Introduction

Anatomical background

Cerebellum and basal ganglia are oligosynaptically interconnected with the cerebral cortex (Nieuwenhuys et al. 2008; Haber et al. 2012; Voogd and Ruigrok 2012). Both structures receive the bulk of their afferences from the cerebral cortex and send major projections back to the cortex via the thalamus. The cerebellum receives its cortical input from motor and sensory areas. These projections are mainly relayed in the pontine nuclei and traverse the medial cerebellar peduncle to innervate the cerebellar cortex. Here, microcircuitry processes this input and sends the resulting output via its Purkinje cells almost entirely to the deep cerebellar nuclei (DCN). These constitute the main output structures of the cerebellum and project to a great extent via the superior cerebellar peduncle to the thalamus. The main neurotransmitter of these projections is glutamate, which elicits mostly excitatory cell responses. Other neurotransmitters, as well as inhibitory and complex response patterns, have also been described (Uusisaari and Knöpfel 2012; Buford et al. 1996). In the thalamus, these signals are relayed for the most part to the primary motor area of the cortex; further projections to premotor and prefrontal areas exhibit a decreasing density gradient (Sakai et al. 2002). The information flow between cerebral cortex and basal ganglia is organized similarly: afferences originating from motor, prefrontal, and orbitofrontal cortical areas innervate the striatum; interactions between their projection territories at the level of the striatum are extensive. These interactions occur in specific regions in which convergence of terminal fields from different functional cortical regions is found. Incoming signals are processed within internal basal ganglia circuitries and outcome is conveyed to the main output structures: the internal part of the globus pallidus (GPi) and the reticular part of the substantia nigra (SNr). The bulk of this output reaches the thalamus and exerts a GABAergic inhibitory influence on projection neurons that target the cerebral cortex. This anatomy suggests a circular information flow (Haber 2016).

These cerebello-thalamo-cortical and basal ganglio-thalamo-cortical loops are topographically organized, constituting anatomically and functionally segregated channels which are relayed in different thalamic nuclei [see Fig. 7 in Middleton and Strick (2000)]. An interaction of cerebellar and basal ganglia loops occurs mainly in motor regions of the cerebral cortex (Kuramoto et al. 2009). Subcortical interactions, however, are often neglected when modeling brain function on a systemic level (Doya 2000). Transneuronal tracing studies in primates point towards substantial subcortical interactions of these two systems. Those studies revealed trisynaptic projections of the cerebellum via thalamus and striatum into the external part of the globus pallidus (Hoshi et al. 2005). Similar projections of the basal ganglia to the cerebellum have been shown to originate in the subthalamic nucleus, being relayed to the cerebellar cortex via pontine nuclei and/or tegmental pontine reticular nucleus (Bostan et al. 2010). Further evidence of subcortical interaction exists in the brainstem [e.g., superior colliculus in Westby et al. (1994)] and in the thalamus (Chevalier and Deniau 1982; Nambu et al. 1988; Sakai and Patton 1993; Sakai et al. 1996). However, discrepancies exist concerning the amount of these convergences. It is rather difficult to compare the quantification of investigated neurons in electrophysiological studies, light microscopy, retrograde tracing studies, and anterograde studies. Descriptions of projection density were commonly performed using categories such as “sparse”, “moderate”, “substantial”, or “dense”. Although synapses cannot be resolved by light microscopy, the general morphology of axon terminals allows for an estimation of the density of the projection. This information is, e.g., inaccessible by retrograde tracing studies. Since the morphology of axon terminals can differ substantially in its collateralization and number of developed synapses, the meaningfulness of such quantifications is limited. To enable a better comparability between such studies, we indicate the relative frequency of neurons in which converging inputs could be shown in relation with the total number of investigated neurons. In this work, we investigated to what degree convergences occur in the thalamus. To provide potential explanatory approaches for conflicting reports, we sorted the studies by species, employed methods as well as thalamic regions.

Inclusion criteria, definitions, and procedures

Animal models

Studies in species of three taxonomic orders were included in this work: (1) rodents, consisting of studies in rats; (2) carnivores, mainly consisting of studies in cats and occasional studies in dogs; and (3) primates, mainly consisting of investigations in representatives of the genus macaca and sporadic studies in other genera (e.g., squirrel monkeys). This allows for a coarse interspecies comparison of interactions in the thalamus. In rodents, we have limited ourselves to the example of studies in rats, as these are to date more extensively discussed; for work on anatomical organization in mice, please refer, e.g., to the Allen Mouse Connectivity Atlas (http://connectivity.brain-map.org) or, specifically, for basal ganglia and cerebellar interaction in mice, e.g., Chen et al. (2014), Neychev et al. (2008).

Projection pattern definition

We name regions that are innervated by a given structure the (terminal) territory of that structure (Percheron et al. 1996). Two territories within a thalamic nucleus are defined as being segregated if they are not innervated by the same region (Fig. 1a) and have no informational exchange with adjacent regions. Due to resolution by the applied methods, a differentiation from partial overlap especially in boundary regions is, however, discussable. To that end, it also needs to be noted that any empirical approaches for discovering connections have apertures that are tuned to particular scales and data features: a given method has only access to a limited range of features and can only partially capture connections at a certain level of detail, i.e., at a restricted level of organization. Hence, a distinct segregation, while theoretical possible, will be empirically hard to be evidenced. Converging projections are such in which a thalamic neuron receives input from both projection systems, i.e., projections end in the same terminal field; this overlap makes an informational exchange between two (or more) projections possible. Electrophysiological and electronmicroscopical studies provide solid means in the case of convergences being present, but due to relatively small sample sizes, the lack of convergences being observed does not prove that they indeed do not exist. Anatomical tracing studies employing light microscopy on the other hand provide a relatively complete picture of the projection territories, but cannot prove convergence in regions of overlapping territories due to the limited resolution. Therefore, we consider overlapping territories as being sites of potential convergence (Fig. 1b). Another putative pattern of interrelations between terminal territories is often termed “interdigitating” in the reviewed literature (Fig. 1c). Here, territories lie in close proximity and reach into each other, mostly without overlapping [cf. Fig. 5, left side in Sakai et al. (1996)], connections can form patches of varied regularity (Goldman-Rakic 1984), or their terminal fields can display striking interrelation. This projection pattern thus indicates rather segregated territories. On the other hand, convergences in border areas have been observed, and oligosynaptic communication via local interneurons, as well as modulatory effects via volume transmission, are also conceivable.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Projection patterns. Three principle projection patterns have been described in studies that examined the relation of cerebellar and ganglia territories: a two territories within a thalamic nucleus are defined as being segregated if they do not innervate the same region and have no informational exchange with adjacent regions. b Converging projections are discussed if projections appear in the same terminal field. c Interdigitating territories lie close together and reach into each other, mostly without overlapping

Thalamus nomenclature and nuclei border definitions

The thalamus can be divided into a multitude of different nuclei with varieties based on interspecies differences (Hassler 1950; Olszewski 1952; Snider and Niemer 1961; Pellegrino and Cushman 1967; Berman 1968; Schaltenbrand et al. 1977; Berman and Jones 1982; Paxinos and Watson 2006; Jones 2007), different approaches to define borders (Krauth et al. 2010)], and different schools of nomenclature (Dewulf and van Winsen 1971). Consequently, the attribution of interspecies equivalent regions is highly ambiguous. A list of nuclei and their interspecies equivalents, as well as synonyms between different nomenclatures can be found in Jones (2007). To enable a comparison of results in different species, we explored the nomenclature and border definitions of individual nuclei and their interspecies equivalents wherever possible. In general, two regions regarding inputs from cerebellum and basal ganglia can be recognized: (1) parts of the thalamus that lie medial to and include the internal medullary lamina; (2) parts of the thalamus that are situated laterally and anteriorly hereto. Figure 2 shows a schematically overview of the involved region, adapted from Nieuwenhuys et al. (2008). The latter is generally termed “motor thalamus” and consists of the ventral anterior and ventral lateral nuclei (VA–VL). VA–VL concavely clings to the medial thalamic region. The medial border is defined by the internal medullary lamina as well as anterior and lateral thalamic nuclei. Posteriorly, it transitions into the ventral posterior nucleus (VP). Based on Jones (2007), the following subnuclei are distinguished in primates. A ventral anterior nucleus (VA) contains a magnocellular (VAmc) component. VAmc misses in cats and dogs, and a paralaminar region (VApl) might represent its equivalent (Sakai and Patton 1993). Furthermore, a ventral lateral nucleus (VL) with an anterior (VLa; pallidal territory) and a posterior (VLp; cerebellar territory) component is distinguished. Olszewski (1952) divided the primate VLp into four further subdivisions: area X (X; anteromedial); pars postrema of ventral lateral nucleus (VLps; posterodorsal); the oral part of the ventral posterior lateral nucleus (VPLo; ventral); and a ventromedial nucleus (VM), which is sometimes also referred to as medial part of the ventral lateral nucleus (VLm). It is difficult to differentiate VM from other nuclei based on cytoarchitectonic features. Hence, the interspecies equivalent of this nucleus is controversial, some assigning it to VA, others to VL or VP (Jones 2007). In cats and dogs, a main division (VL) and a dorsal part (VLd) are distinguished (Sakai and Patton 1993). According to Jones (2007), VPLo represents the equivalent of the ventral intermediate nucleus (Vim) in humans (Hassler 1950), an important target in deep brain stimulation to alleviate symptoms in Parkinson´s disease. The relevant nuclei in the medial region comprise of the medial dorsal (MD) and intralaminar (IL) nuclei. The MD is situated centrally in the thalamus, almost completely surrounded by the internal medullary lamina. Only medially, it does border the third ventricle. The MD consists of three subdivisions: a medially situated magnocellular component (MDmc); a centrally located parvocellular part (MDpc), which constitutes the major portion of this nucleus; and a lateral division in close proximity to the internal medullary lamina. According to its location, this division is called “paralaminar” (MDpl). It is also marked by alternating patterns of complementary fiber densities that traverse this portion of the MD, hence inspiring the term “multiformis” (MDmf). Some authors also distinguish MDpl and MDmf as two distinct entities (Jones 2007). The intralaminar nuclei are a number of cell groups situated within the internal medullary lamina that exhibit a common ontogenetic development. These nuclei can be identified employing immunohistochemical labeling (calbindin, calretinin, parvalbumin, and acetylcholinesterase). They can be subdivided into rostral, central, and caudal groups. The centromedian (CM) and parafascicular (Pf) nuclei are designated as the central or caudal group, depending on how many groups are differentiated. These nuclei are commonly viewed as a complex (CM/Pf). CM misses in some species, for instance, rodents. The central lateral (CL), paracentral (Pc), and central medial (CeM) nuclei are ascribed to the rostral group.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Thalamic nuclei with cerebellar or pallidal afferents. In this horizontal view, nuclei receiving input from either the cerebellum, substantia nigra, or pallidum are highlighted in green. Modified after Nieuwenhuys (2008). Abbreviations of thalamic nuclei are based on Jones nomenclature (2007)

Study design of reviewed literature

Two general methods (i.e., anatomical and electrophysiological) have been widely used to uncover connectivity of projection systems. (1) Anatomical studies place lesions or tracers in the region of interest to label cell bodies, axons, and terminals (Morecraft et al. 2009). Most tracers prefer one direction of intracellular transport and either propagate towards the cell body (retrograde tracers, e.g., Fluoro-Gold) or towards the synapses (anterograde tracers, e.g., Phaseolus vulgaris-leucoagglutinin). Some tracers are bidirectional (e.g., wheat-germ agglutinin). A relatively recent development of this technique allows for labeling of multisynaptic connections via transneuronal tracers (e.g., rabies virus, HSV-1). Lesions show patterns of anterograde and retrograde degeneration, which can be labeled subsequently. Concerning the study design of anatomical studies, convergences were most commonly investigated interindividually. The comparison of projection territories was carried out by orientation via anatomical landmarks (e.g., cytoarchitectonics, stereotaxis). More convincing evidence can be found in intraindividual studies, in which several tracers are injected into different structures in the same animal. This approach allows for a direct comparison of the resulting labeling on the same or adjacent slices. These methods were most commonly combined with light microscopic analyses of the material. The limited resolution of light microscopy limits the power of evidence concerning the question of converging projections in regions in which projection territories come into close contact with each other, since synapses cannot be resolved. Relatively few studies employed electron microscopy to tackle this issue. Another limitation concerns the rate of tracer uptake into the neurons and the intracellular transport along the axon. Different tracers have been shown to have different sensitivities. This may explain some conflicting reports and might be indicative of an underestimation of projection territories in most anatomical studies. (2) Electrophysiological studies can reveal connectivity by stimulating one structure and measuring cell responses in another region using electrodes. By considering the approximate distance, the signal has to travel and the latency of the cell response, and one can determine whether a connection is monosynaptically activating postsynaptic ionotropic receptors (fast cell responses). Longer latencies might be caused by either oligosynaptic connections or slower cell responses elicited by intracellular signal cascades initiated by metabotropic receptors. Electrodes are usually placed using stereotactic coordinates and activity profiles that are specific for different regions. In extracellular single unit recording studies, electrodes are placed close to a neuron to measure its spiking behavior. A single unit is defined as a single firing neuron, whose spike potentials are distinctly isolated by a recording microelectrode. In intracellular single-unit studies, neurons are impaled by a microelectrode to measure the voltage change over time across the membrane during cell responses. This allows the spiking behavior of a neuron to be measured, as well as postsynaptic potentials that do not elicit an action potential. Accordingly, connections that do not elicit obvious changes in spiking behavior can also be revealed. In some studies, the exact localization of the investigated neurons is accurately determined by electrophoretic labeling or electrolytic lesions. Because these procedures are very intricate and time-consuming, sample sizes are often relatively small (Boulton et al. 1990).

Main output structures and their terminal territories

The cerebellum mediates the bulk of its output via the cerebellar nuclei, which lie deep in the cerebellar white matter (Asanuma et al. 1983a). Usually, four deep cerebellar nuclei (DCN) are distinguished: the dentate nucleus, which is situated laterally and is very large in primates; the medially situated fastigial nucleus; the interjacent interposed nuclei, which are distinguished by their relative position to each other; the anterior interposed (AIN); and posterior interposed (PIN) nuclei. The main cerebellar projection targets the VL. All DCN target this nucleus with different densities. At least in primates, the territories of the individual DCN are mainly segregated and display a somatotopic organization (Asanuma et al. 1983b). Further thalamic regions with cerebellar innervations are IL and MD. The basal ganglia structures are interconnected among each other and with other structures in a complex manner. Two grey matter regions constitute the main output structures and project mainly to the thalamus: GPi and SNr (Kuo and Carpenter 1973; Carpenter et al. 1976). In rodents, the entopeduncular nucleus is considered to be equivalent to the GPi and will hereafter be addressed as the GPi. The SNr mainly projects to VAmc, VLm, and MD (Carpenter et al. 1976) and, moreover, to the CM/Pf (Sidib et al. 2002). The GPi mainly projects to VL and VApc and in addition also to CM/Pf (Kuo and Carpenter 1973). The connections of the basal ganglia exhibit a topographical organization with motoric, associative, and limbic components (Ilinsky et al. 1985; Parent and Hazrati 1995a, b).

Evidence suggesting convergences

Rats, anatomical evidence

Anatomical studies in rats repeatedly provided evidence for overlapping territories, although the extent was evaluated differently. Such overlapping territories can mainly be assumed in nuclei such as CL and Pf [please note that in rats, a CM is not described; (Faull and Carman 1978; Di Chiara et al. 1979; Haroian et al. 1981; Gerfen et al. 1982; Wiklund and Cuénod 1984; Cornwall and Phillipson 1988; Deniau et al. 1992)], in MDpl (Di Chiara et al. 1979; Haroian et al. 1981; Gerfen et al. 1982) and in VM (Faull and Carman 1968, 1978; Di Chiara et al. 1979; Herkenham and Nauta 1979; Kultas-Ilinsky et al. 1980; Gerfen et al. 1982; Deniau et al. 1992). Furthermore, interdigitating projections have been described in VA–VL. These are located in adjacent regions of cerebellar and pallidal territories (Deniau et al. 1992). Convergences in VA–VL are, therefore, likely to be limited to border regions of such interdigitations. Deniau et al. (1992) adopted a multitracer approach to investigate the relation of cerebellar and basal ganglia territories. They mainly found the territories to be segregated, but in VM and Pf, these consistently overlapped. The cerebellar innervation of these nuclei was significantly less dense. Terminations seemed to occur via thin axon collaterals in an en passant fashion. Investigations of afferent projections to Pf (Cornwall and Phillipson 1988) and VM (Herkenham and Nauta 1979) employing retrograde tracers found labeled neurons mainly in GPi and SNr. In both instances, labeled neurons were also found in the DCN. Recent histochemical evidence supports the notion of complementary cerebellar and basal ganglial territories in that glutamate and GABA receptors were found in complementary densities in the thalamus (Kuramoto et al. 2010). The presented evidence suggests that territories of cerebellar and basal ganglia projections are mainly segregated in the rat thalamus. Regions of overlap (border regions of VA–VL, Pf, and VM) exhibit clear dominance of one projection system.

Rats, electrophysiological evidence

Evidence for converging projections of basal ganglia and cerebellum in rodents can be found in several electrophysiological studies (MacLeod et al. 1980; Chevalier and Deniau 1982; Buee et al. 1986; MacLeod and James 1984). MacLeod et al. (1980) placed stimulating electrodes in various areas and extracellularly recorded the spiking behavior in VM and Pf neurons. Stimulated regions included SNr and the contralateral superior cerebellar peduncle (brachium conjunctivum)—which carries the bulk of cerebellar efferents, including those to the thalamus. In this study, the brachium conjunctivum was stimulated to establish a stable activity in VM and Pf neurons, which otherwise exhibited a low level of spontaneous activity. Thus, the excitatory effect of cerebellar projections embodied the basis of observations made in this study. Under such conditions, intranigral applications of muscimol (GABA-agonist) led to a significant increase of spiking activity in VM and Pf neurons. Conversely, intranigral bicucullin (GABA-antagonist) increased nigral activity that inhibited the excitatory drive elicited by the superior cerebellar peduncle stimulation. A study with similar experimental design found very similar results (Buee et al. 1986). These studies, however, cannot determine whether such interactions are monosynaptic or mediated in other ways, such as local interneurons or alternative pathways (e.g., via superior colliculus). Other observations support the notion of monosynaptic convergences (MacLeod and James 1984): here, individual VM neurons were characterized by antidromical stimulations. Of 500 investigated VM neurons, approximately 72% were innervated by the DCN and exhibited an excitatory effect. Approximately 88% were innervated by SNr and exhibited an inhibitory effect. These observations suggest substantial convergences (at least ~ 22%) with significant physiological relevance in rat VM and most likely also Pf. VM neurons with cerebellar input displayed short latencies following stimulations in SNr, reticular formation, superior colliculus, and frontal cortical areas. Confirmatory evidence was reported by Chevalier and Deniau (1982): in similar experiments with extracellular as well as intracellular recordings, they found converging inputs from DCN and SNr in ~ 58% of investigated neurons. The evidence presented above strongly suggests significant interactions of cerebellum and basal ganglia (i.e., SNr) in VM and Pf of rats. Regarding potential convergences of cerebellar and pallidal output, no studies in rats were found.

Cats and dogs, anatomical evidence

VA–VL and VM

In cats, territories of basal ganglia were found mainly anteriorly and laterally, whereas cerebellar territories were situated more posteriorly and medially. Pallidal and cerebellar territories interdigitated with minor overlaps (Ilinsky and Kultas-Ilinsky 1984). However, data from intraindividual tracing studies that pursue a multi-tracer approach are missing. In dogs, such techniques were employed to investigate the relation of cerebellar and nigral territories in the thalamus (Sakai and Patton 1993). The findings support the notion of interdigitating territories of cerebellar and basal ganglia projections with minor overlap in VA–VL of cats and dogs. Evidence for overlapping territories in the thalamus was provided by a retrograde tracing study in cats: Anderson and DeVito (1987) placed multiple very small tracer injections in different regions of VA–VL, VM, IL, and MDpl. They found the following patterns of retrogradely labeled neurons: (1) injections in dorsal parts of VL (VLd, n = 3) labeled numerous neurons in DCN (338–1089 neurons), few-to-moderate amounts in GPi (2-318 neurons), and no-to-moderate amounts in SNr (0-102 neurons); (2) injections in caudal and caudolateral VL (VLc, n = 6) labeled few to moderate amounts of neurons in DCN (21–506 neurons), no-to-moderate amounts in GPi (0–94 neurons), and none up to few in SNr (0–2 neurons); (3) injections in CL and MDpl (n = 1) labeled few neurons in DCN (14 neurons), moderate amounts in GPi (116 neurons), and few in SNr (4 neurons); (4) injections in VM (n = 2) labeled few-to-moderate amounts of neurons in DCN (10–72 neurons), few-to-moderate amounts in GPi (9–86 neurons), and moderate amounts in SNr (82–107 neurons). We considered the relative frequencies of labeled neurons between these structures in selected experiments of this study to reveal the complementary relation of these projections in the different nuclei: (1) VLd: BG 0.28, DCN 0.72; (2) VLc: BG 0.21, DCN 0.79; (3) IL: BG 0.9, DCN 0.1; and (4) VM: BG 0.73, DCN 0.27. These observations might be interpreted as evidence for overlapping territories with complementary projection densities. However, tracer placement in regions of interdigitating territories would label the projecting structures, even if no overlap was present. This especially concerns the injections into VLd and VLc. (Ilinsky and Kultas-Ilinsky 1984) describe the relationship of pallidal and cerebellar territories in an interindividual anterograde tracing study in cats as being complementary in the whole thalamus. However, they admit that a small zone of overlap in border regions of the territories cannot be ruled out by this evidence. Similar evidence was provided by another interindividual anterograde tracing study in cats with injections in DCN, GPi, and SNr (Hendry et al. 1979). It is worth mentioning that in this study, no projections of the DCN to VM were found, contradicting evidence from other studies in cats (Kultas-Ilinsky et al. 1978; Sugimoto et al. 1981; Anderson and DeVito 1987; Ilinsky and Kultas-Ilinsky 1984). In dogs, the VM was found to be a site of clearly overlapping cerebellar and nigral territories [see Fig. 4 in Sakai and Patton (1993)]. Furthermore, it was identified as being the main territory of fastigial projections in dogs (Person et al. 1986; Sugimoto et al. 1981; Jimenez-Castellanos and Reinoso-Su rez 1985). One could hypothesize that these inconsistencies might be due to an incomplete coverage of the DCN, differences in nomenclature or interspecies differences in dogs and cats. Indeed, the fastigial nucleus has been neglected in some studies without addressing this issue sufficiently (Ilinsky and Kultas-Ilinsky 1984). Other studies did not show the injection sites (Hendry et al. 1979). Thus, it is not clear to what extent the discrepancy between observations can be attributed to an omission of the fastigial nucleus. For a detailed discussion, see the section, “Interspecies differences—VM”.

IL and MD

In an interindividual anterograde tracing study with injections in SNr, GPi, and DCN, nigral projections to CL, Pc, and CeM and pallidal projections to CM were found to be clearly overlapping with cerebellar projections (Hendry et al. 1979). We propose a potential involvement of MDpl as well, based on reevaluations of the presented figures and the consideration of the difficult distinction between CL and MDpl [(Hendry et al. 1979), please see also Figs. 1, 2 and 3 in Mai and Forutan (2012)]. Retrograde tracing studies in CM/Pf (Royce et al. 1991), Pc, and CL (Anderson and DeVito 1987; Niimi et al. 1990), as well as MD (Ono and Niimi 1986; Anderson and DeVito 1987) support these observations. Projections arising from SNr and GPi were always found to be stronger than those from the cerebellum. This suggests a complementary projection pattern of cerebellar and basal ganglia structures. Furthermore, cerebellar projections seem to invade basal ganglia territories rather than reversed, possibly indicating a cerebellar modulation of restricted regions of basal ganglia territories in the thalamus. Some authors express doubt concerning the labeling in CM and argue that these might represent passing fibers rather than terminating projections (Carpenter et al. 1958; McMasters and Russell 1958; Sugimoto et al. 1981; Ono and Niimi 1986; Ilinsky and Kultas-Ilinsky 1984). Based on this interpretation, these authors conclude that territories of cerebellum and basal ganglia are also segregated in the IL. In contrast, retrograde tracing studies are indicative that terminations do exist. It is conceivable that axons emit collaterals during their course through the CM/Pf. Such collaterals of pallidal projections terminating in the CM/Pf have been shown in intracellular tracings (Arecchi-Bouchhioua et al. 1997). Furthermore, en passant contacts could exist (Sakai et al. 1996). It is possible that certain anterograde tracers employed are not sufficiently sensitive to reveal such filigree structures. Various tracer substances do differ substantially in this regard (Royce et al. 1991). Since the presented anterograde investigations were conducted inter-individually, and no intra-individual comparison via multi tracer approach is available, it is not possible to reach a final conclusion regarding the existence of overlap in MD and IL of cats. Morphological studies in cats (Yamamoto et al. 1985; Tseng and Royce 1986) and primates (Pearson et al. 1984) indicate that a subpopulation of IL neurons—especially in CM/Pf—have dendrites with up to 1 mm length (Tseng and Royce 1986). This circumstance increases the likelihood of converging signals in this region. In dogs, cerebellar projections to CM/Pf and posterior parts of MD have been described (Person et al. 1986; Sakai and Patton 1993). It is unclear if such conflicting observations are due to interspecies differences or methodological limitations. An evaluation of this question is impeded by a very limited number of studies in dogs. Evidence suggests mainly segregated territories of cerebellar, nigral, and pallidal projections in VA–VL of cats. Overlap, if any, was only found in adjacent border regions of the territories. In dogs, VM is highly likely to be a site of convergence of nigral and cerebellar projections. Evidence in cats does not support this observation, although this might be due to methodological issues. The IL and MDpl of cats are consistently described as being regions of overlapping territories of cerebellar and basal ganglial projections. Projections arising from SNr and GPi were always found to be stronger than those from the cerebellum. Data in dogs are very limited.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Relation of projections from cerebellum and basal ganglia in the thalamus. In this schematic, we summarize the relation of the projection territories of cerebellum and basal ganglia to each other. Cerebellar territories are colored orange, basal ganglia ones green and regions with potentially converging territories are indicated by fasciated patterns. The interdigitating projection territories in VA–VL lead to the anteroposterior density gradient described by Sakai (1996). Interspecies differences as well as projection densities are not accounted for in this schematic

Cats and dogs, electrophysiological evidence

Two of the electrophysiological studies in cats reviewed here did not find any evidence for converging projections of basal ganglia and cerebellum on thalamic neurons (Ueki 1983; Ueki et al. 1977). Stimulating electrodes were placed bilaterally in the superior cerebellar peduncle as well as unilaterally in GPi and/or SNr. Several electrodes were placed in SNr and GPi to optimize coverage. In this way, 75 (Ueki et al. 1977) and 42 (Ueki 1983) VA–VL and VM neurons were recorded in several animals. Neurons with monosynaptic cerebellar input were frequently found to be interdigitating with ones that received monosynaptic pallidal or nigral input. Since in these studies, the sample sizes were relatively small—especially when considering the size of the investigated regions—it cannot be excluded that convergences were simply overlooked. A number of studies in cats contradict these findings and report convergences with varying degrees (Desiraju and Purpura 1969; Uno and Yoshida 1975; Deniau et al. 1978; Uno et al. 1978; Yamamoto et al. 1984; Gazzara et al. 1986). These studies also focus on VA–VL and VM. In two studies with similar experimental design, Uno and colleagues found monosynaptic convergences in ~ 5.6% (n = 89) and ~ 3.7% (n = 218) of investigated neurons in border regions of pallidal and cerebellar territories in VA–VL (Uno and Yoshida 1975; Uno et al. 1978). They unilaterally stimulated GPi with six electrodes and the superior cerebellar peduncle bilaterally with two electrodes; recordings were conducted throughout VA–VL. Similar results were found by Yamamoto et al. (1984) in investigations of VA–VL as well as VM (~ 3.7%; n = 163). In addition, Gazzara et al. (1986) reported a similar relation (~ 3%, n = 32). The latter study also quantified convergences with longer latencies, which were found substantially more often (~ 53%; n = 32). Long latencies were interpreted to represent oligosynaptic connections. Another study reported markedly larger numbers of monosynaptic convergences in VL (~ 20%; n = 60) and convergences with longer latencies were found “in a somewhat larger portion of VL neurons” (Desiraju and Purpura 1969). Longer latencies were also interpreted as oligosynaptic connections. We propose to also consider the possibility that such longer latencies might occur due to intracellular signal cascades mediated by G-protein-coupled receptors. Furthermore, it cannot be excluded that glutamate is released via volume transmission or spillover, diffusing to and activating nearby synapses (Agnati et al. 2010; Okubo and Iino 2011); for discussion see the section, “physiological relevance”. Two of the studies reviewed here addressed nigral and cerebellar convergences. One did not find any convergences (Ueki 1983), which is in stark contrast to observations reported by Deniau et al. (1978). They found approximately 27% (n = ~ 100) of relay neurons in VL to receive monosynaptic input both from DCN and SNr. No studies were found in either cats or dogs that investigated convergences in IL or MD. However, stimulations of the fastigial nucleus have been shown to elicit spiking activity in CM/Pf (Bava et al. 1967). Approximately 69% of investigated neurons that exhibited spontaneous activity exhibited such responses. Convergence of pallidal and cerebellar projections on neurons of VA–VL and VM in cats has been shown repeatedly, albeit with notably differing degrees; convergences of nigral with cerebellar projections have been shown but are controversial. Convergences in MD and IL have not been shown, but some data indicate that such convergences are likely to exist in CM/Pf. Equivalent electrophysiological data in the dog are virtually missing.

Primates, anatomical evidence

VA–VL

Sakai et al. (1996) compared cerebellothalamic and pallidothalamic projections in macaca fuscata by injecting two anterograde tracers simultaneously in the same individual. These were subsequently labeled in the same section, which allows for a direct comparison of terminal territories. Large injections in DCN and GPi revealed three general patterns of labeling. First, dense pallidal labeling was found in VApc and VLo, whereas dense cerebellar labeling was situated in VPLo and X. Second, interdigitating patterns of cerebellar and pallidal labeling were found in the transitional zones of both territories. Third, the density of labeling displayed a complementary anteroposterior gradient [see Fig. 5, left in Sakai et al. (1996)]. Overlap was observed occasionally, but interdigitations that indicated segregated territories predominated. Because such interdigitations were found frequently, the question arises whether dendrites of thalamic neurons in those border zones invade both territories. The functional significance of these interdigitations is entirely unknown. Supporting evidence was reported in an earlier study that also described an anteroposterior gradient of pallidal and cerebellar territories. They also employed two tracers in the same individual to compare these territories, but tracers were labeled on successive slices, providing slightly less reliable data. The extent of overlap in VA–VL was estimated at approximately 2.5 mm² [see Fig. 15 E in Rouiller et al. (1994)]. This evidence is contrasted by a number of interindividual comparisons—employing a single tracer substance—concluding that the territories of basal ganglia and cerebellum are segregated (Kuo and Carpenter 1973; Asanuma et al. 1983a; Schell and Strick 1984; Ilinsky and Kultas-Ilinsky 1987; Percheron et al. 1996): Percheron et al. (1996) investigated thalamic projection territories by injecting an anterograde tracer into DCN, GPi, and SNr; intersubject comparisons of territories were carried out using stereotactic orientation techniques. Intersubject variabilities cannot be accounted for in this manner. Other studies with similar experimental design used cytoarchitectonic landmarks as a tool for intersubject territory comparison (Nauta and Mehler 1966; Kuo and Carpenter 1973; Carpenter et al. 1976; Kim et al. 1976; Stanton 1980; Kalil 1981; Asanuma et al. 1983a; Ilinsky and Kultas-Ilinsky 1987; Schell and Strick 1984). These studies distinguished a “cell sparse zone” (X, VPLo, and VLc) from other regions of VA–VL with higher cell densities (especially VLo). The cell sparse zone was defined as the cerebellar territory and was used as a landmark to compare pallidal and cerebellar territories. Regarding this approach of orientation, Sakai et al. (1996) emphasized that they didn´t find such a clear association of territories to the cytoarchitectonic landscape. This view is reinforced by findings in electrophysiological investigations (Nambu et al. 1988). Hence, the interindividual comparison of territories by means of stereotactic as well as cytoarchitectonic orientation might have led to underestimations of overlap in these studies. Moreover, careful inspection of the pictorial material provided by Percheron et al. (1996) also indicates overlap—especially in border regions of cerebellar and pallidal territories [please see Fig. 16 in Percheron et al. (1996)].

VLm

Anterograde tracing studies reported nigral (Carpenter et al. 1976) and pallidal (Kuo and Carpenter 1973; Inase and Tanji 1995) efferents to VLm; cerebellar fibers mainly traverse this nucleus, terminations are less obvious (Stanton 1980; Kalil 1981; Sakai et al. 1996). If territories of cerebellar and basal ganglia projections overlap in this nucleus, these are likely to be sparse. This is peculiar, since in rats, cats, and dogs, VM is a conspicuous candidate to be a site of convergence of cerebellar and basal ganglial territories. This discrepancy will be discussed in detail in the “Interspecies differences”.

IL and MD

Pallidal and nigral projections to primate CM/Pf are well known (Sidibé et al. 1997; Sidib et al. 2002; Parent et al. 1983). Labeling of cerebellar axons in CM/Pf was described to be mainly fibers of passage on their way to CL (Kalil 1981; Ilinsky and Kultas-Ilinsky 1987). Some authors also consider the possibility of some cerebellar terminations, although sparse (Kusama et al. 1971; Sakai et al. 1996, 2000; Rouiller et al. 1994; Chan-Palay 1977; Percheron 1977). Electronmicroscopic investigations of CM following lesions in the DCN did not find any degenerated terminals (Harding 1973). Cerebellar projections to CL were frequently shown (Kalil 1981; Asanuma et al. 1983a; Ilinsky and Kultas-Ilinsky 1987; Percheron et al. 1996), terminating in “patchy clusters” (Sakai et al. 1996). Admittedly, no study in primates reported projections of the basal ganglia to that nucleus. However, a number of anterograde tracing studies reported nigral terminals in the adjacent MDpl (Carpenter et al. 1976; Ilinsky et al. 1985; Percheron et al. 1996; Sidib et al. 2002). The differentiation of MDpl and CL has been assessed to be very difficult (Jones 2007; Mai and Forutan 2012). Furthermore, cerebellar projections to MDpl were also found (Ilinsky and Kultas-Ilinsky 1987; Yamamoto et al. 1992; Sakai et al. 1996). Some overlap in the region of CL and MDpl is, therefore, likely. Although a numerous amount of studies postulate a strong segregation of the BG and cerebellar system, some studies have found existing interdigitating patterns, indicating a potential informational exchange between these two systems; on a closer look, even some studies have demonstrated overlap regions of pallidal and cerebellar projections; this was specifically located in border regions between both territories.

