Skip to main content

A Broader Design Context—The Form/Function Debate in L2A/FLL Research

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Form-Function Mapping in Content-Based Language Teaching

Part of the book series: Second Language Learning and Teaching ((SLLT))

  • 417 Accesses

Abstract

This chapter sets the scene for further discussion by outlining the universal character of the form-function dichotomy in L2A/FLL research. Section 1.2 discusses the roots and manifestations of the form-function dialectic, as present in L1A research. Section 1.3 shows how the dichotomy translates into L2A/FLL studies. Section 1.4 gives the rationale for trying to reconcile both foci in L2A/FLL at the level of theory construction and teaching practice. Section 1.5 provides epistemic and utilitarian justification for introducing formal aspects into L2A/FLL pedagogy, thus further validating the need for the form-function reconciliation.

Now you may be thinking that the form-function dichotomy is a false one

(Larsen-Freeman, 2003, p. 7)

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    For reasons elaborated later on (see Sect. 3.3.1 ) this book draws a distinction between child and adult L2/FL learning.

  2. 2.

    The concept of I-language echoes Saussure’s concept of langue , which emphasizes the need to study the linguistic system, however—an aspect not adopted by formalists—locates the system outside the individual, in the speech community (Anderson & Lightfoot, 2002, pp. 12–13).

  3. 3.

    The concept of E-language is reminiscent of Saussure’s concept of parole the concrete acts of language use (Anderson & Lightfoot, 2002, p. 12).

  4. 4.

    The fact that Ninio, although adopting the minimalist program (e.g., Hauser, Chomsky, & Fitch, 2002), rejects the role of UG in favor of the connectionist account of acquisition, makes her claim of the primacy of form no less forceful, and thus I locate her research within the formalist strand.

  5. 5.

    To exemplify, children pay no attention to the semantic properties of the subject noun phrase when acquiring the auxiliary inversion structure (Newmeyer, 1998, p. 48).

  6. 6.

    According to the generative framework, properties of language are captured in universal principles encoded in the linguistic genome, while parameters—set via exposure to the target language input—account for cross-linguistic variation (Schachter, 1996a, p. 71; Smith, 2004, p. 79).

  7. 7.

    The emphasis on interaction is at times caricatured by the formal L2A camp as “do[ing] nothing but chat” (Gregg, 2000).

  8. 8.

    A few attempts at merging cognitive functionalism (e.g., Langacker, 2008) with L2A/FLL have also been made (e.g., Bielak, Pawlak, & Mystkowska-Wiertelak, 2013; Turewicz, 2000), yet their analysis exceeds the scope of this book.

  9. 9.

    See further discussion for the clarification of the capitalized S.

  10. 10.

    Unless, in a manner proposed by Gozdawa-Gołębiowski (2013) or Hirsh-Pasek and Golinkoff (1999), one interprets top-down as rule-based (and bottom-up as lexically-based)—this time naturally locating top-down within the formal, and bottom-up within the functional orientation.

  11. 11.

    The issue of implicit/incidental learning resurfaces in Chap. 3, where the psycholinguistics of L2A/FLL are zoomed in on.

  12. 12.

    Therefore being sometimes categorized (e.g., Gozdawa-Gołębiowski, 2013) as a utilitarian method.

  13. 13.

    Thus, the CTP may be seen as the forerunner of task-based instruction (TBI) (Pawlak, 2006, p. 153).

  14. 14.

    Why the L2/FL learner needs both—grammar and communicative effectiveness—is explained in Sect. 1.5.

  15. 15.

    Links with Swain’s output hypothesis (1985) are visible here but—the latter being associated with bilingual education—I postpone its presentation till Chap. 2.

  16. 16.

    Close (1992, pp. 1–2) qualifies the meaning and use dimension as grammar as choice , as opposed to the formal dimension, which he qualifies as grammar as fact .

  17. 17.

    This book leans toward Close’s (1992) two-tier distinction, on the grounds that propositional choices entail pragmatic distinctions and it is often difficult to tear them neatly apart (see also 4.5.3.2).

  18. 18.

    The term IE was introduced by Smith (1993) to replace C-R, in order to avoid the assumption that it necessarily alters the learner’s mental state. However, the term IE is typically used to refer to typographical input enhancement (e.g., Reinders & Ellis, 2009, p. 282; VanPatten, 1996, p. 84).

  19. 19.

    Recasting involves implicit, target-like reformulations of ill-formed learners’ utterances, provided by the teacher (e.g., Lyster, 2004, p. 404).

  20. 20.

    For other techniques of output manipulation see Sects. 2.3.2 and 3.5.2.4.

  21. 21.

    Prompts—unlike recasts—withhold correct forms and offer learners an opportunity to self-repair (Lyster, 2004, p. 405).

  22. 22.

    The study also demonstrated that prompts were more effective than recasts in shaping learners’ knowledge of the target feature—a point to be returned to in Sect. 3.4.1.3.

  23. 23.

    Obviously, this is an oversimplification, not doing justice to the complexity of the ELF research—the latter remaining beyond the scope of this book (for more see e.g., Seidlhofer, 2011).

  24. 24.

    Having over ten years of EFL teaching experience, I am actually surprised how many of my students—in the context of vocational education, unrelated directly to English—express interest in the intricacies of English grammar.

  25. 25.

    This is what actually happened to one of my friends—and there was no happy ending.

  26. 26.

    Horwitz (2013) compares the experience to wearing frilly pink dresses, totally not to her taste, yet a gift from a close relative whose feelings she did not wish to hurt.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Magdalena Walenta .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Walenta, M. (2019). A Broader Design Context—The Form/Function Debate in L2A/FLL Research. In: Form-Function Mapping in Content-Based Language Teaching. Second Language Learning and Teaching. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-04699-6_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-04699-6_1

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-04698-9

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-04699-6

  • eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics