Skip to main content

Developmental Patterns in Disabled Infants and Preschoolers

  • Chapter
Assessment of Young Developmentally Disabled Children

Part of the book series: Perspectives in Developmental Psychology ((PDPS))

Abstract

In the review of evidence on the pattern of cognitive and socioemotional development of the young developmentally disabled child, three factors combine to make the job difficult. First, as will become obvious, although for some disabilities, such as retardation, there is ample evidence on developmental patterns across the life span, for other groups, such as the mutiply handicapped, there are large gaps in our knowledge base, particularly below the age of 5 years. Often, only a single study represents our knowledge of development in critical areas. This problem is compounded by the all-too-common practice of aggregating data for different disability conditions under the labels of handicapped or developmentally delayed, and comparing the performance of a heterogeneous group of handicapped children to that of the nonhandicapped (i.e., Mundy, Seibert, & Hogan, 1984). As will become obvious, this assumption that all handicapping conditions are functionally equivalent in their developmental patterns does not appear to be justified by the data. Further, even when supposedly homogeneous handicaps are used, children with differing degrees of handicaps are very often clumped together. Thus, it is not uncommon to find studies in which the term motor-impaired is used to designate children with and without central nervous system (CNS) etiology (i.e., Fishman & Palkes, 1974). As a result, even when consistent relationships are found, the degree of generalizability is unclear, as distinctions based on the degree of the etiology of the handicap are rarely made.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Achenbach, T., & Edelbrock, C. (1978). The classification of child psychopathology: A review and analysis of empirical efforts. Psychological Bulletin, 85(6), 1275–1301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Als, H., Tronick, E., & Brazelton, T. (1980). Stages of early behavioral organization. In T. Field (Ed)., High risk infants and children.

    Google Scholar 

  • Altshuler, K. (1974). The social and psychological development of the deaf child: Problems, their treatment, and prevention. American Annals of the Deaf, 119(4), 365–376.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arnold R., & Tremblay, A. (1979). Interaction of deaf and hearing preschool children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 12, 245–251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ashton, R., & Beasley, M. (1982). Cerebral laterality in deaf and hearing children. Developmental Psychology, 18, 294–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Banham, K. (1972). Progress in mental development of retarded cerebral palsied children. Exceptional Children, 39, 240.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Banham, K. (1976). Progress in motor development of retarded cerebral palsied children. Rehabilitation Literature, 37, 13–16.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, J. (1972). Temperament profile of children with Down’s syndrome. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 14, 640–643.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Battle, C. V. (1977). Disruptions in the socialization of a young severely handicapped child. In R. P. Marinelli & A. E. Dell Orto (Eds.), The psychological and social impact of physical handicap. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Best, B., & Roberts, G. (1976). Early cognitive development in hearing impaired children. American Annals of the Deaf, 121, 560–564.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bigelow, A. (1980). Object permanence for sound producing objects: Parallels between blind and sighted infants. Paper presented to International Conference on Infant Studies. New Haven.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bigelow, A. (1983). Development of the use of sound in search behavior of infants. Developmental Psychology, 19, 317–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blank, M., & Bridge, W. (1966). Conceptual cross-model transfer in deaf and hearing impaired children. Child Development, 37, 29–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bridges, F., & Cicchetti, D. (1982). Mother’s ratings of the temperament characteristics of Down Syndrome infants. Developmental Psychology, 18(2), 238–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks-Gunn, J. & Lewis, M. (1982). Affective exchanges between normal infants and handicapped infants and their mothers. In T. Field & A. Fogel (Eds.), Emotion and early interaction. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. (1974). The role of strategic behavior in retardate memory. In N. Ellis (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation (Vol 8). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunick, P. (1981). Relationship between intellectual functioning and communicative competence in deaf children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 30, 3–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burlingham. D. (1975). Special problems of blind infants. Psychoanalytic study of the child, 30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burlinghan, D. (1979). To be born blind in a sighted world. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 34, 5–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caeg, W. (1970). A simplified method for measuring infant temperament. Journal of Pediatrics, 77, 188–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chess, S., Korn, S., & Fernandez, P. (1971). Psychiatric disorders of children with congenital rubella. New York: Brunner-Mazel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Churchill, D. (1972). The relations of infantile autism and early childhood schizophrenia to developmental language disorders of childhood. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 2(2), 182–197.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ciccletti, D., & Sroute, A. (1978). An organizational view of affect: Illustration from the study of Down Syndrome infants. In M. Lewis & L. Rosenblum (Eds.), The development of affect. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, P., & Rutter, M. (1979). Task difficulty and task performance in autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 20, 271–285.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cruickshank, W., Hallahan, D., & Bice, H. (1976a). The evaluation of intelligence. In W. Cruickshank (Ed.), Cerebral palsy: A developmental disability. Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cruickshank, W., Hallahan, D., & Bice, H. (1976b). Personality and behavioral characteristics. In W. Cruickshank (Ed.), Cerebral palsy: A developmental disability. Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Decarie, T., (1969). A study of the mental and emotional development of the Thalidamide child. In B. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of infant behavior (Vol 4). New York: Barnes & Noble.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeMyer, M., Barton, S., DeMyer, W., Norton, J., Allen, J., & Steele, R. (1973). Prognosis in autism: A followup study. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 3, 199–246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeMyer, M., Barton, S., Alpern, G., Kimberlin, C., Allen, J., Yang, E., & Steele, R. (1974). The measured intelligence of autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 4(1), 42–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dubose, R. (1976). Predictive value of infant intelligence scales with multiply handicapped children. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 81(4), 388–390.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dubose, R. (1977). Assessing severely handicapped children. Focus on exceptional children, 9, 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eichorn, S. (1982). Congenital cytomegalovirus infection: A significant cause of deafness and mental deficiency. American Annuals of the Deaf, 127, 838–843.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fishman, M., & Palkes, H. (1974). The validity of psychometric testing in children with congenital malformations of the central nervous system. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 16, 180–185.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Folstein, S., & Rutter, M. (1977). Infantile autism: A genetic study of 21 twin pairs. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 18, 297–321.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fraiberg, S. (1968). Parallel and divergent patterns in blind and sighted infants. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 23, 264–300.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fraiberg, S., & Freedman, D. (1969). Some studies in the ego development of the congenitally blind child. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 19, 113–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraiberg, S., Siegel, B., & Gibson, R. (1966). The role of sound in the search behavior of a blind infant. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 21, 327–357.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freedman, D. (1981). Speech, language and the vocal-auditory connection. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 36, 105–127.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freedman, D., Canady, A., & Robinson, J. (1971). Speech and psychic structure. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 19, 765–779.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, R. (1967). Emotional reactions of handicapped children. Rehabilitation Literature, 28, 274–282.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Furth, H. (1966). Thinking without languages: Psychological implications of deafness. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galenson, E., Miller, R., Kaplan, E., & Rothstein, A. (1979). Assessment of development in the deaf child. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 18, 128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geffner, D., & Freeman, L. (1980). Assessment of language comprehension of 6 year old deaf children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 13, 455–570.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goetzinger, C., & Proud, G. (1975). The impact of hearing impairment on the psychological development of children. Journal of Auditory Research, 15, 1–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottesman, M. (1971). A comparative study of Piagetian developmental schema of sighted children with that of a group of blind children. Child Development, 42, 573–580.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gottesman, M. (1973). Conservation development in blind children. Child Development, 44, 824–827.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, M., & Marvin, R. (1979). Attachment patterns in profoundly deaf preschool children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 25, 265–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, R., & Field, T. (1982). Temperament ratings of handicapped infants during classroom, mother and teacher interactions. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 7, 387–405.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, H. (1977). Manual on terminology and classification in mental retardation. Washington, DC: American Association on Mental Deficiency.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gruen, G., & Vore, D. (1972). Development of conservation in normal and retarded children. Developmental Psychology, 6(1), 146–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gustafson, G. (1984). Effects of the ability to locomote on infant’s social and exploratory behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 20, 39–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, R. (1978). Impulse control in deaf children: Research and clinical issues. In L. Liben (Ed.), Deaf children developmental perspectives. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heffernan, L., Black, F., & Poche, P. (1982). Temperament patterns in young neurologically handicapped children. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 7, 415–423.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F., & Heider, G. (1941). Studies in the psychology of the deaf. Psychological Monographs, 242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn, D., Lord, E., Lorch, F., & Culatte, B. (1985). Distractability and vocabulary deficits in children with Spina bifida and hydrocephalus. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 16, 180–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hubert, N., & Wachs, T. (1985). Parental perceptions of the behavioral components of infant easiness-difficultness. Child Development, 56, 1525–1537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inhelder, B. (1968). The diagnosis of reasoning in the mentally retarded. New York: Day.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahn, J. (1976). Utility of the Uzgiris and Hunt Scales of Sensorimotor Development with severely and profoundly retarded children. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 80(6), 663–665.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahn, J. (1978). Acceleration of object permanence with severely and profoundly retarded children. AAESPH Review, March, 15-22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahn, J. (1984). Cognitive training and its relationship to the language of retarded children. In J. Berg (Ed.), Perspectives and Progress in mental retardation. Baltimore: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karagan, N. (1979). Intellectual functioning in Duchenne muscular distrophy: A review. Psychological Bulletine, 86, 250–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keogh, B., & Pullis, M. (1980). Temperament influences on the development of exceptional children. In B. Keogh (Ed.), Advances in special education (Vol. 1).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kogan, K. (1980). Interaction systems between preschool handicapped or developmentally delayed children and their parents. In T. Field (Ed.), High risk infants and children: Adult and peer interactions. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kopp, C., & Shaperman, J. (1973). Cognitive development in the absence of object manipulation during infancy. Developmental Psychology, 9, 430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landau, B., Gleitman, H., & Spelke, E. (1981). Spatial knowledge and geometric representation in a child blind from birth. Science, 213, 1275–1278.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leibowitz, D., & Dubowitz, V. (1981). Intellect and behavior in Duchenne muscular distrophy. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 23, 577–590.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levy-Schiff, R., & Hoffman, M. (1985). Social behavior of learning impaired and normally hearing preschoolers. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 55, 111–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lubin, B., & Sherrill, C. (1980). Motor creativity of preschool deaf children. American Annuals of the Deaf, 125, 460–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacDougall, J., & Rabinovitch, S. (1971). Early auditory deprivation and sensory compensation. Developmental Psychology, 5, 368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marquis, P., Palmer, F., Mahoney, W., & Capute, A. (1982). Extrapyramidal cerebral palsy: A changing view. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 3, 65–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meadow, K. (1975). The development of deaf children. In E. M. Hetherington, J. Hagen, R. Kron, & A. Stein (Eds.), Review of child development research (Vol. 5). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meadow, K. (1980). Deafness and child development. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Siebert, J., & Hogan, A. (1984). Relationship between sensorimotor and communication abilities in developmentally delayed children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 30, 33–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mykelbust, H. (1964). The psychology of deafness. New York: Grune & Stratton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neilson, H. (1971). Psychological appraisal of children with cerebral palsy. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 13, 707–720.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reynell, J. (1978). Developmental patterns of visually handicapped children. Child Care, Health, and Development, 4, 291–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, S. (1969). The effects of physical disability on the socialization of a child. In D. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research. Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S. (1977). Characteristics of the cognitive development of profoundly retarded children. Child Development, 48, 837–893.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenzweig, M. (1966). Environmental complexity, cerebral change and behavior. American Psychologist, 21, 3321–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M. (1972). Childhood schizophrenia reconsidered. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 2, 315–337.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M. (1978). Brain damage syndromes in childhood. In S. Chess & A. Thomas (Eds.), Annual progress in child psychiatry and child development. New York: Brunner-Mazel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., & Lockyer, L. (1967a). A five to fifteen year follow-up study of infantile psychosis: 1. Description of the sample. British Journal of Psychiatry, 113, 1169–1182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., & Lockyer, L. (1967b). A five to fifteen year follow-up study of infantile psychosis: 2. Social and behavioral outcome. British Journal of Psychiatry, 113, 1183–1199.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sameroff, A., & Chandler, M. (1975). Reproductive risk and the continuum of caretaking casuality. In P. Horowitz (Ed.), Review of Child Development Research (Vol. 3). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sandier, A. (1963). Aspects of passivity and ego development in the blind child. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 10, 343–360.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schimer, B. (1985). An analysis of the language of young hearing impaired children in terms of syntax, semantics and use. American Annuals of the Deaf, 100, 15–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schlesinger, H. (1978). The effects of deafness on child development: An Ericksonian perspective. In L. Liben (Ed.), Deaf children: Developmental perspectives. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlesinger, H., & Meadow, K. (1972). Sound and sign: Childhood deafness and mental health. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott, R. (1969). The socialization of blind children. In D. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research. Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidel, U., Chadewick, D., & Rutter, M. (1975). Psychological disorders in crippled children: A comparative study of children with and without brain damage. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 17, 563–573.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smits, B., & Mommers, M. (1976). Differences between blind and sighted children on WISC verbal subtests. New Outlook for the Blind, 70, 240–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spain, B. (1974). Verbal and performance ability in preschool children with spina bifida. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 16, 773–780.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spitz, H. (1983). Critique of the developmental position in mental retardation research. Journal of Special Education, 17, 261–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterritt, G., Camp, B., & Lysman, B. (1966). Effects of early auditory deprivation upon auditory and visual information processing. Perceptual Motor Skills, 23, 123–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New York: Brunner/Mazel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, A., Chess, S., & Birch, H. (1968). Temperament and behavior disorders in children. New York: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uzgiris, I., & Benson, J. (1980). Infant use of sound in search for objects. Paper presented to the International Conference on Infant Studies, New Haven, April.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uzgiris, I., & Hunt, J. (1975). Assessment in infancy: Ordinal scales of psychological development. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vandell, D., & George, L. (1981). Social interaction in hearing and deaf preschoolers: Successes and failures in initiations. Child Development, 52, 627–635.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vandell, D., Anderson, L., Ehrhardt, G., & Wilson, K. (1982). Integrating hearing and deaf preschoolers. Child Development, 53, 1354–1363.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • VanderVeer, B., & Schweid, E. (1974). Infant assessment: Stability of mental functioning in young retarded children. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 79(1), 1–4.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Lieshout, C. (1973). The assessment of stability and change in per interactions of normal hearing and deaf preschool children. Paper presented to the International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development, Ann Arbor, August.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wassermann, G., Allen, R., & Solomon, C. (1985). At risk toddlers and their mothers: The special case of physical handicap. Child Development, 56, 73–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wedell-Monig, J., & Lumley, J. (1980). Child deafness and mother-child interaction. Child Development, 55, 766–774.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weisz, J., & Zigler, E. (1979). Cognitive development in retarded and non-retarded persons: Piagetian tests of the similar sequence hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 831–851.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilton, K., & Boersma, F. (1974). Conservation research with the mentally retarded. In N. Ellis (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation (Vol. 8). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zigler, E. (1969). Developmental versus difference theories of mental retardation and the problem of motivation. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 73, 536–566.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1988 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Wachs, T.D., Sheehan, R. (1988). Developmental Patterns in Disabled Infants and Preschoolers. In: Wachs, T.D., Sheehan, R. (eds) Assessment of Young Developmentally Disabled Children. Perspectives in Developmental Psychology. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-9306-2_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-9306-2_1

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4757-9308-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4757-9306-2

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics