Skip to main content

Preparedness to Learn About the World: Evidence from Infant Research

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Abstract

Infants’ learning about the world is determined by a complex interplay of biological predispositions and everyday experiences. As will be demonstrated in this chapter, evolution may have prepared us to perceive objects and events in a pre-specified way. We seem to be able to categorize objects, or to reason about physical and social events from early on. Moreover, recent evidence suggests that we come equipped with mechanisms allowing us to identify instructional interactions and to learn about the value of objects simply by observing the emotional responses of interactive partners towards these targets. As will be argued, this mechanism plays a key role in explaining why certain stimuli (e.g., spiders) induce fear in children and adults who never had any bad experience with these stimuli. It is not the nature of the object, nor the emotional response of an interactive partner in an instructional situation, but rather the combination of both that might explain the transmission of emotional attitudes towards these objects. In more general terms, we claim that infants’ learning about the world is modulated by social interactive experiences, thus allowing for the cultural transmission of object knowledge that has deep roots in phylogeny.

A German version of this chapter can be found in Breyer et al. (2013).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

References

  • Baillargeon, R., & Carey, S. (2012). Core knowledge and beyond: The acquisition of physical and numerical knowledge. In S. Pauen (Ed.), Early childhood development and later outcome. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Behl-Chadha, G. (1996). Basic-level and superordinate-like categorical representations in early infancy. Cognition, 60, 105–141.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Biro, S., & Leslie, A. M. (2007). Infants’ perception of goal-directed actions: Development through cue-based bootstrapping. Developmental Science, 10(3), 379–389.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Booth, A. E., Pinto, J., & Bertenthal, B. I. (2002). Perception of the symmetrical patterning of human gait by infants. Developmental Psychology, Vol 38(4), Jul 2002, 554-563. doi:http://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0012-1649.38.4.554/10.1037/0012-1649.38.4.554.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Breyer, T., Etzelmüller, G., Fuchs, T., Schwarzkopf, G., Tanner, K., & Welker, M. (Eds.). (2013). Interdisziplinäre Anthropologie: Leib – Geist – Kultur. Heidelberg: Winter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Briscoe, T. (2000). Grammatical acquisition: Inductive bias and coevolution of language and the language acquisition device. Language, 76(2), 245–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brugger, A., Lariviere, L. A., Mumme, D. L., & Bushnell, E. W. (2007). Doing the right thing: Infants’ selection of actions to imitate from observed event sequences. Child Development, 78(3), 806–824.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campos, J. J., Thein, S., & Owen, D. (2003). A Darwinian legacy to understanding human infancy—Emotional expressions as behavior regulators. Emotions Inside Out, 1000, 110–134. doi:10.1196/annals.1280.040.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carver, L. J., & Vaccaro, B. G. (2007). 12-month-old infants allocate increased neural resources to stimuli associated with negative adult emotion. Developmental Psychology, 43(1), 54–69. doi:2006-23020-005 [pii] 10.1037/0012-1649.43.1.54.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, E. W., Hodes, R. L., & Lang, P. J. (1986). Preparedness and phobia: Effects of stimulus content on human visceral conditioning. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95(3), 195–207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, M., & Mineka, S. (1989). Observational conditioning of fear to fear-relevant versus fear-irrelevant stimuli in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98(4), 448–459.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4), 148–153. doi:S1364-6613(09)00047-3 [pii]10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Haan, M., Belsky, J., Reid, V., Volein, A., & Johnson, M. H. (2004). Maternal personality and infants’ neural and visual responsivity to facial expressions of emotion. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(7), 1209–1218. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00320.x JCPP320 [pii].

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deligianni, F., Senju, A., Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2011). Automated gaze-contingent objects elicit orientation following in 8-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 47(6), 1499–1503.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deloache, J. S., & LoBue, V. (2009). The narrow fellow in the grass: Human infants associate snakes and fear. Developmental Science, 12(1), 201–207. doi:DESC753 [pii]10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00753.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elsner, B. (2007). Infants’ imitation of goal-directed actions: The role of movements and action effects. Acta Psychologica, 124(1), 44–59. doi:S0001–6918(06)00122–3[pii]10.1016/j.actpsy. (2006.09.006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elsner, B., & Pauen, S. (2007). Social learning in artifact function in 12- and 15-month-olds. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4(1), 80–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elsner, B., Jeschonek, S., & Pauen, S. (in press). Event-related potentials for 7-month-olds processing of animals and furniture items. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 53–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farroni, T., Mansfield, E. M., Lai, C., & Johnson, M. H. (2003). Infants perceiving and acting on the eyes: Tests of an evolutionary hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85(3), 199–212.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Farroni, T., Johnson, M. H., Menon, E., Zulian, L., Faraguna, D., & Csibra, G. (2005). Newborns’ preference for face-relevant stimuli: Effects of contrast polarity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 102(47), 17245–17250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fredrikson, M., Annas, P., Fischer, H., & Wik, G. (1996). Gender and age differences in the prevalence of specific fears and phobias. Behavioral Research and Therapy, 34(1), 33–39. doi:0005796795000483 [pii].

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, R., & Spelke, E. S. (1981). The development of thoughts about animate and inanimate objects: Implications for research in social cognition. In J. H. Flavell & L. Ross (Eds.), The development of social cognition in children (pp. 43–66). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G., Egyed, K., & Kiraly, I. (2007). On pedagogy. Developmental Science, 10(1), 139–146. doi:DESC576 [pii]10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00576.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grossmann, T., Johnson, M. H., Vaish, A., Hughes, D. A., Quinque, D., Stoneking, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2011). Genetic and neural dissociation of individual responses to emotional expressions in human infants. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(1), 57–66. doi:S1878–9293(10)00002–2 [pii] 10.1016/j.dcn.2010.07.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gunnar, M. R., & Stone, C. (1984). The Effects of Positive Maternal Affect on Infant Responses to Pleasant, Ambiguous, and Fear-provoking Toys. Child Development, 55, 1231–1236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, E., Call, J., Lloreda, M., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science, 317, 1360–1366.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hertenstein, M. J., & Campos, J. J. (2004). The retention effects of an adult’s emotional displays on infant behavior. Child Development, 75(2), 595–613. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004. 00695.xCDEV695 [pii].

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoehl, S. & Pauen, S. (in preparation). Do infants associate evolutionary fear-relevant stimuli with fearful facial expressions?

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoehl, S., & Striano, T. (2008). Neural processing of eye gaze and threat-related emotional facial expressions in infancy. Child Development, 79(6), 1752–1760.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoehl, S., & Striano, T. (2010). The development of emotional face and eye gaze processing. Developmental Science, 13(6), 813–825.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoehl, S., Wiese, L., & Striano, T. (2008). Young infants’ neural processing of objects is affected by eye gaze direction and emotional expression. PLoS One, 3(6), e2389. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0002389.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoehl, S., Wahl, S., Michel, C., & Striano, T. (2012). Effects of eye gaze cues provided by the caregiver compared to a stranger on infants’ object processing. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2(1), 81–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jeschoneck, S., Marinovic, V., Höhl, S., Elsner, B., & Pauen, S. (2010). Do animals and furniture items elicit different brain responses in human infants? Brain Development, 32(10), 863–871. doi:10.1016/j.braindev. 2009.11.010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M., & Morton, M. H. (1991). Conspec and conlearn: A two-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review, 98, 164–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klinnert, M. D., Emde, R. N., Butterfield, P., & Campos, J. J. (1986). Social Referencing: The infant’s use of emotional signals from a friendly adult with mother present. Developmental Psychology, 22, 427–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kosugi, D., Ishida, H., & Fujita, K. (2003). 10-month-old infants’ inference of invisible agent: Distinction in causality between object motion and human action. Japanese Psychological Research, 45, 15–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V. (2010). And along came a spider: An attentional bias for the detection of spiders in young children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107(1), 59–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S. (2008). Detecting the snake in the grass: Attention to fear-relevant stimuli by adults and young children. Psychological Science, 19(3), 284–289. doi:PSCI2081 [pii] 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02081.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S. (2010). Superior detection of threat-relevant stimuli in infancy. Developmental Science, 13(1), 221–228. doi:DESC872[pii]10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009. 00872.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, J. M. (1992). How to build a baby II: Conceptual primitives. Psychological Review, 99(4), 587–604.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, J. M., & McDonough, L. (1994). Concept formation in infancy. Cognitive Development, 8, 291–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E. M. (1994). Constraints on early word learning in language acquisition. Lingua, 92, 199–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development, 54, 702–709.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mineka, S., & Öhman, A. (2002). Phobias and preparedness: The selective, automatic, and encapsulated nature of fear. Biological Psychiatry, 52(10), 927–937. doi:S000632230 2016694 [pii].

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mineka, S., Davidson, M., Cook, M., & Keir, R. (1984). Observational conditioning of snake fear in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93(4), 355–372.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moses, L. J., Baldwin, D. A., Rosicky, J. G., & Tidball, G. (2001). Evidence for referential understanding in the emotions domain at twelve and eighteen months. Child Development, 72(3), 718–735.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mumme, D. L., & Fernald, A. (2003). The infant as onlooker: Learning from emotional reactions observed in a television scenario. Child Development, 74(1), 221–237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108(3), 483–522.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, C. A., & de Haan, M. (1996). Neural correlates of infants’ visual responsiveness to facial expressions of emotion. Developmental Psychobiology, 29(7), 577–595. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098–2302(199611)29:7<577::AID-DEV3>3.0.CO;2-R[pii]10.1002/(SICI) 1098-2302(199611)29:7&lt;577::AID-DEV3 &gt;3.0.CO;2-R.

    Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(3), 466–478.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olsson, A., & Phelps, E. A. (2007). Social learning of fear. Nature Neuroscience, 10(9), 1095–1102. doi:nn1968 [pii]10.1038/nn1968.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pauen, S. (2000). Developing ontological categories of matter: Stable dimensions and changing concepts. In W. Schnotz, S. Vosniadou, & M. Carretero (Eds.), New perspectives on conceptual change (pp. 15–31). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pauen, S. (2002a). The global-to-basic level shift in infants’ categorical thinking. evidence from a longitudinal study. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26(6), 492–499.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pauen, S. (2002b). Evidence for knowledge-based categorization in infancy. Child Development, 73, 116–1033.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pauen, S., & Träuble, B. (2009). How 7-month-olds interpret ambiguous motion events: Category-specific reasoning in infancy. Cognitive Psychology, 59, 275–295.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, P. C., & Johnson, M. H. (2000). Global before basic object categorization in connectionist networks and 2-month-old infants. Infancy, 1(1), 41–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rakison, D. H. (2009). Does women’s greater fear of snakes and spiders originate in infancy? Evolution and Human Behavior, 30(6), 439–444. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.06.002.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pauen, S., Wahl, S., & Höhl, S. (2011). The beginnings or infant categorization: An ERP study with 4- and 7-month-olds. Poster presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Montreal, Canada.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rakison, D., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2001). Developmental origin of the animate-inanimate distinction. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 209–228.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reck, C., Hunt, A., Fuchs, T., Weiss, R., Noon, A., Moehler, E., Downing, G., Tronick, E. Z., & Mundt, C. (2004). Interactive regulation of affect in postpartum depressed mothers and their infants: An overview. Psychopathology, 37(6), 272–280.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reid, V., Striano, T., Kaufmann, J., & Johnson, M. H. (2004). Eye-gaze cueing facilitates neural processing of objects in 4-month-olds. NeuroReport, 15(6), 2553–2555.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, G. D., & Richards, J. E. (2005). Familiarization, attention, and recognition memory in infancy: An event-related potential and cortical source localization study. Developmental Psychology, 41(4), 598–615. doi:2005-08221-002 [pii] 10.1037/0012-1649.41.4.598.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, M. F., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Young children enforce social norms selectively depending on the violator’s group affiliation. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Cognition, 124(3), 325–333. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E. (1971). Phobias and preparedness. Behavior Therapy, 2, 307–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Senju, A., Csibra, G., & Johnson, M. H. (2008). Understanding the referential nature of looking: Infants’ preference for object-directed gaze. Cognition, 108, 303–319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simion, F., Regolin, L., & Bulf, H. (2008). A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(2), 809–813.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E. S. (1990). Principles of object perception. Cognitive Science, 14, 29–36.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E. S. (2011). Core systems and the growth of human knowledge: Natural geometry. In A. M. Battro, S. Dehaene, & W. J. Singer (Eds.), The proceedings of the working group on human neuroplasticity and education, vol. 117. Human neuroplasticity and education (pp. 73–99). Vatican City: The Pontifical Academy of Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E. S., & Kinzler, K. D. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10(1), 89–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E. S., Phillips, A. T., & Woodward, A. L. (1995). Infants’ knowledge of object motion and human action. In D. Sperber, D. Premack, & A. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate (pp. 44–78). Oxford: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stenberg, G., & Hagekull, B. (2007). Infant looking behavior in ambiguous situations: Social referencing or attachment behavior? Infancy, 11(2), 111–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Striano, T., & Rochat, P. (2000). Emergence of selective social referencing in infancy. Infancy, 1(2), 253–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Striano, T., & Stahl, D. (2005). Sensitivity to early triadic interaction in infancy. Developmental Science, 8(4), 333–343.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Striano, T., Vaish, A., & Henning, A. (2006). Selective looking by 12-month-olds to a temporally contingent partner. Interaction Studies, 7, 233–250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2011). Human culture in evolutionary perspective. In M. Gelfand, C. Chiu, & Y. Hong (Eds.), Advances in culture and psychology (vol. 1, pp. 5–51). Oxford: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Träuble, B., & Pauen, S. (2007). The role of functional information for infant categorization. Cognition, 105(2), 362–379. doi:S0010-0277(06)00211-3[pii]10.1016/j.cognition.2006. 10.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Träuble, B., & Pauen, S. (2010). Cause or effect—What matters? How 12-month-old infants learn to categorize artefacts. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29(3), 357–374. doi:10.1348/02615 1009X479547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tronick, E., & Reck, C. (2009). Infants of depressed mothers. Harvard Review Psychiatry, 17(2), 147–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaish, A., Grossmann, T., & Woodward, A. (2008). Not all emotions are created equal: The negativity bias in social-emotional development. Psychological Bulletin, 134(3), 383–403. doi:2008–04614-002 [pii]10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.383.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vouloumanos, A., Hauer, M. D., Werker, J. F., & Martin, A. (2010). The tuning of neonates preference for speech. Child Development, 81(2), 517–527.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wynn, K. (1995). Origins of numerical knowledge. Mathematical Cognition, 1, 35–60.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sabina Pauen Ph.D., Dipl.-Psych. .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Pauen, S., Hoehl, S. (2015). Preparedness to Learn About the World: Evidence from Infant Research. In: Breyer, T. (eds) Epistemological Dimensions of Evolutionary Psychology. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-1387-9_8

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics