. (2016). Do infants find snakes aversive? Infants’ physiological responses to “fear-relevant” stimuli. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 142, 382–390.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tipples, J., Young, A. W., Quinlan, P., Broks, P., & Ellis, A. W. (2002). Searching for threat. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 1007–1026.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomarken, A. J., Mineka, S., & Cook, M. (1989). Fear-relevant selective associations and covariation bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 381–394.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Le, Q., Isbell, L. A., Matsumoto, J., Ngyuen, M., Hori, E., Maior, R. S., Tomas, C., Tran, A. H., Ono, T., & Nishijo, H. (2013). Pulvinar neurons reveal neurobiological evidence of past selection for rapid detection of snakes. Proceeding of the National Academy of Science, 110, 19000–19005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vieth, W., Curio, E., & Ernst, U. (1978). The adaptive significance of avian mobbing. III. Cultural transmission of enemy recognition in blackbirds: Cross-species tutoring and properties of learning. Animal Behavior, 28, 1217–1229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walker-Andrews, A. S. (1986). Intermodal perception of expressive behaviors: Relation of eye and voice? Developmental Psychology, 22(3), 373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B., & Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emotional reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, M. A., Moss, S. A., Bradshaw, J. L., & Mattingly, J. B. (2005). Look at me, I’m smiling: Visual search for threatening and nonthreatening facial expressions. Visual Cognition, 12, 29–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yerkes, R. M. (1943). Chimpanzees: A laboratory colony. Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Younger, B. A., & Cohen, L. B. (1986). Developmental change in infants’ perception of correlations among attributes. Child Development, 57, 803–815.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yule, W., Udwin, O., & Murdoch, K. (1990). The ‘Jupiter’ sinking: Effects in children’s fears, depression and anxiety. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 1051–1106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David H. Rakison .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2022 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Rakison, D.H. (2022). Fear Learning in Infancy: An Evolutionary Developmental Perspective. In: Hart, S.L., Bjorklund, D.F. (eds) Evolutionary Perspectives on Infancy. Evolutionary Psychology. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-76000-7_14

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics