Skip to main content
Log in

The Symbolic Benefits of Descriptive and Substantive Representation

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Political Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

One of the major challenges in providing quality representation comes from the desire to balance the will of the majority with the needs of political minorities. Of particular importance is whether substantive or descriptive representation are necessary to create symbolic representation and perceptions that government outcomes are fair and legitimate. In this paper, we employ a novel experimental design to investigate how citizens feel about the relative importance of descriptive and substantive representation. Drawing on data from a nationally representative sample and two supplemental experiments, we show that citizens value descriptive representation independently of substantive representation. We also demonstrate that the degree of descriptive representation desired is conditional on the nature of the policy being considered.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For notable exceptions, see Tate (2003) and Casellas and Wallace 2015).

  2. This literature traces its roots to the work of Thibaut and Walker (1975)

  3. A finding supported with regard to courts by Scherer and Curry (2010), although their study did not take into account whether this effect would be independent of substantive outcomes.

  4. It is possible that subjects’ evaluations of these rates of descriptive representation depend on the type of community they are thinking of. For example, those living in diverse communities might find 5 black representatives too low, whereas those living in homogeneous white communities might find 5 black representatives too high. To investigate this, we also ran an identical treatment on an MTurk sample that allowed us to identify subjects’ zip codes. Including either actual or perceived percent black in the community as covariates had no effect on our substantive findings.

  5. All data and replication code for this paper can be accessed at http://dx.doi.org/10.7910/DVN/JWXMFE.

  6. There were 23 respondents who self-identified as “Mixed Race,” but they were not required to identify what combination of races they identify as. These respondents were excluded from analysis.

  7. These three measures are all highly correlated and the index has a standardized \(\alpha\) of 0.89 for Study 1, 0.86 for Study 2, and 0.91 for study 3. The index was re-scaled to run from 0 (least fair) to 1 (most fair). Descriptive statistics on the dependent variables can be found in the Appendix (Supplementary material). The substantive results are unchanged when we run the analyses on each dependent variable separately.

  8. Since subjects are randomly assigned to conditions and we have good balance on demographic items across treatment conditions, we are not particularly concerned about potential confounds. To investigate other possible predictors of perceptions of fairness, we also re-ran the results adjusting for several demographic and political covariates. Higher levels of education were associated with higher perceptions of fairness. Being female, black, and possessing a more liberal ideology were associated with lower perceptions of fairness.

  9. Due to the relatively small number of African Americans in our sample, we report 90 % confidence intervals throughout for both white and black subjects. Given the strikingly similar results we find in each of our studies, we are confident that our results are not a statistical artifact due to the relatively small number of African Americans in any one sample. Across all of our samples, the total number of African Americans is 287.

  10. Amazon’s Mechanical Turk is an online system through which workers can complete short tasks for compensation. It has received increasing attention as a source of subjects for social science researchers. For a discussion of the representativeness and validity of MTurk as a data source, see Buhrmester et al. (2011) and Berinsky et al. (2012).

  11. See appendix (Supplementary material) for detailed comparison of CCAP and MTurk data.

References

  • Abney, F. G., & Hutcheson, J. D. (1981). Race, representation, and trust: Changes in attitudes after the election of a black mayor. Public Opinion Quarterly, 45(1), 91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atkeson, L. R. (2003). Not all cues are created equal: The conditional impact of female candidates on political engagement. Journal of Politics, 65(4), 1040–1061.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atkeson, L. R., & Carrillo, N. (2007). More is better: The influence of collective female descriptive representation on external efficacy. Politics and Gender, 3(1), 79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, A., & Cook, C. (2005). Representing black interests and promoting black culture: The importance of African American descriptive representation in the US House. Du Bois Review, 2(02), 227–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Banducci, S. A., Donovan, T., & Karp, J. A. (2004). Minority representation, empowerment, and participation. Journal of Politics, 66(2), 534–556.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, T. D. & Burchard, S. M. (2012). Engendering politics the impact of descriptive representation on womens political engagement in sub-Saharan Africa. Comparative Political Studies, 0010414012463884.

  • Berinsky, A. J., Huber, G. A., & Lenz, G. S. (2012). Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: Amazon.com’s mechanical turk. Political Analysis, 20(3), 351–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bobo, L., & Gilliam, F. D, Jr. (1990). Race, sociopolitical participation, and black empowerment. The American Political Science Review, 84, 377–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s mechanical turk a new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(1), 3–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cameron, C., Epstein, D., & O’Halloran, S. (1996). Do majority-minority districts maximize substantive black representation in congress? American Political Science Review, 90, 794–812.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. E., & Wolbrecht, C. (2006). See Jane run: Women politicians as role models for adolescents. Journal of Politics, 68(2), 233–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casellas, J. P., & Wallace, S. J. (2015). The role of race, ethnicity, and party on attitudes toward descriptive representation. American Politics Research, 43, 144–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cerulo, K. A. (1993). Symbols and the world system: National anthems and flags. Sociological Forum, 8(2), 243–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, M. C. (1995). Behind the mule: Race and class in African-American politics. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Desposato, S., & Norrander, B. (2009). The gender gap in Latin America: Contextual and individual influences on gender and political participation. British Journal of Political Science, 39(01), 141–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Easton, D. (1975). A re-assessment of the concept of political support. British Journal of Political Science, 5(4), 435–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, R. S. (1978). Constituency opinion and congressional behavior: A reexamination of the Miller-Stokes representation data. American Journal of Political Science, 511–535.

  • Gay, C. (2001). The effect of black congressional representation on political participation. American Political Science Review, 95, 589–602.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gay, C. (2002). Spirals of trust? The effect of descriptive representation on the relationship between citizens and their government. American Journal of Political Science, 717–732.

  • Grose, C. R. (2011). Congress in black and white. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Guinier, L. (1995). The tyranny of the majority: Fundamental fairness in representative democracy. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hansen, S. B. (1997). Talking about politics: Gender and contextual effects on political proselytizing. The Journal of Politics, 59(01), 73–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hero, R. E. & Tolbert, C. J. (1995). Latinos and substantive representation in the US House of Representatives: Direct, indirect, or nonexistent? American Journal of Political Science, 640–652.

  • Hibbing, J. R., & Theiss-Morse, E. (2002). Stealth democracy: Americans’ beliefs about how government should work. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Koch, J. (1997). Candidate gender and women’s psychological engagement in politics. American Politics Quarterly, 25(1), 118–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuklinski, J. H. (1978). Representativeness and elections: A policy analysis. The American Political Science Review, 165–177.

  • Lawless, J. L. (2004). Politics of presence? Congresswomen and symbolic representation. Political Research Quarterly, 57(1), 81–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lombardo, E., & Meier, P. (2014). The symbolic representation of gender: A discursive approach. Farnham: Ashgate Publishing Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mansbridge, J. (1999). Should blacks represent blacks and women represent women? A contingent yes. The Journal of Politics, 61(03), 628–657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCrone, D. J. & Kuklinski, J. H. (1979). The delegate theory of representation. American Journal of Political Science, 278–300.

  • Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1963). Constituency influence in Congress. The American Political Science Review, 57(1), 45–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Overby, L. M., Brown, R. D., Bruce, J. M., Smith, C. E, Jr., & Winkle, J. W, III. (2005). Race, political empowerment, and minority perceptions of judicial fairness. Social Science Quarterly, 86(2), 444–462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Page, B. I., Shapiro, R. Y., Gronke, P. W., & Rosenberg, R. M. (1984). Constituency, party, and representation in Congress. Public Opinion Quarterly, 48(4), 741–756.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pantoja, A. D., & Segura, G. M. (2003). Does ethnicity matter? Descriptive representation in legislatures and political alienation among latinos. Social Science Quarterly, 84(2), 441–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pitkin, H. F. (1967). The concept of representation. California: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rocha, R. R., Tolbert, C. J., Bowen, D. C., & Clark, C. J. (2010). Race and turnout: Does descriptive representation in state legislatures increase minority voting? Political Research Quarterly, 63(4), 890–907.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, N., & Curry, B. (2010). Does descriptive race representation enhance institutional legitimacy? The case of the US courts. Journal of Politics, 72, 90–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwindt-Bayer, L. A. (2010). Political power and womens representation in Latin America. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Schwindt-Bayer, L. A., & Mishler, W. (2005). An integrated model of women’s representation. Journal of Politics, 67(2), 407–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tate, K. (2001). The political representation of blacks in Congress: Does race matter? Legislative Studies Quarterly, 623–638.

  • Tate, K. (2003). Black faces in the mirror: African Americans and their representatives in the US Congress. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thibaut, J. W., Walker, L., et al. (1975). Procedural justice: A psychological analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T. R. (1990). Why people obey the law: Procedural justice, legitimacy, and compliance. New Haven: Yale University press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, T. R. (1988). What is procedural justice-criteria used by citizens to assess the fairness of legal procedures. Law and Society Review, 22, 103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ulbig, S. G. (2007). Gendering municipal government: Female descriptive representation and feelings of political trust. Social Science Quarterly, 88(5), 1106–1123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weissberg, R. (1978). Collective vs. dyadic representation in Congress. The American Political Science Review, 535–547.

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Bill Bianco, Eileen Braman, Christopher DeSante, Bernard Fraga, Brian Gaines, Jim Kuklinski, Jeff Mondak, Diana Z. O’Brien, and Cara Wong for guidance and feedback on earlier drafts. We would also like to thank attendees at Vanderbilt’s Experimental Methods Speaker Series as well as anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Matthew Hayes.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (pdf 145 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hayes, M., Hibbing, M.V. The Symbolic Benefits of Descriptive and Substantive Representation. Polit Behav 39, 31–50 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-016-9345-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-016-9345-9

Keywords

Navigation