Introduction

Autism is a developmental disability that affects how a person communicates with and relates to other people, as well as how that person experiences the world around them (National Autistic Society 2018). Autism and related disorders comprise the autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Children with autism/ASD have varying levels of difficulties with social behavior and communication, have limited interests, and engage in restricted and repetitive behaviors. The course of ASD symptoms appears to be lifelong for the majority of cases. These challenges with social communication, behavior, and social interaction often co-occur with other disorders such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, intellectual disorder, and epilepsy, making it difficult for many persons with autism/ASD to live independently (Matson and Kozlowski 2011). ASD is, therefore, considered a complex developmental disability.

Prevalence studies in many countries during previous decades (Elsabbagh et al. 2012; Özerk 2016) show that there has been a considerable rise in the prevalence rates for autism/ASD around the globe. Given these increases, educational and health care authorities need to be informed about the prevalence of autism/ASD in their jurisdictions. Such data is essential for policy developers at the local and national levels to properly plan for medical, psychological, behavioral, and educational interventions (Cardinal and Fraumeni-McBride 2017; Isaksen et al. 2012; Özerk 2014; Williams et al. 2008). Research shows that early intensive behavioral interventions based on evidence-based practices for young children with autism/ASD can enhance developmental outcomes (Eikeseth et al. 2007; Eldevik et al. 2009; Warren et al. 2011; Wong et al., 2015).

International Prevalence of Autism/ASD

Studies in different areas of the USA during the period 2000–2016 showed a considerable increase in prevalence rates for autism/ASD among 8-year-old children. In 2000, the prevalence rate among 8-year-olds was 1 in 150. In 2012, the estimated prevalence of autism/ASD among 8-year-olds increased dramatically to 1 in 68 (Blumberg et al. 2013; Christiansen et al. 2016; Zablotsky et al. 2015), then 1 in 59 in 2014 (Baio et al. 2018). The prevalence increased again in 2016 to 1 in 54 in 2016 (Maenner et al., 2020). Furthermore, there are considerable geographical differences in prevalence rates among states and even among different counties or regions in the same state or county (Elsabbagh et al. 2012; Özerk 2016; Baio et al. 2018; Maenner et al. 2020).

Studies of the prevalence of autism/ASD in different countries (see Table 1) illustrate apparent differences in prevalence rates. This can be attributed to differences in data gathered from different age groups and different geographical areas and using varying research methodologies, differences in accessibility to expertise, and more. Sometimes, the differences are due to inclusion criteria (in some studies, all of the disorders under spectrum (ICD-10: F84.0–F84.9) are included; in other studies, only one of the subcategories of pervasive developmental disorders (F84.0 Childhood autism) is included) (Özerk 2016). Even with these factors in mind, the overall trend in the studies conducted during the last two decades is that the prevalence of autism/ASD is increasing among children and young adults globally.

Table 1 International studies on prevalence of autism/ASD

Table 1 shows representative prevalence studies from different countries.

In addition to American studies, the other studies presented in Table 1 reveal high rates of prevalence of autism/ASD—particularly the Israeli study by Davidovitch et al. (2013), the study by Roelfsema et al. (2012) in the Netherlands, and three Swedish studies (Gilberg et al. 2006; Idring et al. 2012; Sandin et al. 2014). These studies each show higher rates of prevalence in their respective countries than earlier studies (Elsabbagh et al. 2012; Özerk 2016). As we will discuss in the following sections, a comparison of prevalence among preschool-age children versus school-age children will highlight a school-age effect on the prevalence of autism/ASD.

School-age Effect on Prevalence of Autism/ASD

We use the term school-age effect to denote the higher prevalence rates among school-age children compared with preschool-age children. As Table 1 shows, the study conducted by Green et al. (2004) in the UK presents that the prevalence rate of autism/ASD among 5- to 16-year-old British children is quite high. Specifically, 1 in 111 school-age children has received a diagnosis of autism/ASD. The highest prevalence rate is found among the subgroup of schoolchildren of 5 to 10 years of age. In this group, 1 in 100 children was diagnosed with autism/ASD. In two British regional studies (Baird et al. 2006; Baron-Cohen et al. 2009), the rate of autism/ASD among school-age children (5 to 9 years old) is higher than that in preschool-age children (1 to 5 years old). A similar trend can also be seen in Japanese studies (Honda et al. 2005; Kawamura et al. 2008). The Australian study by Williams et al. (2008) also shows what we call the school-age effect, while the prevalence rate of autism/ASD among preschoolers (0 to 5 years old) is 1 in 455, the rate increases considerably to 1 in 160 among school-age children (6- to 12-year-olds). A similar ascending trend is seen in the Canadian longitudinal study (Ouellette-Kuntz et al. 2012). In 2003, the rate was 1 in 300 among preschoolers (2 to 4 years old) and 1 in 189 among school-age children (5 to 14 years old). In 2008, the rate of autism/ASD increased to 1 in 159 among preschoolers and to 1 in 101 among school-age children. The Danish nationwide study (Parner et al. 2008) of prevalence rates shows a rise among school beginners (5.9 years old and 5.8 years old) compared with younger children (5.3 years old). As one can see in Table 1, there is a 57% increase in the prevalence rate among Danish school-age children (6 to 11 years old) compared with preschoolers (3 to 5 years old): from 1 in 621 to 1 in 395.

Prevalence of Autism/ASD and Gender Differences

A comprehensive meta-analysis of 54 prevalence studies conducted by Loomes et al. (2017) mentions the well-known male-to-female ratio of 4:1, though they claim that the gender gap is less than this. One of the arguments in their study is that the characteristics of autism in males differ from autism characteristics in females. Compared with males, the likelihood of females with autism showing “limited interests” is less; thus, females are often underrepresented in prevalence rates. Another factor is that females appear to be better at camouflaging their autism-related difficulties, which makes their diagnosis more difficult. A third factor potentially contributing to what the researchers call the diagnostic bias is that the key people involved in children’s lives, including parents, referral (parents, teachers, school counselors, and school psychologists), and other professionals, have preconceived notions that autism/ASD is a male disorder, thereby reducing their sensitivity to autism symptoms in females.

Finally, Loomes et al. (2017) further argue that a disproportionately high number of females who exhibit autistic traits when using different observational instruments do not meet the autism/ASD criteria in DSM or ICD. They argue that this is because the criteria lack sensitivity to the autistic traits found particularly in females, such as social communication difficulties, sensory-related difficulties, and difficulties with flexibility. Nevertheless, Loomes et al. (2017) point to several studies where the “gender gap” becomes narrower and claim that “Of children meeting criteria for ASD, the true male-to-female ratio is not 4:1, as is often assumed; rather, it is closer to 3:1” (p. 446).

Purpose of the Study

During the 1980s and 1990s, 1 in 1000 children received a diagnosis of autism/ASD in Norway (Gundersen and Hem 2005). Another nationwide prevalence study among birth to 10-year-old Norwegian children showed that 1 in 166 had a diagnosis of autism/ASD in 2010 (Stoltenberg et al. 2010) and 1 in 125 children, age 11, were diagnosed with autism/ASD not long after (Surén et al. 2012). However, none of these earlier Norwegian studies highlighted the prevalence rates separately among preschoolers compared with all school-age children.

The purpose of the present study is to provide new data on the prevalence of autism/ASD among preschool and school-age children in Norway in 2014 and 2016 and to discuss the educational consequences of the increasing prevalence rates of autism/ASD in the country. Our data include the total number of children at ages 1–16 who had received the diagnosis of autism/ASD in 2014 and 2016.

Method and Data Sources

The present study covers the prevalence of autism/ASD throughout Norway, among 1- to 16-year-old children and subgroups of 1- to 5-year-old and 6- to-16-year-old children who had a diagnosis of autism/ASD in 2014 and 2016. More specifically, our data includes those who were diagnosed with childhood autism (ICD-10's F84.0) and Asperger syndrome (ICD-10’s F84.5) in 2014 and 2016. We purposefully did not include other subcategories of pervasive developmental disorders such as atypical autism, Rett’s syndrome, other childhood disintegrative disorders, and other pervasive developmental disorders. This was done to ensure that the data focused on those with clear diagnoses of autism and Asperger syndrome rather than related disorders.

Norway’s universal health care services are provided free of charge, and the country has a well-developed Norwegian Patient Registry (NPR). This paper draws on the data provided by the NPR (NPR, Data Document Dated 15.06.17, Nr. 17/2362-9), Norwegian Statistics (2014, 2016), and several local educational-psychological services. Children in Norway who are suspected of having autism/ASD are referred by family physicians (medical doctors) or educational-psychological services to child pediatric and specialist services. The children then undergo a thorough assessment by a multidisciplinary team and are assigned a final diagnosis by a child psychiatrist based on the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10).

Both the use of ICD-10 for diagnoses and registration with the Norwegian National Patient Registry (NPR) at the Royal Department of Health are mandatory in Norway. The NPR contains nationwide data about diagnoses assigned by Norwegian specialist health services located in public hospitals and outpatient clinics. In addition to these public institutions, some private practicing pediatricians and child psychiatrists have agreements with the government and receive public funding to provide health care services and receive reimbursement. Both public and private health care service providers are obliged to report every case with their 11-digit personal identification number to the Norwegian Patient Registry. Therefore, the likelihood of having missing data is extremely low, even in a country with a total population of about 5.3 million (Norwegian Statistics 2016).

The raw data for this study were drawn from the NPR and included children up to 16 years of age who were registered as having a diagnosis of autism/ASD in 2014 and 2016 based on ICD-10’s F84.0 Childhood autism and F84.5 Asperger syndrome criteria, as well as the child’s age, gender, and county of residence. There are 19 counties in Norway. In each county, there are several different municipalities.

Systematizing the Raw Data and Data Analysis

As a rule, the NPR did not provide the number of children with autism/ASD in any age group in an administrative area (i.e., county or municipality) if the number of children was fewer than 5 but higher than 0. In contrast, if no children were diagnosed with autism/ASD in any age group in a county or municipality in a given year, the row data from the NPR is noted as 0. And if the number of children with a diagnosis of autism/ASD in a county or municipality in an age group in a given year is 5, the row data notes this number. This means that if the number of children in an age group who received a diagnosis of autism/ASD in that county or municipality is 1, 2, 3, or 4, the row data does not note any of these numbers as a means to preserve anonymity. For 2014 and 2016, there was no missing data.

In the NPR row data, the age groups are 0–5 and 6–16 years. When we calculated the total number of children in the first group, we excluded those who were younger than 1 year of age in the total population because no child receives a diagnosis of autism/ASD at so young an age.

We calculated the prevalence rates of autism/ASD by dividing the total number of children in each age group with the number of children with autism/ASD in that age group. We obtained the total number of children in each of the two age groups (1 to 5 years old group, called preschool-age children, and 6 to 16 years old group, called school-age children) from the Norwegian Statistics Norway (2014, 2015, 2016, 2017) for the respective year. Our aim in asking about row data for children 6 to 16 years of age was to determine the number of children who had a diagnosis of autism/ASD at what in Norway is referred to as basic school age.

Basic school is compulsory in Norway; it starts at the age of 6 and lasts 10 years. To ensure that the category “6–16” includes the exact number of children at the 10-year basic school, we used the education statistics from Norwegian Statistics Norway (2013, 2014, 2015, 2016, 2017) for children who were 6–15 years of age. We then compared these statistics with the total number of children in the municipalities in each of the 19 counties in the country. This was necessary because children who were registered as 15 years old at the beginning of 10th grade, the last grade in basic school, become 16 years old at the end of that academic year. By operating in this manner, we had, with almost 100% certainty, the exact number of school-age children for 2014 and 2016 as the official educational statistics showed for those years. The data were systematized, coded in, and analyzed using IBM SPSS, Version 24.0 for Windows, and Excel 2016.

Results

Table 2 presents the overall results for the nationwide prevalence of autism/ASD among 1- to 16-year-old children in Norway. The table shows that 2748 children between 1 and 16 years old in 2014 and 3157 children in the same age group in 2016 had autism/ASD (ICD-10’s F84.0 Childhood autism and F84.5 Asperger syndrome). As seen in the table, the prevalence rate of autism/ASD among 1- to 16-year-olds nationwide was 1 in 340 in 2014, and 1 in 297 in 2016. This indicates a 14.9% increase in the prevalence rates from 2014 to 2016. Similar to the international trend, more males than females received a diagnosis of autism/ASD. Specifically, 1 in 220 males in 2014 and 1 in 192 males in 2016 had autism/ASD. This is a 14.8% increase for males from 2014 to 2016.

Table 2 Prevalence of autism/ASD among 1- to 16-year-old males and females

The increase in the prevalence rates for females is 15.2% for the same period. In 2014, 1 in 797 females had autism/ASD, while, in 2016, this rate rose to 1 to 695. The gender ratio (male to female) was 3.80:1 in 2014 and 3.79:1 in 2016. This means that the male-to-female ratio was almost unchanged from 2014 to 2016 for the 1- to 16-year-old group as a whole. These results show that the gender gap remains the same among 1- to 16-year-old children with autism/ASD in Norway. Compared with the international 4:1 male-to-female ratio, however, the gender gap is slightly narrower for 1- to 16-year-olds in Norway.

Prevalence of Autism/ASD Among Preschool-age Compared with School-age Children

Table 3 also illustrates the prevalence of autism/ASD among males and females at preschool-age (1 to 5 years old) and school-age (6 to 16 years old) children in 2016 compared with that in 2014. The results indicate that 377 of 1- to 5-year-old Norwegian children, out of 316,409 children in this age group, had autism/ASD in 2014. The prevalence rate is 1 in 839 children at 1–5 years of age in 2014. In 2016, 551 children out of 310,725 at 1–5 years of age had autism/ASD. The prevalence rate was, therefore, 1 in 564 in 2016. These figures show that the prevalence rate of autism/ASD among preschool-age children from 2014 to 2016 increased by 48.7%—a notable increase.

In 2014, 2371 school-age children (6–16-year-olds), out of 618,117 in this age group, had autism/ASD, whereas, in 2016, out of 628,122 school-age children, 2606 had autism/ASD. The prevalence rate for autism/ASD among school-age children was 1 in 261 in 2014 and 1 in 241 in 2016. This shows that the prevalence rate of autism/ASF among school-age children increased by 8.3 from 2014 to 2016 (Table 4).

Table 3 Prevalence of autism/ASD among males and females in preschool age and school age

Gender and Prevalence of Autism/ASD

Table 4 shows the prevalence of autism/ASD and the increase in the prevalence rates during a 2-year period among different age groups and genders. For the nationwide 1- to 16-year-old population, as noted earlier, the rates increased 14.5% from 2014 to 2016. For males, it was 14.6%; for females, 14.7%.

Table 4 Increase in the prevalence rates of autism/ASD for the two age groups (2014 to 2016)

The results also show that 1 in 839 Norwegian preschool children within the ages of 1–5 years old had autism/ASD in 2014. In 2016, the rates for autism/ASD among preschoolers were 1 in 564. The prevalence rate for autism/ASD among preschool-age children from 2014 to 2016 increased by 48.7%. In 2014, the prevalence rate of autism/ASD among school-age children was 1 in 261, and in 2016, 1 in 241. This indicates an 8.3% increase in the prevalence rate of autism/ASD among school-age children from 2014 to 2016.

As seen in Table 4, there is a gender difference in the prevalence rates for autism/ASD among preschool-age and school-age children. Among preschool-age males, 1 in 523 had autism/ASD in 2014 and, in 2016, 1 in 349, indicating a 49.8% increase. In contrast, 1 in 2331 preschool-age females had autism/ASD in 2014. In 2016, the prevalence rate was 1 in 1594, indicating an increase of 46.2%. The results also show that the male-to-female ratio for autism/ASD at preschool age was 4.46:1 in 2014 and 4.57:1 in 2016. More preschool-age males and more preschool-age females had autism/ASD in 2016 compared with 2014. Furthermore, when we look at the gender gap more closely, we see that among preschool children, it is slightly widening.

Among school-age (6 to 16 years old) males, 1 in 169 had autism/ASD in 2014. In 2016, the prevalence rate became 1 in 157, indicating a 7.6% increase for males. For school-age (6 to 16 years old) females, the prevalence rate of autism/ASD is 1 in 597 for 2014, and 1 in 544 for 2016, showing a 9.7% increase in prevalence. The male-to-female ratio is 3.53:1 for 2014 and 3.46:1 for 2016. Interestingly, we see an opposite tendency here compared with preschool-age children, with the gender gap narrowing for school-age children.

School-age Effect for Prevalence of Autism/ASD

As noted, the results that show an increase in the prevalence rates of autism/ASD among all of the age groups and genders from 2014 to 2016 are not based on prevalence estimates but the actual number of officially registered children with autism/ASD in 2014 and 2016. Table 5 compares the results between the prevalence rates of autism/ASD among school-age (6 to 16 years old) children and preschool-age (1 to 5 years old) children. These results show what we can call a school-age effect in terms of the increase in prevalence rates.

Table 5 School-age effect for the prevalence rates of autism/ASD in 2014 and 2016

The results show that there is a likelihood that, for every preschool-age child with autism/ASD, 3.21 school-age children had a diagnosis of autism/ASD in 2014. This is equivalent to a 221% rise in prevalence rates of autism/ASD diagnoses among school-age children compared with preschool-age children in 2014. Concerning gender differences, we see that for every male of preschool age, 3.09 males of school age were diagnosed with autism/ASD in 2014. This indicates a 209% increase. For females, the increase is even more significant. For every female with a diagnosis of autism/ASD at preschool age, 3.90 females of school age were diagnosed with autism/ASD in 2014. This means that the prevalence rates for the school-age females were 290% greater than the prevalence rates for the preschool-age females in 2014.

Two years later, in 2016, the trend can be seen as similar: The likelihood of receiving a diagnosis of autism/ASD at school age compared with preschool age is higher. For every preschool-age child with autism/ASD in 2016, 2.34 school-age children had autism/ASD. This means that 134% more school-age children had autism/ASD compared with preschool-age children. When gender differences are considered, again, we see a school-age effect. For every male of preschool age with autism/ASD, 2.22 males of school age had autism/ASD. This means that 122% more males at school age had autism/ASD compared with the males of preschool age. In contrast, for every female of preschool age with autism/ASD, there were 2.93 females of school-age autism/ASD, indicating 193% higher rates for school-age females compared with preschool-age females in 2016.

These results reveal an apparent school-age effect in terms of the increase in prevalence rates of autism/ASD. A higher rate of school-age children had autism/ASD. While more males overall had autism/ASD—both in general and at school age in particular—from 2014 to 2016, an increasing number of females have been receiving a diagnosis of autism/ASD. This makes the school-age effect more visible among females compared with males. This trend also means that the gender gap in prevalence among school-age children is narrowing.

The school-age effect and narrowing gender gap among school-age children with autism/ASD from 2014 to 2016 can be seen even more clearly in Table 6. The table provides a comparison of the prevalence rates of autism/ASD for males and females at preschool age in 2014 with the prevalence rates of autism/ASD among males and females at school age in 2016.

Table 6 School-age effect for the prevalence of autism/ASD and the gender gap in 2014 and 2016

The table shows that the likelihood of being a school-age child with autism/ASD in 2016 is 3.48 times higher than that of children of preschool age in 2014. Furthermore, for every preschool-age male in 2014, 3.33 times more school-age males in 2016 had autism/ASD. For every preschool-age female in 2014, 4.28 times more school-age females in 2016 had autism/ASD. Most startling, however, is that the prevalence rate of autism/ASD among school-age children in 2016 is 248% greater than the prevalence rate among preschool-age children in 2014. Specifically, 233% more males and 328% more females of school age had autism/ASD in 2016 compared with the males and females of preschool age in 2014.

These increases illuminate a school-age effect on the prevalence of autism/ASD. As noted, more males and females had autism/ASD in 2016 compared with 2014, and, as seen in Table 5, the gender difference among preschool-age children did not narrow from 2014 to 2016. The increase for preschool-age males with autism/ASD is 49.8% from 2014 to 2016, while it is 46.2% for the preschool-age females. Nevertheless, the gender gap narrowed among school-age children with autism from 2014 to 2016. For every school-age male with autism/ASD in 2014, there were 1.08 school-age males with the same diagnosis in 2016, for an increase of 7.6%. In contrast, for every school-age female in 2014, 1.10 females in 2016 had /ASD. This shows an increase of 9.7% among school-age females compared with 7.6% among school-age males, indicating a narrowing gender gap in this age group.

Overall, there was an increase in prevalence rates in terms of both age groups and genders. Figure 1 illustrates the increase in the prevalence rates from 2014 to 2016.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Increase in prevalence rates of autism/ASD among boys and girls in 2014 and 2016

Discussion

Diagnoses of autism/ASD are based on ICD-10 in Norway. Also, all incidences of autism/ASD are registered in the Norwegian Patient Registry noting gender and an 11-digit ID number. The current data on the prevalence of autism/ASD in Norway includes 1- to 16-year-old children in 2014 and 2016. The overall results suggest that there is a higher risk of receiving a diagnosis of autism/ASD at school age than at preschool age. There may be several interrelated factors that account for this finding. Here, it is also important to mention that 92% of 1- to 5-year-old preschool-age children in Norway are enrolled in kindergartens (Statistics Norway 2017). Whether a kindergarten is public or private, it is state-subsidized in the country. All 1- to 5-year-old preschoolers are under the responsibility of educational-psychological services and public health care services, and they go through several compulsory health controls free of charge. Thus, a referral to a specialist/pediatrician is not an economic burden for any family. A typical Norwegian kindergarten for 1- to 5-year-old preschoolers is predominantly play-oriented. It is worth noting the extended time for free play at the kindergartens because the symptoms and consequences of autism/ASD at this age may be easier to tolerate during play.

Furthermore, for children of this age, adults tend to take a more “wait and see” attitude compared to their views of school-age children. It is certainly possible as well that since the onset and diagnosis of autism/ASD are skewed toward the middle or even higher age range of preschool children (from among those ages 1–5), the school-age effect could be partially due to some children within the preschool group simply being unlikely candidates for such a diagnosis given their young age. As mentioned, the data used in this study from the Norwegian Patient Registry does not differentiate preschool data by individual age, only delineating the entire 1- to 5-year-old range, thus not permitting an accessible analysis of the specific impact that this data format may have on our school-age effect findings.

School settings may also impact the school-age effect. Being of school-age means that children participate in more structured as well as more demanding social and academic activities, with social communication, group work, teamwork, and collaboration playing essential roles in school life. There is less tolerance for unstructured and individualistic initiatives or free play in the school-age settings. School rules include sitting behind a desk, listening to the teacher, compliance with the classroom rules, turn-taking, collective projects, class activities, and group and teamwork activities, with a sharper academic focus and cognitively demanding school language and textbooks. Also, it may be easier for a teacher at this level to compare students who are sitting in a classroom together. Thus, we can speculate that the school-age effect of increasing prevalence rates may be due to the factors embedded in the school setting itself. This array of school-related factors may lead to a higher degree of detection, referral, and diagnosis and, therefore, a significant increase in prevalence rates of autism/ASD among school-age children compared with preschool-age children.

One limitation of this study is that the data did not allow for separation of “school beginners” (6- to 7-year-olds) for comparison with the rest of the school-age children to determine whether school age, in general, or school-beginning age, in particular, accounts for the increase in prevalence rates of autism/ASD among 6- to 16-year-old children. Further research is needed for a more detailed study of subgroups among school-age children. More research is also needed with regard to gender-related issues in the assessment of females. Nevertheless, the findings of this study of the prevalence of autism/ASD among preschool (1 to 5 years old) and school-age (6–16 years old) Norwegian children are informative for international researchers and essential for policymakers in the health, social, and educational sectors. Planning for appropriate services for treatment, training, and teaching that will serve an increased number of children with autism/ASD at school age is essential. Reliable prevalence rates for children at different age groups can make this type of planning easier, ensuring proper accommodations for and provision of equitable services for children with autism/ASD.

There is a substantial amount of research (Odom et al. 2013; Klintwall and Eikeseth 2014; Koegel et al. 2014; Wong et al., 2015, 2015; Steinbrenner et al. 2020) detailing 28 evidence-based practices in the autism field (e.g., Discrete Trial Teaching, Pivotal Response Training, Video Modeling, Cognitive Behavioral Intervention, PECS and Social Narratives). Implementing evidence-based practices can make a difference in the life of children with autism/ASD. Thus, the findings of this study also show the clear need for national initiatives in Norway focused on improving ASD-related competence among teachers, preschool teachers, special education teachers, educational psychologists, counselors, speech pathologists, and school leaders. For this purpose, we suggest designing credential programs or specially designed certification courses at universities, focusing on evidence-based practices and recent research findings in the field of ASD for these professionals who work directly with children with autism/ASD.

Such improvements in capacity-building in Norway’s kindergartens and schools are of the utmost importance for enhancing the quality of children’s treatment, as well as training, teaching, and learning conditions.