Skip to main content
Log in

The Impact of Budget Goal Difficulty and Promotion Availability on Employee Fraud

  • Published:
Journal of Business Ethics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The purpose of this research is to examine the effect of two organizational variables, budget goal difficulty and promotion availability, on employee fraud (e.g., stealing). Limited research shows that difficult, specific goals result in more unethical behavior than general goals (Schweitzer et al., Acad Manag J 47(3):422–432, 2004). We predict that goal difficulty and promotion availability will interact to affect employee fraud. Specifically, we contend that the availability of promotions will have little, if any, effect on employee fraud under easy goals but have a substantial effect on fraud under extremely difficult goals. To test this prediction, we use an experiment in which participants play the role of production employees. All participants are assigned cost goals and are responsible for reporting costs to the organization. Information asymmetry exists such that the organization is unable to assess whether costs have been reported accurately, and any overstatement of costs directly increases the employee’s compensation at the expense of the organization. The results of our experiment support the prediction, even though the cost goal is not tied to current period compensation (e.g., a mere goal). Our results have implications for academics and practitioners concerned with factors that affect fraud in organizations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The primary difference between fraudulent financial reporting and employee fraud is that fraudulent financial reporting benefits the organization by making financial performance appear better than it is. In contrast, employee fraud benefits the individual at the expense of the organization (Albrecht et al. 2011).

  2. Our research uses student participants in the role of employee. Thus, we use the terms employee and participant interchangeably throughout the paper.

  3. Some of the participants in the experiment were assigned to the role of owners. As discussed more fully below, these participants do not make reporting decisions and have limited involvement in the experiment.

  4. Evans et al. (2001) refer to contracts involving this type of information asymmetry as “trust contracts.”

  5. Of course, important differences exist between the costs reported by employees in our setting and the costs reported in the binding and nonbinding conditions examined by Rankin et al. (2003). For example, in our setting the owners were required to accept the cost report whereas in the nonbinding condition examined by Rankin et al. (2003) the owner can choose whether or not to fund the project based on the cost report.

  6. While this is often the case, we note that in some circumstances, such as when a difficult goal is perceived as threatening, performance can be inhibited (Drach-Zahavy and Erez 2002). For a more general discussion of the potentially disruptive effects of difficult goals, see Sitkin et al. (2011).

  7. Using actual participants as owners, rather than programming the computer to act as an owner, serves to highlight for the employees that there is an actual counter-party who suffers a loss as a result of any employee fraud. This strengthens the experiment by biasing against finding support for our hypothesis.

  8. Following Bloomfield and Luft (2006) the production recommendation made by participants did not relate in any way to the earnings of the production employees or owners. This part of the task was intended to better reflect a production environment, stimulate participant’s interest in the task, and increase participants’ feelings of responsibility for production cost goals.

  9. Consistent with prior accounting research involving participant recommendations and pre-determined outcomes used to compare across conditions (e.g., Bloomfield and Luft 2006; Tayler 2010) no definitive assertions stating a connection between production recommendations and cost outcomes were made.

  10. Consistent with prior studies on managerial honesty (e.g., Evans et al. 2001; Rankin et al. 2008) and because we are concerned with reporting behavior, the cost of effort is assumed to be zero and therefore, does not affect the participants’ payout.

  11. Under a flat wage contract, the only way an employee can increase their utility is to over-report costs and then increase perquisite consumption. Prior honesty research on perquisite consumption (e.g., Evans et al. 2001; Rankin et al. 2008) captures this element experimentally by paying subjects the difference between the subjects cost report and actual cost. Thus, there are two elements to the payoff function under a flat wage contract, the 175 experimental dollar base salary and the amount of money claimed above the actual cost (report − actual cost).

  12. Had we chosen to also include an economic incentive, then we would not have been able to discern the psychological effects of the goal difficulty manipulation, which are important to our theory. To the extent that our set-up deviates from that which is found in practice, we believe that this would affect the level of fraud but would not affect the shape of our predicted interaction between goal difficulty and promotion availability.

  13. It is important to point out that the procedures used in this experiment were adapted from Evans et al. (2001), Hannan et al. (2006), and Rankin et al. (2008). The procedures assure anonymity and were set up so that participants should pay all of their attention to economic incentives and always claim costs of $200 to maximize their payouts. Deviations from this economic maximizing behavior suggests, by design, that participants are persuaded, at least partially, by psychological influences over economic influences. In the case of promotions tied to a small raise, participants focusing on economic incentives would still be enticed monetarily to report the maximum cost of $200 as participants were informed of the small (and capped) raise sizes before making their reporting decisions and would be able to see that they would be better off financially over-reporting costs.

  14. In the promotion absent condition, we needed to select a single title that would be held constant throughout the experiment. We selected what we believed to be a very generic title, “production manager.” As this title was held constant, we do not believe our results are sensitive to the specific title we selected. For example, we have no reason to believe that our results would have been any different if we had selected “production supervisor” as the title.

  15. The results of pilot testing indicated that participants considered a promotion without at least a nominal raise to be highly unusual.

  16. We analyzed the data using a repeated measures analysis-of-variance to evaluate potential period effects. Controlling for the within-subject error variance, period was not significant and did not interact with any of the between-subjects factors. Similarly, we find that round also did not significantly interact with any of the between-subjects factors (F = 0.70, p = 0.71). This suggests that the instructions effectively communicated the nature of the task to participants.

  17. Descriptive statistics are in experimental dollars, unless otherwise indicated.

  18. Recall that participants observed three different cost sequences. To ascertain whether different cost sequences had a significant effect on the results we conducted a 2 (goal difficulty: easy vs. extremely difficult) × 2 (promotion availability: present vs. absent) × 3 (cost sequence), ANOVA using the fraud measure as the dependent variable. The results show that the main effect for cost sequence was insignificant (F = 0.85, p = 0.43), and cost sequence did not interact with either or both of the other independent variables. Consequently, we do not discuss cost sequence further.

  19. All p-values are two-tailed unless otherwise stated.

  20. We also examine an alternative measure of fraud in which the average reported cost is greater than actual cost. To compute this measure we set to zero all observations for which the average reported cost is less than the actual cost. This occurs for 7 of the 59 participants. Using this alternative measure of fraud we find results that are consistent with those reported in the text. Specifically, the main effect of promotion availability remains marginally significant (F = 3.96, p = 0.06), the main effect of goal difficulty remains non-significant (F = 0.03, p = 0.86), and the interaction between goal difficulty and promotion availability remains significant (F = 8.56, p = 0.01).

  21. Unfortunately, due to an experimental error, this data was not collected for all subjects.

References

  • Adams, J. (1965). Inequity in social exchange. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 267–299). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Albrecht, W. S., Albrecht, C. O., Albrecht, C. C., & Zimbelman, M. F. (2011). Fraud examination (4th ed., pp. 9–12). Mason, OH: South-Western, Cengage Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Association of Certified Fraud Examiners (ACFE). (2012). Report to the nations on occupational fraud and abuse. Austin, TX: ACFE.

  • Baker, G., Jensen, M., & Murphy, K. (1988). Compensation and incentives: Practice vs. theory. The Journal of Finance, 43(3), 593–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barsky, A. (2008). Understanding the ethical cost of organizational goal-setting: A review and theory development. Journal of Business Ethics, 81(1), 63–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blau, F. D., & DeVaro, J. (2007). New evidence on gender differences in promotion rates: An empirical analysis of a sample of new hires. Industrial Relations, 46(3), 511–550.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloomfield, R. J., & Luft, J. L. (2006). Responsibility for cost management hinders learning to avoid the winner’s curse. The Accounting Review, 81(1), 29–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowen, R. M., Call, A. C., & Rajgopal, S. (2010). Whistle-blowing: Target firm characteristics and economic consequences. The Accounting Review, 85(4), 1239–1271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cichello, M. S., Fee, C. E., Hadlock, C. J., & Sonti, R. (2009). Promotions, turnover, and performance evaluation: Evidence from the careers of division managers. The Accounting Review, 84(4), 1119–1143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cobb-Clark, D. A. (2001). Getting ahead: The determinants of and payoffs to internal promotion for young U.S. men and women. In S. W. Polachek (Ed.), Worker wellbeing in a changing labor market (Vol. 20, pp. 339–372). New York: JAI Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., Ding, Y., Lesage, C., & Stolowy, H. (2011). Corporate fraud and managers’ behavior: Evidence from the press. Journal of Business Ethics, 95, 271–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., Holder-Webb, L., Sharp, D. J., & Pant, L. W. (2007). The effects of perceived fairness on opportunistic behavior. Contemporary Accounting Research, 24, 1119–1138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coletti, A. L., Sedatole, K. L., & Towry, K. L. (2005). The effect of control systems on trust and cooperation in collaborative environments. The Accounting Review, 80, 477–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coram, P., Ferguson, C., & Moroney, R. (2008). Internal audit, alternative internal audit structures and the level of misappropriation of asset fraud. Accounting and Finance, 48, 543–559.

    Google Scholar 

  • Correll, J., Spencer, S. J., & Zanna, M. P. (2004). An affirmed self and an open mind: Self-affirmation and sensitivity to argument strength. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 350–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cropanzano, R., Byrne, Z. S., Bobocel, D. R., & Rupp, D. E. (2001). Moral virtues, fairness heuristics, social entities, and other denizens of organizational justice. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 58(1), 164–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drach-Zahavy, A., & Erez, M. (2002). Challenge versus threat effects on the goal–performance relationship. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 88, 667–682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dyck, I. J. A., Morse, A., & Zingales, L. (2010). Who blows the whistle on corporate fraud? The Journal of Finance, 65(6), 2213–2253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. H., Hannan, R. L., Krishnan, R., & Moser, D. V. (2001). Honesty in managerial reporting. The Accounting Review, 76(4), 537–559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischbacher, U. (2007). Z-tree: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments. Experimental Economics, 10(2), 171–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, J., Frederickson, J. R., & Peffer, S. A. (2002a). The effect of information asymmetry on negotiated budgets: An empirical investigation. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 27, 27–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, J. G., Maines, L. A., Peffer, S. A., & Sprinkle, G. B. (2002b). Using budgets for performance evaluation: Effects of resource allocation and horizontal information asymmetry on budget proposals, budget slack, and performance. The Accounting Review, 77(4), 847–865.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Folger, R. (1986). Rethinking equity theory: A referent cognitions model. In H. W. Bierhoff, R. L. Cohen, & J. Greenberg (Eds.), Justice in social relations (pp. 145–162). New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Folger, R. (1987). Reformulating the preconditions of resentment: A referent cognitions model. In J. C. Masters & W. P. Smith (Eds.), Social comparison, social justice, and relative deprivation (pp. 183–215). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Folger, R., & Cropanzano, R. (1998). Organizational justice and human resource management. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Folger, R., & Cropanzano, R. (2001). Fairness theory: Justice as accountability. In J. Greenberg & R. Cropanzano (Eds.), Advances in organizational justice (pp. 1–55). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedand, Y., & Slowick, L. H. (2004). Enriching goal-setting theory with time: An integrated approach. Academy of Management Review, 29(3), 404–422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galinsky, A. D., Mussweiler, T., & Medvec, V. H. (2002). Disconnecting outcomes and evaluations: The role of negotiator focus. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1131–1140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hales, J., & Williamson, M. G. (2010). Implicit employment contracts: The limit of management reputation for promoting productivity. Journal of Accounting Research, 48(1), 147–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hannan, R. L., Krishnan, R., & Newman, A. H. (2008). The effects of disseminating relative performance feedback in tournament and individual performance compensation plans. The Accounting Review, 83(4), 893–913.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hannan, R. L., Rankin, F. W., & Towry, K. L. (2006). The effect of information systems on honesty in managerial reporting: A behavioral perspective. Contemporary Accounting Research, 23(4), 885–918.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hannan, R. L., Rankin, F. W., & Towry, K. L. (2010). Flattening the organization: The effect of organizational reporting structure on budgeting effectiveness. Review of Accounting Studies, 15, 503–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heath, C., Larrick, R. P., & Wu, G. (1999). Goals as reference points. Cognitive Psychology, 38(1), 79–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hogan, C. E., Razaee, Z., Riley, R. A., Jr., & Velury, U. K. (2008). Financial statement fraud: Insights from the academic literature. Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory, 27(2), 231–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huberman, B. A., Loch, C. H., & Onculer, A. (2004). Status as a valued resource. Social Psychology Quarterly, 67(1), 103–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johansson-Stenman, O., & Martinsson, P. (2006). Honestly, why are you driving a BMW. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 60, 129–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kachelmeier, S. J., Reichert, B. E., & Williamson, M. G. (2008). Measuring and motivating quantity, creativity, or both. Journal of Accounting Research, 46(2), 341–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kerlinger, F. N., & Lee, H. B. (2000). Foundations of behavioral research (4th ed.). Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt College Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosteas, V. D. (2009). Job satisfaction and promotions. Unpublished working paper, Cleveland State University.

  • KPMG. (2009). Fraud survey 2009. KPMG.

  • Larrick, R. P., Heath, C., & Wu, G. (2009). Goal-induced risk taking in negotiation and decision making. Social Cognition, 27, 342–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Latham, G. P., & Kinne, S. B. (1974). Improving job performance through training in goal setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 59, 187–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Libby, T. (2001). Referent cognitions and budgetary fairness: A research note. Journal of Management Accounting Research, 13, 91–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lind, E. A. (2001). Fairness heuristic theory: Justice judgments as pivotal cognitions in organizational relations. In J. Greenberg & R. Cropanzanno (Eds.), Advances in organizational justice (pp. 56–88). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A. (2001). Motivation by goal setting. In R. T. Golembiewski (Ed.), Handbook of organizational behavior (2nd ed.). New York: Marcel Dekker Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation: A 35-year odyssey. American Psychologist, 57, 705–717.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2006). New directions in goal-setting theory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 265–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magner, N. R., Welker, R. B., & Campbell, T. L. (1995). The interactive effect of budgetary participation and budget favorability on attitudes toward budgetary decision makers: A research note. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 20(7/8), 611–618.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCue, K. (1996). Promotions and wage growth. Journal of Labor Economics, 14(2), 175–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mead, N., Baumeister, R., Gino, F., Schweitzer, M., & Ariely, D. (2009). Too tired to tell the truth: Self-control resource depletion and dishonesty. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 594–597.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, P. R., & Dacin, M. T. (2011). Psychological pathways to fraud: Understanding and preventing fraud in organizations. Journal of Business Ethics, 101(4), 601–618.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mustafa, S. T., & Meier, H. H. (2006). Audit committees and misappropriation of assets: Publicly held companies in the United States. Canadian Accounting Perspectives, 5(2), 307–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ordóñez, L., Schweitzer, M., Galinsky, A., & Bazerman, M. (2009). On good scholarship, goal setting, and scholars gone wild. Academy of Management Perspectives, 23, 82–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Owens, D. A., & Sutton, R. I. (2002). Status contests in meetings: Negotiating the informal order. In M. E. Turner (Ed.), Groups at work: Advances in theory and research. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • PricewaterhouseCoopers (PwC). (2011). The global economic crime survey: Cybercrime: Protecting against the growing threat.

  • Proell, C., & Smith, S. D. (2014). The impact of goals and responsibility on managerial reporting honesty. Working paper, Texas Christian University.

  • Rankin, F. W., Schwartz, S. T., & Young, R. A. (2003). Management control using nonbinding budgetary announcements. Journal of Management Accounting Research, 15, 75–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rankin, F. W., Schwartz, S. T., & Young, R. A. (2008). The effect of honesty and superior authority on budget proposals. The Accounting Review, 83(4), 1083–1099.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schweitzer, M. E., Ordonez, L., & Douma, B. (2004). Goal setting as a motivator of unethical behavior. Academy of Management Journal, 47(3), 422–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sitkin, S. B., See, K. E., Miller, C. C., Lawless, M. W., & Carton, A. M. (2011). The paradox of stretch goals: Organizations in pursuit of the seemingly impossible. Academy of Management Review, 36(3), 544–566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sprinkle, G. B., Williamson, M. G., & Upton, D. R. (2008). The effort and risk-taking effects of budget-based contracts. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 33, 436–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Staw, B. M., & Boettger, R. D. (1990). Task revision: A neglected form of work performance. Academy of Management Journal, 33(3), 534–559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tayler, W. B. (2010). The balanced scorecard as a strategy-evaluation tool: The effects of implementation involvement and a causal-chain focus. The Accounting Review, 85(3), 1095–1117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Washington, M., & Zajac, E. J. (2005). Status evolution and competition: Theory and evidence. Academy of Management Journal, 48(2), 282–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webb, A. (2002). The impact of reputation and variance investigations on the creation of budget slack. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 27(4–5), 361–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webb, R. A. (2004). Managers’ commitment to the goals contained in a strategic performance measurement system. Contemporary Accounting Research, 21(4), 925–958.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, J. T. (2004). New approaches to fraud deterrence. Journal of Accountancy, 197, 72–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, J. T. (2008). Principles of fraud examination (2nd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, Y. (2008). The effects of perceived fairness and communication on honesty and collusion in a multi-agent setting. The Accounting Review, 83(4), 1125–1146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shana M. Clor-Proell.

Appendices

Appendix 1: Instructions for Experimental Task

figure a

Appendix 2: Compensation Information

figure b

Appendix 3: Comprehension Checks for Experimental Procedures

figure c

Appendix 4: Production Recommendations

figure d

Appendix 5: Actual Cost Report

figure e

Appendix 6: Outcome Summary

figure f

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Clor-Proell, S.M., Kaplan, S.E. & Proell, C.A. The Impact of Budget Goal Difficulty and Promotion Availability on Employee Fraud. J Bus Ethics 131, 773–790 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-013-2021-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-013-2021-7

Keywords

Navigation