Primates, electrophysiological evidence

Electrophysiological investigations regarding convergences of cerebellar and pallidal projections to the thalamus focused on the motor thalamus (VA–VL). Some studies found neurons with converging input, albeit very few [between ~ 0.5%, n = 180 and ~ 3.4%, n = 86; Yamamoto et al. (1983); Nambu et al. (1988); Jinnai et al. (1993)]. In these studies, stimulations covered the DCN and GPi as much as possible. Multiple neurons were recorded along the electrode track through various regions of VA–VL and tested for monosynaptic responsiveness following GPi and DCN stimulation. Two studies did not find any neurons with converging input (Anderson and Turner 1991; Buford et al. 1996). Anderson and Turner (1991) confined the stimulated regions in GPi and superior cerebellar peduncle to those that were involved in arm movements. An observation in this study that conflicts with common findings was the very small number of neurons receiving pallidal input in relation with those with cerebellar input. In addition, Buford et al. (1996) focused on regions that are associated with arm movements. They stimulated regions in GPi and VA–VL, aiming to distinguish cerebellar and pallidal territories based on their “micro excitability”, i.e., eliciting an arm movement following a stimulus. Micro-excitable regions were assumed to be cerebellar territories. Indeed, an injection of a retrograde tracer into a micro-excitable region that was directly adjacent to a region with pallidal input (border region of VLc and VLo) did label neurons in DCN but not GPi. This evidence rather supports the notion that the interdigitating projection pattern in VA–VL resembles segregated territories. Regions for which anatomical evidence of cerebellar interactions with nigral projections exists (especially MDpl and surrounding regions) have not yet been examined with electrophysiological methods. One study found that nigral stimulation elicited spiking behavior in lateral and paralaminar regions of MD (Tanibuchi et al. 2009). Convergences with cerebellar input to this area, therefore, seem likely.

Interspecies differences

Observations regarding the question as to whether thalamic efferents of the main basal ganglia output structures (GPi/SNr) converge with cerebellar output mediated by the DCN are partially conflicting.

VA–VL and VM

Projections from cerebellum and basal ganglia to VA–VL were found to be mainly segregated in rats, cats, dogs as well as primates. Comparisons of pallidal and cerebellar territories in primates employing anterograde tracing techniques revealed a complementary anteroposterior density gradient and interdigitating projection patterns [Fig. 2 and 6; Rouiller et al. (1994); Sakai et al. 1996]. Such interdigitations were also found in cats (Ilinsky and Kultas-Ilinsky 1984), dogs (Sakai and Patton 1993), and rats (Deniau et al. 1992). These observations indicate a consistency of this organizational principle in VA–VL across different taxonomical orders. It is controversial which regions in the primate thalamus correspond to the VM of rats, cats, and dogs (Jones 2007). Some authors designated VM to the “unspecific” nuclei and, therefore, being related to the intralaminar nuclei, because it diffusely projects to lamina I of large portions of the cerebral cortex (Krettek and Price 1977; Herkenham and Nauta 1979). In primates, VLm was proposed as being its equivalent (Olszewski 1952), other authors assigned it to a rostral nucleus (Percheron et al. 1996), medial portions of VA (Paxinos et al. 1999), or VP (Jones 2007). We assume that medial portions of VL (VLm) in primates represent the interspecies equivalent of the rat VM. No electrophysiological study in primates found convergences in that region that exhibited a greater extent than was found in the remaining VA–VL [< 4%; Uno and Yoshida (1975); Yamamoto et al. (1983); Nambu et al. (1988); Anderson and Turner (1991); Jinnai et al. (1993); Buford et al. (1996)]. In addition, anatomical studies employing tracing techniques did not provide any evidence of substantial overlap in VLm. Contrasting observations were made in the rat VM, revealing notable overlap of cerebellar and nigral territories (Faull and Carman 1968; Di Chiara et al. 1979; Herkenham and Nauta 1979; Kultas-Ilinsky et al. 1980; Gerfen et al. 1982; Deniau et al. 1992). Nigral projections dominated this nucleus, whereas cerebellar fibers were found to be thinner and less dense. Electrophysiological studies support these findings (MacLeod et al. 1980; Chevalier and Deniau 1982; Buee et al. 1986; MacLeod and James 1984). Evidence from anatomical studies in dogs forms a similar picture (Person et al. 1986; Sakai and Patton 1993). In cats, anatomical as well as electrophysiological evidence is heterogeneous, but rather suggests limited convergences comparable to ratios in primates (Hendry et al. 1979; Anderson and DeVito 1987; Desiraju and Purpura 1969; Chevalier and Deniau 1982; Yamamoto et al. 1984; Gazzara et al. 1986; Ilinsky and Kultas-Ilinsky 1984). On the basis of the existing studies, we assume that the amount of convergences in VA–VL has diminished from rodents > carnivores > primates. Because intraorder variabilities do exist, one should be careful to extrapolate findings concerning the amount of convergences in one representative of a taxonomic order to another (e.g., macaca to human). The available evidence is not sufficient to draw a final conclusion in this matter: in rats, no electrophysiological studies examining pallidal and cerebellar interactions were conducted; investigations in mice seem to be completely missing and would be valuable to allow for further comparison of species within one taxonomic order; in cats, studies employing multiple tracers to intraindividually compare terminal territories are missing; electrophysiological evidence is missing in dogs; in primates, and electrophysiological evidence should be extended, especially concerning nigral and cerebellar interactions in the VA–VL.

IL

Besides the anterior group (CL, Pc, and CeM), this particularly holds for the caudal group (CM/Pf). Cerebellar projections to the anterior group have frequently been shown in rats (Faull and Carman 1978; Haroian et al. 1981), cats (Hendry et al. 1979; Anderson and DeVito 1987; Niimi et al. 1990), dogs (Person et al. 1986; Sakai and Patton 1993), and primates (Kalil 1981; Asanuma et al. 1983a; Ilinsky and Kultas-Ilinsky 1987; Percheron et al. 1996). Indications of nigral projections to this region can be found in rats (Gerfen et al. 1982), cats (Hendry et al. 1979; Anderson and DeVito 1987; Niimi et al. 1990), and dogs (Sakai and Patton 1993), but not in primates. It is possible that missing observations of such projections in primates can be ascribed to the difficult differentiation of CL and MDpl (Jones 2007; Mai and Forutan 2012). Nigral and cerebellar projections to the primate MDpl are well described (see below). In cats and primates, studies employing multiple tracers and electrophysiological techniques to determine cerebellar and nigral territories in the same animal are missing. Indications of convergence in the rat Pf can be frequently found in anatomical (Faull and Carman 1978; Di Chiara et al. 1979; Haroian et al. 1981; Deniau et al. 1992; Gerfen et al. 1982; Wiklund and Cuénod 1984; Cornwall and Phillipson 1988) and electrophysiological studies (MacLeod et al. 1980; MacLeod and James 1984). In cats (Carpenter et al. 1958; McMasters and Russell 1958; Hendry et al. 1979; Sugimoto et al. 1981; Royce et al. 1991; Ilinsky and Kultas-Ilinsky 1984) and primates (Kusama et al. 1971; Chan-Palay 1977; Percheron 1977; Rouiller et al. 1994; Sakai et al. 1996, 2000), similar evidence points towards CM. Since no CM is defined in rats, such observations indicate that Pf constitutes equivalent connectivities and functions. Some studies in cats, however, reported cellular responses in this region that were elicited by cerebellar (Bava et al. 1967, 1979) and nigral (Gauchy et al. 1983; Miyamoto and Jinnai 1994) stimulation.

MD

In all species included in this review, convergences of cerebellar and nigral projections were likely to be found in lateral regions of MD (MDpl, MDmf, and MDpc). Anatomical studies in rats (Haroian et al. 1981; Gerfen et al. 1982), cats (Ono and Niimi 1986), dogs (Person et al. 1986), and primates (Carpenter et al. 1976; Stanton 1980; Asanuma et al. 1983a; Ilinsky et al. 1985; Ilinsky and Kultas-Ilinsky 1987; Yamamoto et al. 1992; Percheron et al. 1996) provide evidence that indicates substantial projections arising from these structures. In cats, these projections were described to be arranged in a complementary fashion with cerebellar projections being located posteriorly and those arising from SNr anteriorly (Ono and Niimi 1986). In primates, such an arrangement was not described (Yamamoto et al. 1992; Tanibuchi et al. 2009). Further indications suggesting convergence exist when considering the difficult differentiation of MDpl and CL (Jones 2007; Mai and Forutan 2012). In this way, conflicting reports from electrophysiological studies might be explained that found neuronal responses in CL but not MD following stimulations of the fastigial nucleus (Bava et al. 1967). The frequently described cerebellar projections to CL very likely also invade the MDpl (Person et al. 1986). The nigral territory in MD was recently described employing electrophysiological techniques (Tanibuchi et al. 2009). Investigations specifically exploring potential interactions of SNr and DCN in the MD are lacking in all animals included in this review.

Studies in the human species

As in vivo tracer studies are not applicable in humans, evidence on thalamic interconnectivity with the cerebellum and basal ganglia in humans is rather sparse. Few studies either relate to either postmortem studies or MRI-based diffusion tractography.

An influencial postmortem study in the human brain regarding cerebello- and basal ganglia-thalamic projections is reported by Gallay et al. (2008). Using multiple immunohistochemical staining procedures, these authors demonstrate that the majority of the cerebellothalamic and pallidothalamic tracts are clearly separated relative to their thalamic entrance; yet, exchanges between the two fiber systems could not be completely excluded. At subthalamic level, however, evidence for a clearer separation between the two tracts was apparent. At thalamic level, Kultas-Ilinsky et al. (2011) recently also provided data on basis of GAD65 immunoreactivity for GABAergic nigro- and pallido-thalamic as well as cerebellar afferent territories in different motor nuclei of the human thalamus; in principle, the gross distribution pattern of GAD65 was similar in human thalami to those in resus monkeys. This view, however, has been challenged by multiple studies in non-human primates (see above): evidence for a convergence of pallido- and cerebello-thalamic fibers in the same thalamic nuclei on basis of anatomical tracing has been demonstrated only by experimental studies in monkeys yet (Rouiller et al. 1994; Sakai et al. 1996; Sakai et al. 1999, 2000). Whether this incongruence is due to methodological limitation of post-mortem studies (e.g., Schmierer et al. 2010; Stüber et al. 2014; van Duijn et al. 2011), or indeed, a divergence in the organization in the human brain has to be investigated by alternative approaches.

Diffusion MRI and tractography provides such an alternative, yet less well validated method to identifying anatomical connections in the living human brain (for further detail and a discussion on limitations, see Jbabdi et al. 2015; Lerch et al. 2017). Briefly, in the analysis of connectivity distributions, two different approaches can be taken: (1) tractography-based parcellation as recently shown for the thalamic connections with the cerebellum and basal ganglia in (Lambert et al. 2016) or (2) characterization of projections and atlas-based assessment of connectivity fingerprints per thalamic regions as recently demonstrated by our group (Pelzer et al. 2017).

Parcellation approaches as those applied by Lambert et al. (2016) have the inherent limitation of grouping connections into segregated subdivisions (according to the principle “the winner takes it all”): Putative regions of convergence cannot be deduced from this. The authors report cerebellar connections in the posterior-anterior group (consisting of lateral posterior nucleus and medial pulvinar) and the ventrolateral thalamic group. Connections of the thalamus with the basal ganglia were associated with the mediodorsal group and the ventroanterior group. Cerebellar connections to the thalamus, however, did not exhibit the typical fiber crossing at the decussation of the superior cerebellar peduncle, which is not in line with the actual anatomical literature and calls validity into question [see Fig. 6h in Lambert et al. (2016)].

Conversely, we (Pelzer et al. 2017) demonstrated a decreasing anterior-to-posterior gradient for pallido-thalamic connections in: (1) the ventral-anterior thalamus; (2) the intralaminar nuclei; and (3) midline regions. Moreover, we presented evidence for a decreasing posterior-to-anterior gradient for dentato-thalamic projections predominantly in: (1) the ventral–lateral and posterior nucleus; (2) dorsal parts of the intralaminar nuclei and the subparafascicular nucleus, and (3) the medioventral and lateral mediodorsal nucleus. By our approach, a considerable overlap of connectivity pattern between basal ganglia and cerebellum was apparent in intralaminar nuclei and midline regions. Yet, also here, results need to be carefully considered with uncertainty due to limitations of the applied technique, diffusion tractography: the approach cannot, by principle, differentiate converging synapses from interdigitating projection patterns, and it does neither lead to discriminate direction of information transfer nor to determine an axonal termination. Still, it greatly holds promise to define thalamic territories that are likely for projection overlaps.

Nevertheless, next to putatively overlapping regions, however, also direct communication in the human brain between basal ganglia and cerebellum, via the internal part of the pallidum, has been described by (Milardi et al. 2016; Pelzer et al. 2013), which is in accordance with transneuronal tracing studies in nonhuman primate studies (Hoshi et al. 2005).

Organizational principles of territories

The key results of our extensive literature research are summed up in Fig. 3. Based on our detailed inspection of the enlisted studies above, we propose two different organization principles: (1) a network specific convergence and (2) a “core and shell” projection.

Network-specific convergences

Network-specific convergence is a general principal in information transfer; here, only one “pipe” is used to deliver all forms of communication. This means that all neurons that are involved in, e.g., the oculomotor network project to one “pipe” that initiates, for example, the movement of the eye. There is evidence to suggest that the oculomotor network receives substantial converging input from cerebellum and basal ganglia. Such a network comprises structures of the brainstem (reticular formation, superior colliculus, and red nucleus), thalamus (CM/Pf, MD), basal ganglia, and cortex (frontal eye fields). Nigral and cerebellar projections have been shown to converge on neurons of the superior colliculus in the rat (Westby et al. 1994). Similarly, these two structures seem to interact in the reticular formation (Shammah-Lagnado et al. 1983, 1987, 1992; Schneider et al. 1985) and the nucleus of Darkschewitsch, which has been associated with the red nucleus based on its similar connectivity patterns (Onodera and Hicks 2009, 1995, 1998). The involved nuclei in the thalamus—that is, MDpl and CM/Pf—receive input from all structures mentioned here in the context of the assumed oculomotor network (Ono and Niimi 1986; Royce et al. 1991; MacLeod and James 1984) and project to the frontal eye fields (Sadikot and Rymar 2009; Tanaka and Kunimatsu 2011). Furthermore, monosynaptic projections arising in SN pars compacta and the ventral tegmental area have been shown to project to the vermis of the cerebellum, which is associated with oculomotor function (Ikai et al. 1992; Melchitzky 2000). Another potential network receiving converging inputs on a subcortical level might be associated with facial movements (Bava et al. 1979; Pong et al. 2008).

“Core and shell” organization

Another discussable idea is the one of a “core and shell” projection pattern. Years ago, Berkley (1983) introduced this idea for the thalamic region VPL; this region could not be sharply separated from each other or from more rostral regions by their pattern of afferent input, their efferent output or their cytoarchitecture; he defined it as consisting of (1) an inner “core” that receives dense afferents that exclusively arise from one projection system and (2) an outer “shell” that is characterized by decreasing projection densities and an intermingling with afferents of other projection systems [see Fig. 6 in Berkley (1983)]. A “core and shell” organization seems to be present in cerebellar and pallidal projection territories in VA–VL: regions with high projection densities that are virtually exclusive to one projection system are found in the bigger parts of VA (pallidal) and VL (cerebellar). An anteroposterior density gradient has been described that suggests a successive blending of cerebellar and pallidal territories. This blending is characterized by interdigitating projection patterns that only overlap sparsely in adjacent border regions [Fig. 1 B/C; Sakai et al. (1996); Rouiller et al. (1994)]. Such observations support the notion of a “core and shell” organization of these output systems of basal ganglia and cerebellum.

