Skip to main content

Cognitive Approaches to Decision Making

  • Chapter
Behavioral Decision Making

Abstract

Decision making is such a fundamental part of human activity that a satisfactory explanation of how people choose between various courses of action would seem to be a central problem for psychology. Though there has been much research addressing this problem, until recently it has remained comparatively in-dependent of other areas of psychology with little evidence for the exchange of ideas that has occurred between these other areas. In trying to explain this sep-arate development, one may point to the fact that the intellectual tradition informing research in decision making was rather different from the traditions informing other areas of psychology. Until recently, decision theory has provided the dominant perspective and because this has its origins in economics and statistics, it differs from other areas of psychology that are founded on such intellectual traditions as associationism.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beach, L. R., & Mitchell, T. R. (1978). A contingency model for the selection of decision strategies. Academy of Management Review, 3, 439–449.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. R. (1981). Working memory and the developmental analysis of probability judgements. Psychological Review, 88, 463–502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. London: Pergamon.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. T., Lewis, N. A., & Hunt, W. A. (1958). Context effects with judgemental language that is absolute, extensive and extra-experimentally anchored. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 220–228.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chase, W. G. (1978). Elementary information processes. In W. K. Estes (Ed.), Handbook of learning and cognitive processes (Vol. 5 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 55–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, G. (1977;. The psychology of cognition. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawes, R. M. (1979). The robust beauty of improper linear models in decision making. American Psychologist, 34, 571–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch, J. A., & Deutsch, D. (1963). Attention: Some theoretical considerations. Psychological Review, 70, 80–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, J. (1980). The locus of interference in the perception of simultaneous stimuli. Psychological Review, 87, 272–300.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, W. (1954). The theory of decision making. Psychological Bulletin, 51, 380–417.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Einhorn, H. J. (1971). Use of nonlinear, noncompensatory models as a function of task and amount of information. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 6, 1–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Einhorn, H. J., Kleinmuntz, D. N., & Kleinmuntz, B. (1979). Linear regression and process tracing models of judgement. Psychological Review, 86, 465–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Englander, T., & Tyszka, T. (1980). Information seeking in open decision situations. Acta Psychologica, 45, 169–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1980). Verbal reports as data. Psychological Review, 87, 215–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Estes, W. K. (1980). Comments on directions and limitations of current efforts towards theories of decision making. In T. S. Wallsten (Ed.), Cognitive processes in choice and decision behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W. (1982). Attention and arousal. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischhoff, B. (1983). Predicting frames. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 9, 103–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, J. (1980). Making decisions under the influence of memory. Psychological Review, 87, 190–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fryback, D. G., Goodman, B. C., & Edwards, W. (1973). Choices among bets by Las Vegas gamblers: Absolute and contextual effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 98, 271–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haber, R. N. (1974). Information processing. In E. D. Carterette & M. P. Friedman (Eds.), Handbook of perception (Vol 1 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation-level theory. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hitch, G. H. (1978). The role of short-term working memory in mental arithmetic. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 302–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decisions under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klayman, J. (1983). Simulation of six decision strategies: Comparisons of search patterns, processing characteristics and response to task complexity. Center for Decision Research Report, Graduate School of Business. Chicago: University of Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D. (1980). Automatic information processing: A review. In J. Long & A. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and performance (Vol 9 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montgomery, H. (1977). A study of intransitive preference using a thinking aloud procedure. In H. Jungermann & G. de Zeeuw (Eds.), Decision making and change in human affairs. Dordrecht, Holland: Reidel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A. (1973a). You can’t play twenty questions with nature and win. In W. G. Chase (Ed.), Visual information processing. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A. (1973b). Production systems: Models of control structures. In W. G. Chase (Ed.), Visual information processing. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A. (1980). Harpy, production systems and human cognition. In R. Cole (Ed.), Perception and production of fluent speech. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. (1981). Twelve issues for cognitive science. In D. A. Norman (Ed.), Perspectives on cognitive science. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, J. W. (1976). Task complexity and contingent processing in decision making: An information search and protocol analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 16, 366–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Payne, J. W. (1982). Contingent decision behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 382–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Payne, J. W., Braunstein, M. L., & Carroll, J. S. (1978). Exploring pre-decisional behavior: An alternative approach to decision research. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 22, 17–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pitz, G. F. (1977). Decision making and cognition. In H. Jungermann & G. de Zeeuw (Eds.), Decision making and change in human affairs. Dordrecht, Holland: Reidel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & McLeod, P. (1982). Information processing models—In search of elementary operations. Annual Review of Psychology, 33, 477–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, S. K. (1982). Cognition: Theory and applications. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E. (1977). Toward an interactive model of reading. In S. Dornic (Ed.), Attention and performance (Vol. 6 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russo, J. E., & Dosher, B. A. (1983). Strategies for multi-attribute binary choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 9, 676–696.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, A. F. (1980). State analysis of reaction processes. In E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior. Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R. M. (1976). Capacity limitations in information processing, attention and memory. In W. K. Estes (Ed.), Handbook of learning and cognitive processes (Vol. 4 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R. M., & Dumais, S. T. (1981). The development of automatism. In J. R. Anderson (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shugan, S. M. (1980). The cost of thinking. Journal of Consumer Research, 7, 99–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1955). Behavioral model of rational choice. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 69, 99–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1979). Information processing models of cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 30, 363–396.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A., & Hayes, J. R. (1976). The understanding process: Problem isomorphs. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 165–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slovic, P., Fischhoff, B., & Lichtenstein, S. (1982). Response mode, framing and information processing effects in risk assessment. In R. M. Hogarth (Ed.), New directions for methodology of social and behavioral science: Question framing and response consistency (Vol 11 ). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1977). Intelligence, information processing, and analogical reasoning: The componential analysis of human abilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: Extension of Donder’s method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A. (1969). Intransitivity of preferences. Psychological Review, 76, 31–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, P. (1975). The harassed decision maker: Time pressure, distraction, and the use of evidence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 59, 555–561.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1985 Plenum Press, New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Maule, A.J. (1985). Cognitive Approaches to Decision Making. In: Wright, G. (eds) Behavioral Decision Making. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-2391-4_4

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-2391-4_4

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4612-9460-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4613-2391-4

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics