Skip to main content

A Cognitive Reinterpretation of Classical Introspectionism

The Relation between Introspection and Altered States of Consciousness and Their Mutual Relevance for a Cognitive Psychology of Metaphor and Felt Meaning

  • Chapter

Part of the book series: Annals of Theoretical Psychology ((AOTP,volume 4))

Abstract

This paper is a step toward relinking the scattered subjectivist-phenomenological traditions in psychology to offer some balance for our current “extraverted” obsession with the experimental management of information and computation. Felt meaning, physiognomy, and metaphor are crucial to such a Fechnerian endeavor, because these most subjective aspects of symbolic cognition are also most strikingly attuned to the qualia afforded by the environment.

Specifically, the striking overlap between the techniques and phenomena of altered states of consciousness and classical introspectionism, understood through a holistic cognitive perspective, exteriorizes normally masked aspects of metaphor—the synaesthetically mediated reorganization of microgenetic-iconic stages of perception. Titchener’s sensory-affective core has the place in a psychology of metaphor that it so lacked in functional perception (Gibson), and the disparity between Würzburg “impalpables” of thought and Cornell imagery protocols can be resolved if we assume that all representational processes have a presentational aspect based on complex or geometric synesthesias. Support for this approach is drawn from Wittgenstein’s later notebooks.

For many individuals external reality remains to some extent a subjective phenomenon. In the extreme case the individual hallucinates either at certain specific moments, or perhaps in a generalized way. There exist all sorts of expressions for this state (“fey,” “not all there,” “feet off the ground,” “unreal”) and psychiatrically we refer to such individuals as schizoid.... To balance this one would have to state that there are others who are so firmly anchored in objectively perceived reality that they are ill in the opposite direction of being out of touch with the subjective world and with the creative approach to fact.

Schizoid people are not satisfied with themselves any more than are extroverts who cannot get in touch with dream. These two groups of people come to us for psychotherapy because in the one case they do not want to spend their lives irrevocably out of touch with the facts of life, and in the other case because they feel estranged from dream. They have a sense that something is wrong and that there is a dissociation in their personalities, and they would like to be helped to achieve... a state of time-space integration in which there is one self containing everything instead of dissociated elements that exist in compartments, or are scattered around and left lying about.

This gives us our indication for therapeutic procedure—to afford opportunity for formless experience, and for creative impulses, motor and sensory, which are the stuff of playing. And on the basis of playing is built the whole of man’s experiential existence.... This if reflected back, but only if reflected back, becomes part of the organized individual personality, and eventually this in summation makes the individual to be, to be found.... No longer are we either introvert or extrovert. We experience life in the area of transitional phenomena, in the exciting interweave of subjectivity and objective observation, and in an area that is intermediate between the inner reality of the individual and the shared reality of the world that is external to individuals.

D. W. Winnicott, 1971, pp. 64, 66–67

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Ach, N. (1951). Determining tendencies; awareness. In D. Rapaport (Ed.), Organization and pathology of thought (pp. 15–38). New York: Columbia University Press. (Original work published 1905)

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Adams, G. (1923). An experimental study of memory color and related phenomena. American Journal of Psychology, 23, 359–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Angyal, A. (1935). The perceptual basis of somatic delusions in a case of schizophrenia. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 34, 270–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Angyal, A. (1936). The experience of the body-self in schizophrenia. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 35, 1029–1053.

    Google Scholar 

  • Angyal, A. (1937). Disturbances of activity in a case of schizophrenia. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 38, 1047–1054.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnheim, R. (1969). Visual thinking. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnheim, R. (1974). Art and visual perception. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartlett, F. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bion, W. R. (1965). Transformations. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boisen, A. (1962). The exploration of the inner world. New York: Harper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brentano, F. (1960). The distinction between mental and physical phenomena. In R. M. Chisholm (Ed.), Realism and the background of phenomenology (pp. 39–61). Glencoe, IL: Free Press. (Original work published 1874)

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, D., Forte, M., & Dysart, M. (1984). Visual sensitivity and mindfulness meditation. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 58, 775–784.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bühler, K. (1951). On thought connections. In D. Rapaport (Ed.), Organization and pathology of thought (pp. 39–57). New York: Columbia University Press. (Original work published 1908)

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Burnett, N., & Dallenbach, K. (1927). The experience of heat. American Journal of Psychology, 38, 418–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cassirer, E. (1955). The philosophy of symbolic forms (Vol. 2). New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R. (1930). The subjective character of cognition and the presentational development of perception. British Journal of Psychology Monographs, 14, 1–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Celine, L. (1968). Castle to castle. New York: Delacorte.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chang, G. (Ed.). (1963). Teachings of Tibetan yoga. New York: University Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, J. (1966). The early symptoms of schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 112, 225–251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, H. M. (1911). Conscious attitudes. American Journal of Psychology, 22, 214–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deikman, A. (1971). Bimodal consciousness. Archives of General Psychiatry, 25, 481–489.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. (1978). Toward a cognitive theory of consciousness. In C. Savage (Ed.), Perception and cognition issues in the foundations of psychology. Minnesota studies in the philosophy of science (pp. 201–228). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Don, W., & Weld, H. (1924). Lapse of meaning with visual fixation. American Journal of Psychology, 35, 446–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncker, K. (1947). Phenomenology and epistemology of consciousness of objects. Philosophical and Phenomenological Research, 7, 505–541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edelson, M. (1975). Language and interpretation in psychoanalysis. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, R. (1972). E. B. Titchener and his lost system. Journal of the History of Behavioural Sciences, 8, 168–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, R. (1975). Cartography of inner space. In R. Siegel & L. West (Eds.), Hallucinations: Behavior, experience, and theory (pp. 197–239). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J., & Draguns, J. (1957). A microgenetic approach to perception and thought. Psychological Bulletin, 54, 197–217.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foucault, M. (1978). The history of sexuality. New York: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1957). The unconscious. In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), The complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 14, pp. 159–215). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1915)

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1959). The “uncanny.” In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), The complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 17, pp. 217–256). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1919)

    Google Scholar 

  • Gendlin, E. (1962). Experiencing and the creation of meaning. New York: Free Press of Glencoe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geschwind, N. (1965). Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man. Brain, 88, 237–294, 585–644.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. (1963). The useful dimensions of sensitivity. American Psychologist, 18, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Globus, G. G. (1973). Consciousness and brain: II. Introspection, the qualia of experience, and the unconscious. Archives of General Psychiatry, 29, 167–176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1974). Frame analysis. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goleman, D. (1972). The Buddha on meditation and states of consciousness. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 4, 1–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Govinda, A. (1960). Foundations of Tibetan mysticism. New York: Dutton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haber, R. N. (1983). The impending demise of the icon. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 6, 1–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haskell, R. (1984). Empirical structures of mind: Cognition, linguistics, and transformation. Journal of Mind & Behavior, 5, 29–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D., & Thompson, W. (1968). The social significance of animal studies. In G. Lindzey (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 729–774). Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heidegger, M. (1971). On the way to language. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heidegger, M. (1977). The question concerning technology and other essays. New York: Harper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hillman, J. (1978). The therapeutic value of alchemical language. Dragonflies, pp. 33–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofstadter, D. (1979). Gödel, Escher, Bach: An eternal golden braid. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey, G. (1951). Thinking. New York: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, H. T. (1984). A cognitive psychology of mystical and altered-state experience. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 58, 467–513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, H. T. (1985). Cognition and states of consciousness: The necessity for empirical study of ordinary and nonordinary consciousness for contemporary cognitive psychology. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 60, 239–282.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, H. T. (n.d.). [Research protocol]. Unpublished data.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, H. T., & Chefurka, C. M. (1976). A test of the psychedelic model of consciousness. Archives of General Psychiatry, 33, 867–896.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, W. A. (1933). The meaning of pleasantness and unpleasantness. American Journal of Psychology, 45, 345–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1970). The crisis of European sciences and transcendental phenomenology. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson, E. (1911). On meaning and understanding. American Journal of Psychology, 22, 553–577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (Vols. 1 & 2). New York: Dover.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1912). Does consciousness exist? In W. James, Essays in radical empiricism (pp. 1–38). New York: Longmans, Green. (Original work published 1904)

    Google Scholar 

  • Jung, C. G. (1953). Psychology and alchemy. New York: Pantheon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jung, C. G. (1959). A study in the process of individuation. In C. G. Jung, The archetypes and the collective unconscious (pp. 290–354). New York: Pantheon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kakise, H. (1911). A preliminary experimental study of the conscious concomitants of understanding. American Journal of Psychology, 22, 14–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katz, D. (1935). The world of colour. London: Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klüver, H. (1966). Mescal and mechanisms of hallucination. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt psychology. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, F. (1928). The essence of feeling: Outline of a systematic theory. In M. Reymert (Ed.), Feelings and emotions (pp. 58–85). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laski, M. (1961). Ecstasy. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levi-Strauss, C. (1966). The savage mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K. (1936). Principles of topological psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. (1975). Mind and emotion. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcel, A. (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: An approach to the relations between phenomenal experience and perceptual processes. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 238–300.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marks, L. E. (1978). The unity of the senses. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milner, M. (1957). On not being able to paint. New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Minkowski, E. (1970). Lived time. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nafe, J. P. (1924). An experimental study of the affective qualities. American Journal of Psychology, 35, 507–544.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Natsoulas, T. (1981). Basic problems of consciousness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 14, 132–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality. San Francisco, CA: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Otto, R. (1958). The idea of the holy (J. Harvey, Trans.). New York: Galaxy Books. (Original work published 1923)

    Google Scholar 

  • Perky, C. W. (1910). An experimental study of imagination. American Journal of Psychology, 21, 422–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rahn, C. (1913). The relation of sensation to other categories in contemporary psychology: A study in the psychology of thinking. Psychological Review Monographs, 67, 1–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rahn, C. (1928). Science and the religious life: A psycho-physiological approach. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reich, W. (1960). Selected writings. New York: Noonday Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ricoeur, P. (1977). The rule of metaphor. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rycroft, C. (1979). The innocence of dreams. New York: Pantheon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. New York: Barnes & Noble.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sampson, E. (1981). Cognitive psychology as ideology. American Psychologist, 36,730–743.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sander, F. (1930). Structures, totality of experience, and gestalt. In C. Murchison (Ed.), Psychologies of 1930 (pp. 188–204). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sartre, J. P. (1947). A fundamental idea of the phenomenology of Husserl: Intentionality. In J. P. Sartre, Situations I. Paris: Gallinard. (Original work published 1939, J. Mayer, Trans.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Schilder, P. (1933). Experiments on imagination, after images, and hallucinations. American Journal of Psychiatry, 13, 597–611.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schilder, P. (1942). Mind: Perception and thought in their constructive aspects. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schutz, A. (1962). On multiple realities. In A. Schutz, The problems of social reality: Collected papers of Alfred Schutz (Vol. 1, pp. 207–259). The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. (1980). Minds, brains, programs. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 3, 427–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Severance, E., & Washburn, M. (1907). The loss of associative power in words after long fixation. American Journal of Psychology, 18, 182–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shattock, E. (1960). An experiment in mindfulness. New York: Dutton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, D., & Raygor, A. (1956). Verbal satiation and personality. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 323–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, G. (1957). Visual perception: An event over time. Psychological Review, 64, 306–313.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spearman, C. (1923). The nature of intelligence and the principles of cognition. London: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swartz, P. (1983). Concerning the continuous present. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 57,159–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E. B. (1909). Lectures on the experimental psychology of the thought processes. New York: Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E. B. (1912). Description vs. statement of meaning. American Journal of Psychology, 23, 165–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E. B. (1915). Sensation and system. American Journal of Psychology, 26, 258–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E. B. (1929). Systematic psychology: Prolegomena. New York: Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • VanDusen, W. (1972). The natural depth in man. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1962). Thought and language (E. Hanfmann & G. Vakar, Trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Original work published 1934)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H. (1956). Microgenesis and aphasia. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 347–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H. (1961). Comparative psychology of mental development. New York: Science Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winnicott, D. W. (1971). Playing and reality. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1966). Lectures and conversations on aesthetics, psychology and religious belief Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1980). Culture and value. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1980). Remarks on the philosophy of psychology (Vols. 1 & 2). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1982). Last writings on the philosophy of psychology (Vol. 1). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodworth, R. S. (1906). Imageless thought. Journal of Philosophy, Psychology, and Scientific Method, 3, 701–708.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young, P. (1927). Studies in affective psychology. American Journal of Psychology, 38, 157–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zener, K., & Gaffron, M. (1962). Perceptual experience: An analysis of its relation to the external world through internal processings. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. 4, pp. 516–618). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1986 Plenum Press, New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Hunt, H.T. (1986). A Cognitive Reinterpretation of Classical Introspectionism. In: Mos, L.P. (eds) Annals of Theoretical Psychology. Annals of Theoretical Psychology, vol 4. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-6453-9_18

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-6453-9_18

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4615-6455-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4615-6453-9

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics