Introduction

Motor neuro-rehabilitation aims to improve patient’s functional abilities, replacing skills that have been lost fully or partially. A general neuro-rehabilitation mechanism of action is the potentiation of a group of latent neuronal connections that are utilized repeatedly during challenging behavioural practice. The repeated and persistent practice, over several weeks, of a challenging movement facilitates neural synapsis, which may result in lasting physiological changes in motor neural networks [1, 2].

The motor skills acquisition process may be described in distinct phases [2, 3], from the early to the consolidation stages, when the newly acquired skill is performed with minimal cognitive resources. The final stage is defined as when performance can be executed after long delays between training sessions [3]. Although the literature on neurological rehabilitation is vast, there are numerous and inconsistent parameters of intensity, frequency and therapy duration related to induced movement studies [1, 4].

The fovea, the centre of the macula, is responsible for detailed vision and fixation. Patients with central vision loss attempt fixation with an eccentric retinal zone known as the preferred retinal locus (PRL) [5]. Macular functional characteristics of light threshold sensitivity, fixation stability (FS) and PRL can be assessed with a microperimetry test, a psychophysical examination similar to static automated perimetry, where patients press a response button when light stimuli are visualized. In addition, a retinal eye-tracker samples and corrects for eye movements, whilst fixation location and FS are plotted as a cloud of fixation points over a reference retinal image [6]. FS in microperimetry is classified as stable, relatively unstable or unstable [7]. The MAIA microperimeter (CenterVue, Italy) scores FS with different indexes; the most representative are P1 and the bivariate contour ellipse area (BCEA) with proportional values of 95% [8]. P1 describes the amount of retinal displacement occurring within 1° from an initial reference point, whilst BCEA describes 95% of retinal loci used during fixation attempt.

Eyes with eccentric fixation regularly demonstrate unstable FS with associated low vision. However, it has been reported that FS can be improved with oculomotor exercises known as biofeedback fixation training (BFT) [6, 9,10,11,12,13], a task-oriented behavioural therapy, which according to some authors may drive neural plasticity changes in the visual system [11, 14]. BFT consists of asking patients to perform ocular movements towards a specific direction, attempting to align a selected retinal locus with a visual target. This locus is known as the fixation training target. Biofeedback audio signals (beep sounds) aid patients during the oculomotor task by increasing the auditory frequency as the training target approaches the desired alignment.

To be of value, the training target should have better functional characteristics than the PRL previously used by the eye with unstable fixation. However, the selection of this training target has not been previously described.

In this study, we describe a methodology for selecting the best fixation training target, with the aim of improving eccentric fixation through BFT with microperimetry in patients with unstable fixation.

Methods

This was a prospective, consecutive, case series study of a cohort of patients with irreversible bilateral central vision loss, poor FS and best corrected visual acuity (VA) of 0.3 LogMAR or worse, who performed and completed BFT sessions with the MAIA. The eye with better VA was selected for this study. If both eyes had equal VA, the eye with better FS was chosen. The study included participants from the Low Vision and the Macular Clinics of the Queen’s Medical Centre, Nottingham (UK), from January 2013 to June 2017. Local ethical board approval (NRES Committee East Midlands—Nottingham 1) and written informed consent were obtained from all individual participants included in the study, whilst the study met the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Ninety recruited patients were alternatively divided in 2 different groups. In group A (mean age 64.7 ± 22 years), the retinal locus for BFT was set on the baseline patient’s spontaneous PRL, assessed with the MAIA Standard-Macula-Test (10°, 37 stimuli). In group B (mean age 70.4 ± 14 years), fixation training was set on the locus with the best functional characteristics. To determine this locus, two custom MAIA examinations were performed: (a) the “Low-Vision-Assessment” grid test (30°, 83 stimuli) with the 4-levels-fixed projection strategy, which scores retinal sensitivity as “good” (25 dB), “relatively good” (15 dB), “relatively poor” (5 dB), “poor” (0 dB) and “scotoma” (< 0 dB); and (b) the “Fixation-Training-Target” grid test (7° × 5°, 35 stimuli) with the 4–2 projection strategy. The first one, (a), was centred on the estimated foveal location or on the patient’s PRL in cases of GA larger than 3 times the optic nerve head size (ONH). The Low-Vision-Assessment grid output was used to identify retinal loci with at least 2 consecutive stimuli, distributed horizontally, showing “good” or “relatively good” light sensitivity, and served as a reference to centre the Fixation-Training-Target grid, prioritizing the superior retina (inferior visual field) and the smaller distance from the anatomical fovea (Fig. 1). The second custom test, (b), is used to select the target locus for BFT. This locus was set in the centre of the two adjacent stimuli with highest light sensitivity and lowest distance from both the anatomical fovea and the baseline PRL.

Fig. 1
figure 1

a “Low-Vision-Assessment” grid centred on patient’s PRL with the 4-levels-fixed projection strategy, showing four different dB levels of intensity, plus absolute scotoma: green = 25 dB, yellow = 15 dB, red = 5 dB, purple = 0 dB, black = scotoma. b “Fixation-Training-Target” grid with the 4–2 projection strategy, used to select the locus with highest retinal sensitivity for subsequent BFT sessions

Adapting the training frequency reported in the literature [10,11,12, 15, 16], and with the scope to reach consolidation and retention therapeutic stages, we performed 2 sets of 12 weekly training sessions separated by a 3-month period of no training. Each session lasted 10 min. BFT consisted of asking patients to slightly move their gaze towards the training locus. The auditory signal increased frequency as the desired eye movement reached the target. Patients were asked to remember the gaze movement performed during the training sessions and to try to reproduce the same movement in their daily life when attempting to steadily see a visual target.

Final results were assessed 2 weeks after completing BFT sessions. Primary outcomes were classification of FS, fixation indices P1 and BCEA@95%, VA, and reading speed (IReST) [17]. FS values after the first training session and after 6 months from baseline were also recorded. Secondary outcomes were mean light sensitivity. The anatomical location and the visual field correspondence of the PRL were also annotated.

Statistical analysis and graphics (GraphPadPrism_7.04) included standard errors, 95% confidence, interquartile intervals and robust regression outlier removal. Assuming a nonparametric distribution, a one-tailed Mann–Whitney test was applied to compare outcomes between baseline and the last training sessions for each group, and between groups with a significant difference of p < 0.05. Outcome correlations were analysed through Spearman’s rank-order coefficient (rs).

Results

Sixty-seven patients completed the study; 30 had geographic atrophy (GA), 19 moderate dry AMD, 9 Best’s disease, 6 myopic macular degeneration, and 3 central serous retinopathy (CSR). Group A included 28 (20 female) and group B 39 (27 female). Mean central scotoma sizes were 5.4° ± 3.8° and 5.7° ± 4.5° for groups A and B, respectively.

At baseline, FS classification in group A was unstable in 18 subjects (64%), relatively unstable in 9 (32%) and stable in 1 (4%). At the end of therapy, 16 subjects (57%) were classified as unstable, 11 (39%) as relatively unstable, and 1 (4%) as stable. In group B, baseline classification was unstable in 19 (49%) subjects, relatively unstable in 19 (49%), and stable in 1 (2%). At therapy end, 11 subjects (28%) were classified as unstable, 16 (41%) as relatively unstable, and 12 (31%) as stable (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Classification of fixation stability (FS) on baseline (BL) and end of biofeedback training for both groups A and B

In group A, mean FS index P1(%) was 32 ± 19 at baseline, 26 ± 18 after the first BFT session, 34 ± 22 at 6 months, and 35 ± 23 at the end of therapy. In group B, mean P1 was 40 ± 24 at baseline, 27 ± 20 after the first BFT, 48 ± 29 after 6 months, and 55 ± 29 at therapy end (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Box-and-whisker plot showing quartile distribution and mean data of fixation index P1 and BCEA@95% at baseline (BL), after the first BFT session, 6 months after first treatment and at the end of all BFT sessions (EBFT)

The mean area (deg2) of BCEA@95 in group A was 38 ± 23 at baseline, 51 ± 47 after the first BFT, 33 ± 22 at 6 months, and 32 ± 25 at study end. In group B, it was 39 ± 40 at baseline, 64 ± 70 at first BFT, 30 ± 31 at 6 months, and 19 ± 18 at therapy end (Fig. 3).

FS index P1 did not improve in 50% of subjects in group A and 18% in group B. Similarly, 35% of group A subjects did not improve in BCEA@95%, compared to 10% of those from group B.

Mean VA (LogMAR) improvement was observed in 16 (57%) subjects from group A (1.0 ± 0.48 to 0.86 ± 0.53) and 26 (67%) from group B (1.0 ± 0.51 to 0.84 ± 0.49). VA was unchanged in 4 (14%) subjects from group A and 10 (25%) from group B, whilst a decrease in VA was seen in 8 (29%) participants from group A and 3 (8%) from group B. Mean reading speed (wpm) improved from 56 ± 30 to 58 ± 32 in group A and from 63 ± 36 to 89 ± 46 in group B.

Treatment efficacy (baseline vs therapy end) showed no significant difference in any of the studied variables in group A, as demonstrated with the one-tailed Mann–Whitney test shown in Table 1a. In contrast, differences were found in all group B variables except on light threshold sensitivity (LTS) (Table 1b). When comparing final outcomes between groups, a significant difference in all parameters was found, except for VA (Table 1c).

Table 1 Mann–Whitney test of (a) baseline versus therapy end for group A. (b) Baseline versus therapy end for group B. (c) Therapy-end comparison for group A versus group B

Baseline FS indexes in group A showed moderate correlation (0.50 < rs < 0.70) with their final FS values and scotoma extension. However, high correlation (rs > 0.7) was found with final reading speed. In group B, baseline FS indexes were highly correlated (rs > 0.7) with final FS values, whilst moderately correlated with final VA and reading speed. FS outcomes demonstrated a negligible correlation with scotoma size (rs = 0.2), whilst low dependence with the trained location (rs = 0.3) was found. Final VA showed better correlation with baseline FS in group B (rs = 0.4) than in group A (rs = 0.2). Final reading speed correlation with FS in group A was high (rs = 0.7) and moderate (rs = 0.5) with the trained location, whilst in group B correlation with FS was moderate with FS (rs = 0.5) and low with the trained location (rs = 0.3). Low correlation was found (rs < 0.3) in both groups between functional outcomes and patient’s age. Similarly, no correlation was found (rs < 0.3) after performing a subgroup analysis to study the PRL behaviour in the different pathologies investigated.

Discussion

Although task-specific training to enhance motor representations has been reported for several decades [1], only a few authors have demonstrated FS improvement in patients with foveal impairment using biofeedback and microperimetry [9,10,11,12,13, 15, 16, 18, 19]. Furthermore, detailed methodologies adopted to define the best functional retinal locus for such training have not been fully described.

Ramirez et al. [20], following our suggestions, demonstrated the effectiveness of BFT 1 week after completion of therapy, whilst Ratra et al. [21] recently demonstrated in a small number of patients that the BFT effect can be maintained for up to 6 months with a slight reduction in fixation stability. Our study demonstrated a similar reduction, suggesting that such visual training should be attempted for longer periods to achieve maximum results.

Our study adds additional credence to the notion that fixation in patients with eccentric vision can be improved through biofeedback therapy (Fig. 4). Nudo [2] suggested that without behavioural training, plasticity in spared motor areas, which occurs spontaneously, largely reflects the development of compensatory motor patterns rather than patterns of true recovery. Our findings highlight the concept that localized fixation training may enhance plasticity more efficiently than when training is performed on the PRL which was spontaneously developed by the individual after the loss of foveal function.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Example of a group B patient, showing the cloud of fixation points with different PRL location and improvement of fixation stability from baseline (a), to end of biofeedback training (b). Fixation improved from unstable to relatively unstable and visual acuity from 1.03 to 0.8 LogMAR

BFT is reported to be dependent on the location, with the highest retinal sensitivity in small central scotomata [22]. However, we explored the possibility of standardized BFT in cases with any central scotoma size.

The BFT theory is based on neuro-plasticity, where healthy neural sensors are frequently stimulated. When retina photoreceptors and ganglion cells are healthy, microperimetry outcomes demonstrate high light threshold sensitivity values. For this reason, our first FTT selection criteria are a retinal location with good light sensitivity.

Detailed vision is performed with high packing density of cone photoreceptors. Its density peak, located at the foveal centre, decreases rapidly within the central 2 mm, with a gradual decrease further away [23]. Subjects with healthy vision perform fixation within the central 2°, as demonstrated with the MAIA [8]. Consequently, the assumed correlation between density of cones and fixation abilities is valid. Recent studies confirmed photoreceptor’s density decreases at 1°, 2°, 4 and 6° of eccentricity, showing a homogenous drop in each of the four retinal meridians, and high agreement between nasal and temporal locations [23, 24]. These results suggest that patients may have similar anatomical visual capabilities at any retinal meridian with eccentric equidistance from the fovea. In light of these associations, the second FTT selection criterion corresponds to an area located closer to the anatomical fovea without discrimination of the retinal meridian.

Previous studies suggest that reading with eccentric viewing may be more efficient if the PRL is located on the left hemisphere and the lower visual field [25,26,27]. These observations reinforced our third FTT selection criterion, suggesting the predilection on the left and/or superior side of the central scotoma whenever good light sensitivity is present.

Finally, it is well known that PRL positions may also depend on the visual task. In the western world, reading is performed from left to right. Reading tasks involve eye fixation and saccadic movements following a horizontal path. For that reason, our methodology locates the training location in the middle of a horizontal line, with at least 2 adjacent stimuli with good sensitivity.

Our investigation contributes to the literature with a thorough BFT analysis and scope to understand the rationale behind the selection process of an effective retinal locus useful during eccentric fixation training in patients with foveal function loss. This methodology is summarized as follows:

  1. 1.

    Perform the “Low-Vision-Assessment” grid test with the 4-levels-fixed projection strategy centred on the anatomical foveal or on the patient’s baseline PRL in eyes with Geographic Atrophy larger than 3 times the optic nerve head (ONH).

  2. 2.

    Identify loci with at least 2 consecutive stimuli, distributed horizontally, of good or relatively good threshold sensitivity (GTS).

  3. 3.

    Perform the “Fixation-Training-Target” grid test with the 4–2 projection strategy. Centre the grid on the GTS loci. If there are more than 1 GTS option, prioritize the smaller distance to the fovea on either the superior retina or the left visual field with lower distance from the baseline PRLs.

  4. 4.

    Use the “Fixation-Training-Target” grid outcomes to select the final trained retinal locus to perform biofeedback training (BFT). This locus should be set in the centre of the 2 highest horizontal adjacent threshold stimuli.

  5. 5.

    Perform 10-min BFT sessions over the selected training target on a weekly basis for 12 weeks. After a resting period of 3 months, perform a new set of 12 weekly BFT sessions to aid visual plasticity consolidation.

To conclude, in this study we have described a methodology for biofeedback training with microperimetry, with the scope to improve eccentric vision through better fixation control. Further studies are needed to validate the effectiveness of this methodology in everyday visual tasks, such as reading and other visuomotor activities. Of paramount importance is an investigation of the different motor-sequence adaptation stages during BFT, in particular, the recognition of the consolidation and automatic stages, as these may be the key to optimizing frequency and duration for individual therapeutic strategies, as well as to understanding whether long-term plasticity changes can be achieved and retained.