Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Do Theory of Mind and Executive Function Deficits Underlie the Adverse Outcomes Associated with Profound Early Deprivation?: Findings from the English and Romanian Adoptees Study

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Theory of Mind (ToM) and Executive Function (EF) have been associated with autism and with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and hence might play a role in similar syndromes found following profound early institutional deprivation. In order to examine this possibility the current study included a group of 165 Romanian adoptees, of whom 144 were adopted into the UK from deprived institutional settings before 43months of age, and a group of 52 within-UK adoptees, all adopted before 6months of age. Both groups were assessed at 6 and 11years. The Strange Stories task was used to assess ToM and the Stroop task was used to assess EF, both at age 11. The Romanian adoptees displayed deficits in both ToM and EF compared with the within-UK adoptee group. The degree of deficit was greater for children who had experienced more than 6months of institutional deprivation. Deficits in both domains (ToM and EF) were associated with each of the three apparently deprivation-specific problems, namely quasi-autism, disinhibited attachment and inattention/overactivity. Statistical analyses indicated a mediating role for both ToM and EF with respect to quasi-autism; possibly a partial mediating role for EF with respect to inattention/overactivity; and probably no mediating role for either ToM or EF in the case of disinhibited attachment. In conclusion, there is evidence for a possible mediating role for ToM and EF in the development of some apparently deprivation-specific difficulties in institution-reared Romanian adoptees, but neither accounts for the overall pattern of deprivation-related difficulties.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18 and 1991 profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., & Robertson, M. (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: Evidence from very high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 813–822.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beckett, C., Maughan, B., Castle, J., Colvert, E., Groothues, C., Kreppner, J., et al. (2006). Do the effects of early severe deprivation on cognition persist into early adolescence? Findings from the English and Romanian adoptees study. Child Development, 77, 696–711.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beckett, C., Maughan, B., Rutter, M., Castle, J., Colvert, E., Groothues, C., et al. (2007). Scholastic attainment following severe early institutional deprivation: A study of children adopted from Romania. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 1063–1073.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Behar, L. B. (1977). The pre-school behaviour questionnaire. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 5, 265–275.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boyce, L., & Cole, T. (1993). Growth programme (Version 1 & 2) (Computer software). Ware, UK: Castlemead.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brocki, K. C., & Bohlin, G. (2004). Executive functions in children aged 6 to 13: A dimensional and developmental study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, 571–593.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buckler, R. (1990). A longitudinal study of adolescent growth. London: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2001). Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15, 481–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., Maughan, A., Toth, S. L., & Bruce, J. (2003). False belief understanding in maltreated children. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 1067–1091.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Comalli Jr., P. E., Wapner, S., & Werner, H. (1962). Interference effects of Stroop colour-word test in childhood, adulthood, and ageing. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 100, 47–53.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenmajer, R., & Prior, M. (1991). Cognitive linguistic correlates of “Theory of Mind” ability in autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 351–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elander, J., & Rutter, M. (1996). Use and development of the Rutter parents’ and teachers’ scales. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 6, 63–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flynn, J. R. (1987). Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure. Psychological Bulletin, 10, 171–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frankenberg, W. K., Van Doornick, W. J., Lidell, T. N., & Dick, N. P. (1986). Revised Denver Prescreening Developmental Questionnaire (R-PDQ). High Wycombe, UK: DDM/The Test Agency.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (2003). Autism: Explaining the enigma (2nd ed.). Malden, MA, US: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581–586.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Green, S., Pring, L., & Swettenham, J. (2004). An investigation of first-order false belief understanding of children with congenital profound visual impairment. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guttentag, R. E., & Haith, M. M. (1978). Automatic processing as a function of age and reading ability. Child Development, 49, 707–716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. G. E. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 129–154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P. L. (1992). From simulation to folk psychology: The case for development. Mind and Language, 7, 120–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heavey, L., Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., & Rutter, M. (2000). The awkward moments test: A naturalistic measure of social understanding in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 225–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hogg, C., Rutter, M., & Richman, N. (1997). Emotional and behavioural problems in children. In I. Sclare (Ed.) Child Psychology Portfolio. Windsor: NFER-Nelson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C. (1996). Control of action and thought: Normal development and dysfunction in autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology, Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 37, 229–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C. (1998). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 233–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., & Russell, J. (1993). Autistic children’s difficulty with mental disengagement from an object: Its implications for theories of autism. Developmental Psychology, 29, 498–510.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., Russell, J., & Robbins, T. W. (1993). Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia, 32, 477–492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, A., & Venables, P. H. (1982). Specificity of attention in the Stroop test—An Ep study. Biological Psychology, 15, 75–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). The strange stories test: A replication with high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 395–406.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaland, N., Moller-Nielsen, A., Smith, L., Mortensen, E. L., Callesen, K., & Gottlieb, D. (2005). The strange stories test: A replication study of children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 14, 73–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kraemer, H. C., Stice, E., Kazdin, A., & Kupfer, D. (2001). How do risk factors work together? Mediators, moderators, and independent overlapping and proxy risk factors. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 848–856.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kreppner, J. M., O’Connor, T. G., Dunn, J., & Andersen-Wood, L. (1999). The pretend and social role play of children exposed to early severe deprivation. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 319–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kreppner, J. M., Rutter, M., Beckett, C., Castle, J., Colvert, E., Groothues, C., Hawkins, A., O’Connor, T. G., Stevens, S. E., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2007). What predicts normality and impairment following profound early institutional deprivation? A longitudinal examination through childhood. Developmental Psychology, 43, 931–946.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Le Couteur, A., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Rios, P., Robertson, S., Holdgrafter, M., et al. (1989). Autism Diagnostic Interview: A standardized investigator-based instrument. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 363–387.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretence and representation: The origins of “Theory of Mind”. Psychological Review, 94, 412–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lezak, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lillard, A. S. (1994). Making sense of pretence. In C. Lewis, & P. Mitchell (Eds.) Children’s early understanding of mind: Origins and development (pp. 211–234). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

  • Lord, C., & Bailey, A. (2002). Autism spectrum disorders. In M. Rutter, & E. Taylor (Eds.) Child and adolescent psychiatry: Modern approaches ((pp. 636–663)4th ed.). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised: A revision of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163–203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, D. (1972). The McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities. New York: Psychological Corporation/ Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Connor, T., & Rutter, M. (2001). Attachment disorder behavior following early severe deprivation: Extension and longitudinal follow-up. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 703–712.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology, Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 32, 1081–1105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., South, M., & Provencal, S. (2005). Executive functions. In F.R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.) Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders Vol. 1 ((pp. 606–627)3rd ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pears, K. C., & Fisher, P. A. (2005). Emotion understanding and theory of mind among maltreated children in foster care: Evidence of deficits. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 47–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 51–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C. C., & Siegal, M. (1995). Deafness, conversation and theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 459–474.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioural and Brain Science, 4, 515–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rust, J., Golombok, S., & Trickey, G. (1993). Wechsler Objective Reading Dimensions. London: The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Berument, S. K., Le Couteur, A., Lord, C., & Pickles, A. (2003). Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Colvert, E., Kreppner, J., Beckett, C., Castle, J., Groothues, C., et al. (2007a). Early adolescent outcomes for institutionally-deprived and non-deprived adoptees. I. Disinhibited attachment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 17–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Kreppner, J., Croft, C., Murin, M., Colvert, E., Beckett, C. et al. (2007b). Early adolescent outcomes of institutionally deprived and non-deprived adoptees. III. Quasi-autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, DOI 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01792.x.

  • Rutter, M. L., Kreppner, J. M., & O’Connor, T. G. (2001). Specificity and heterogeneity in children’s responses to profound institutional privation. British Journal of Psychiatry, 179, 97–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., & the English and Romanian Adoptees study team (1998). Developmental catch-up, and deficit, following adoption after severe global early deprivation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 465–476.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, Down syndrome and developmental delays. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64, 1–114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sodian, B., Hülsken, C., & Thoermer, C. (2003). The self and action in theory of mind research. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 777–782.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sparrevohn, R., & Howie, P. M. (1995). Theory of mind in children with autistic disorder: Evidence of developmental progression and the role of verbal ability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 249–263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, S. E., Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Kreppner, J. M., Beckett, C., Castle, J., Colvert, E. et al. (2008). Inattention/overactivity following early severe institutional deprivation: Presentation and associations in early adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(3), 385–398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children—Revised. New York: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wickett, J. C., Vernon, P. A., & Lee, D. H. (2000). Relationships between factors of intelligence and brain volume. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 1095–1122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1336–1346.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are most grateful to all the families who have generously given their time for participating in this study, and whose comments and suggestions have been very helpful in relation to the interpretation of findings. The data collection phase of the study was supported by grants from the Helmut Horten Foundation and the UK Department of Health. Ongoing support is provided by grants from the Department of Health, the Nuffield Foundation and the Jacobs Foundation. We are glad to express our thanks to our external Advisory Group, whose input has been invaluable. The views expressed in this article are ours and do not necessarily represent those of the funders.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Emma Colvert.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Colvert, E., Rutter, M., Kreppner, J. et al. Do Theory of Mind and Executive Function Deficits Underlie the Adverse Outcomes Associated with Profound Early Deprivation?: Findings from the English and Romanian Adoptees Study. J Abnorm Child Psychol 36, 1057–1068 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-008-9232-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-008-9232-x

Keywords

Navigation