Skip to main content
Log in

The Brain’s Reward Response Occurs Even Without Actual Reward!

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Gambling Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

What if the brain’s response to reward occurs even when there is no reward? Wouldn’t that be a further concern for people prone to problem gambling and other forms of addiction, like those related to eating? Electroencephalography was employed to investigate this possibility using probabilistic feedback manipulations and measures of known event-related potentials (ERPs) related to reward processing. We tested the hypothesis—that reward-based ERPs would occur even in the absence of a tangible reward and when manipulations on expectation are implicit. The well-known P300 response potential was a key focus, and was assessed in non-gambling volunteer undergraduates on a task involving experimentally-manipulated probabilities of positive or negative feedback comprising three trial types—80, 50, or 20% positive feedback. A feedback stimulus (F1) followed a guess response between two possible outcomes (implicit win/loss), and then a second feedback stimulus (F2) was presented to confirm an alleged ‘win’ or ‘loss’ (explicit win/loss). Results revealed that amplitude of the P300 in F1-locked data (implicit manipulation) was larger (more positive) on average for feedback outcomes that were manipulated to be less likely than expected. The effect is pronounced after increased time on task (later trials), even though the majority of participants were not explicitly aware of our probability manipulations. For the explicit effects in F2-locked data, no meaningful or significant effects were observed. These findings point to the existence of proposed success-response mechanisms that operate not only explicitly but also with implicit manipulations that do not involve any direct indication of a win or loss, and are not associated with tangible rewards. Thus, there seems to be a non-explicit form of perception (we call ‘implicit’) associated with an internal experience of wins/losses (in the absence of actual rewards or losses) that can be measured in associated brain processes. The potential significance of these findings is discussed in terms of implications for problem gambling.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aine, C. J. (1994). A conceptual overview and critique of functional neuroimaging techniques in humans: I. MRI/FMRI and PET. Critical Reviews in Neurobiology, 9(2–3), 229–309.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5 ® ). Arlington: American Psychiatric Association Publishing.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bednark, J. G., & Franz, E. A. (2014). Agency attribution: Event-related potentials and outcome monitoring. Experimental Brain Research, 232(4), 1117–1126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bednark, J. G., Reynolds, J. N. J., Stafford, T., Redgrave, P., & Franz, E. A. (2013). Creating a movement heuristic for voluntary action: Electrophysiological correlates of movement-outcome learning. Cortex, 49(3), 771–780.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bednark, J. G., Reynolds, J. N. J., Stafford, T., Redgrave, P., & Franz, E. A. (2016). Action experience and action discovery in medicated individuals with Parkinson’s disease. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10.

  • Bondolfi, G., Osiek, C., & Ferrero, F. (2000). Prevalence estimates of pathological gambling in Switzerland. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 101(6), 473–475.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Caplin, A., Dean, M., Glimcher, P. W., & Rutledge, R. B. (2010). Measuring beliefs and rewards: A neuroeconomic approach. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 125(3), 923–960.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, M. X., Elger, C. E., & Ranganath, C. (2007). Reward expectation modulates feedback-related negativity and EEG spectra. Neuroimage, 35(2), 968–978.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Comings, D., Gade-Andavolu, R., Gonzalez, N., Wu, S., Muhleman, D., Chen, C. E., et al. (2001). The additive effect of neurotransmitter genes in pathological gambling. Clinical Genetics, 60(2), 107–116.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Courchesne, E., Hillyard, S. A., & Courchesne, R. Y. (1977). P3 waves to the discrimination of targets in homogeneous and heterogeneous stimulus sequences. Psychophysiology, 14(6), 590–597.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Debnath, R., & Franz, E. A. (2016). Perception of hand movement by mirror reflection evokes brain activation in the motor cortex contralateral to a non-moving hand. Cortex, 81, 118–125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134(1), 9–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eitam, B., Kennedy, P. M., & Higgins, E. T. (2013). Motivation from control. Experimental Brain Research, 229(3), 475–484.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 16(1), 143–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feigelman, W., Kleinman, P. H., Lesieur, H. R., Millman, R. B., & Lesser, M. L. (1995). Pathological gambling among methadone patients. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 39(2), 75–81.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Franz, E. A., & Fu, Y. (2017). Pre-movement planning processes in people with congenital mirror movements. Clinical Neurophysiology, 128, 1985–1993.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Franz, E. A., Fu, Y., Moore, M., Winter, T., Mayne, T., Debnath, R., et al. (2016). Fooling the brain by mirroring the hand: Brain correlates of the perceptual capture of limb ownership. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 34(5), 721–732.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Franz, E. A., & Miller, J. (2002). Effects of response readiness on reaction time and force output in people with Parkinson’s disease. Brain, 125, 1733–1750.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Galvan, A., Hare, T. A., Davidson, M., Spicer, J., Glover, G., & Casey, B. (2005). The role of ventral frontostriatal circuitry in reward-based learning in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(38), 8650–8656.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gehring, W. J., Coles, M. G., Meyer, D. E., & Donchin, E. (1995). A brain potential manifestation of error-related processing. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology-Supplements Only, 44, 261–272.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gehring, W. J., Goss, B., Coles, M. G., Meyer, D. E., & Donchin, E. (1993). A neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science, 4(6), 385–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gneezy, U., & Rustichini, A. (2000). Pay enough or don’t pay at all. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 115(3), 791–810.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hajcak, G., Holroyd, C. B., Moser, J. S., & Simons, R. F. (2005). Brain potentials associated with expected and unexpected good and bad outcomes. Psychophysiology, 42(2), 161–170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, G. W., Carriero, N. J., Takushi, R. Y., Montoya, I. D., Preston, K. L., & Gorelick, D. A. (2000). Pathological gambling among cocaine-dependent outpatients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(7), 1127–1133.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holroyd, C. B., & Coles, M. G. (2002). The neural basis of human error processing: Reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 109(4), 679–709.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holroyd, C. B., Nieuwenhuis, S., Yeung, N., & Cohen, J. D. (2003). Errors in reward prediction are reflected in the event-related brain potential. NeuroReport, 14(18), 2481–2484.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ito, T. A., Larsen, J. T., Smith, N. K., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: the negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(4), 887–900.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, R., & Donchin, E. (1980). P300 and stimulus categorization: Two plus one is not so different from one plus one. Psychophysiology, 17(2), 167–178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knutson, B., Adams, C. M., Fong, G. W., & Hommer, D. (2001). Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. Journal of Neuroscience, 21(16), 159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mognon, A., Jovicich, J., Bruzzone, L., & Buiatti, M. (2011). ADJUST: An automatic EEG artifact detector based on the joint use of spatial and temporal features. Psychophysiology, 48(2), 229–240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nakahara, D., Ozaki, N., Miura, Y., Miura, H., & Nagatsu, T. (1989). Increased dopamine and serotonin metabolism in rat nucleus accumbens produced by intracranial self-stimulation of medial forebrain bundle as measured by in vivo microdialysis. Brain Research, 495(1), 178–181.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oberg, S. A., Christie, G. J., & Tata, M. S. (2011). Problem gamblers exhibit reward hypersensitivity in medial frontal cortex during gambling. Neuropsychologia, 49(13), 3768–3775.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov, I. (1903). 1928 Lectures on conditioned reflexes (W. H. Gantt, Trans.). New York: International.

  • Pavlov, I. P., & Anrep, G. V. E. (1927). Conditioned reflexes: An investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex (Translated and Edited by G. V. Anrep). London.

  • Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118(10), 2128–2148.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sato, A., Yasuda, A., Ohira, H., Miyawaki, K., Nishikawa, M., Kumano, H., et al. (2005). Effects of value and reward magnitude on feedback negativity and P300. NeuroReport, 16(4), 407–411.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Volberg, R. A., Abbott, M. W., Rönnberg, S., & Munck, I. M. (2001). Prevalence and risks of pathological gambling in Sweden. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 104(4), 250–256.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, Y., & Zhou, X. (2009). The P300 and reward valence, magnitude, and expectancy in outcome evaluation. Brain Research, 1286, 114–122.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yeung, N., & Sanfey, A. G. (2004). Independent coding of reward magnitude and valence in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(28), 6258–6264.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Thanks to D. Fielding, Rev. J. Fielding, C. Wu, and A. Pirola-Merlo for helpful comments and discussion.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to E. A. Franz.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

No authors have any conflicts of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional committee (University of Otago Ethics Committee) and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Fielding, A., Fu, Y. & Franz, E.A. The Brain’s Reward Response Occurs Even Without Actual Reward!. J Gambl Stud 34, 449–463 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-017-9721-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-017-9721-3

Keywords

Navigation