Extrathalamic cerebello-BG communication

Next to the described “network specific convergence” and “core and shell” organization principle, which may exist inside the thalamus, it has to be considered that also extrathalamic regions with cerebellar and basal ganglia projections may be part of a subcortical communication. To this end, one imminent region is the subthalamic region, which embraces the subthalamic nucleus, zona incerta, Forel’s fields (H, H1, and H2) and their nucleus (McKenzie 2009). Both, the cerebellum and the basal ganglia, project to this subthalamic region. For the subthalamic nucleus, recent findings reveal the existence of an anatomical substrate for the bidirectional communication between cerebellum and basal ganglia, as shown in rats (Ichinohe et al. 2000) and monkeys (Bostan et al. 2010, 2013; Bostan and Strick 2010) sending disynaptic projections to the cerebellar cortex by way of the pontine nuclei. Similar evidence has been recently reported in the human brain (Pelzer et al. 2013; Milardi et al. 2016), consciously that these techniques are not able to dissolve direct synaptic relay. Next to a direct basal ganglia-cerebellar communication, the dentate nucleus sends efferent projections to the subthalamic region via red nucleus. In the rostral part of the red nucleus, a large bundle of fibers turns dorso-rostrally away from the ascending limb of the crossed superior cerebellar peduncle. The bundle sends some fibers to the external lamina of the thalamus and the rest traverse the centrum medianum and parafascicular nucleus to continue in the internal lamina of the thalamus. Further rostrally, another large bundle turns away from the ascending limb; these traverses the prerubral field and Forel’s field (H1). The fibers from the dentate nucleus pass the dorsal part of the red nucleus, reach the pre-rubral field, and enter the ventral part of the VL nucleus of the thalamus, and finally terminate in the ventral anterior nucleus (VA). A small fascicle of fibers leaves the field of Forel and passes laterally into this nucleus and the zona incerta [page 299 and ff in Chan-Palay (1977)]. In addition, the interposite nucleus (Plaha et al. 2008) and the basal ganglia (Chivileva and Gorbachevskaya 2008) project to the zona incerta making a subcortical communication between the cerebellum and the basal ganglia highly probable. For the dentate nucleus, a direct communication between this nucleus and the pallidum has been described (Hoshi et al. 2005).

Physiological relevance

The physiological relevance of subcortical interactions of basal ganglia and cerebellum is mostly unknown and has not been addressed as yet. In the light of the studies presented in this review, the question arises whether the observed convergences represent casual overlaps of territories in the sense of a “core–shell” principle (Berkley 1983) or if they represent distinct functional networks that differ in their mode of information processing from such networks without convergences, as mentioned above. Considering their limited occurrence in the thalamus, the functional impact might indeed be vanishingly low. We propose that this connectivity pattern might be of physiological significance if it is a distinct property of specific functional networks as proposed above. Assuming that convergences are specific to certain functional networks (as described in the network-specific convergences), it is likely that signal processing in networks with convergences in the thalamus substantially differs from such without convergences. One conceivable function of convergences could be a competitive gating mechanism. Here, inhibitory and excitatory signals from basal ganglia and cerebellum would be integrated in thalamic projection neurons, which would convey the results of that computation to the cortex. Since projection densities were found to be complementary in regions of overlap, such a scenario seems to be less likely. A modulation of the excitability of thalamic projection neurons seems more suitable to the available data. One transmitter that influences the excitability of the two systems is glutamate. This transmitter is well known to have neuromodulatory functions that are conveyed via metabotropic glutamate receptors (Bonsi et al. 2005). These trigger intracellular signal cascades that can have various effects on the responsiveness of a neuron (Platt 2007). Such effects take longer to reach full effect than ligand-gated ion channels. This coincides with observations from several studies that report converging inputs with longer latencies and complex neuronal response patterns (Desiraju and Purpura 1969; Gazzara et al. 1986; Deniau et al. 1992; Sakai et al. 1996, 2000; Buford et al. 1996). In these studies, longer latencies were interpreted as oligosynaptic connections. Such indirect interactions might be mediated by local interneurons, intrathalamic circuitries (e.g., IL or reticular thalamic nucleus), or cerebral cortex (Niimi et al. 1990). The impact of such neuromodulation might be increased by volume transmission or spill-over effects. By such means, the neuromodulatory substance diffuses from the location of release to nearby receptors that are not necessarily strictly related to a synapse. Such modi of transmitter release have been shown for glutamate (Agnati et al. 2010; Okubo and Iino 2011). Observations of en passant varicosities in cerebellar projection territories further support this notion (Sakai et al. 1996; Deniau et al. 1992). In this way, sparse and diffuse projections might also exert a substantial influence on a terminal territory. In addition, the interdigitating projections in VA–VL might be related in such a fashion. Reports of longer latencies and substantial convergences in this region support this supposition (Desiraju and Purpura 1969; Deniau et al. 1978; Gazzara et al. 1986). Assuming the existence of a trend of decreasing subcortical convergences—as described in the section, “Interspecies differences”, one could infer that the interaction of basal ganglia and cerebellum was progressively translocated to the cerebral cortex during brain development. This might have led to enhanced flexibility of the organism’s behavior. Conversely, pronounced convergences on thalamic levels might facilitate faster and more robust behavioral responses. In such an organism, genetic factors might exert a stronger influence on behavior than environmental factors when compared to organisms with pronounced cortical signal integration. The above considerations to explain potentially differing organizations of signal flow within basal ganglio-thalamo-cortical and cerebello-thalamo-cortical loops should have a detectable effect on behavioral levels. Such effects might be revealed by comparing behavior in networks that exhibit different degrees of convergences on thalamic levels (e.g., oculomotor versus skeletomotor). Comparing changes in their behavioral output due to pathologies of basal ganglia (e.g., Parkinson´s disease) or cerebellum (e.g., cerebellar ataxia) might further highlight the importance to study such differences in network organization.

Conclusion

Although numerous amounts of studies exist analyzing the existence of convergences of BG and the cerebellar territory in the thalamus, the results of these different studies are partially conflicting: three neuronal patterns of information processing are presented: (1) a segregation of the cerebellar and BG territories; (2) an interdigitating pattern; and (3) an overlap between both territories. Most of these studies postulate a segregation of the cerebellar and BG projections reflected by interdigitating patterns and some studies described a potential overlap, predominantly in border regions.

To sum up, the amount of convergences has diminished from rodents to carnivores to primates. Interestingly, new evidence from reconstruction based on phylogenetically informative retroposons (Kriegs et al. 2006) suggests that humans and rats developed on another branch then carnivores, and hence, evolution might not have a strong role here. Despite the increasing complexity of informational flow and processing, it might also be possible that the amount of overlap just decreased due to extensive brain growth. In general, a trend to greater network complexity is apparent in larger brains indicating that this complexity is required for maintaining stable levels of neural activation (Kaiser and Hilgetag 2010): larger brains maintain larger-size networks to possess a larger number of hierarchical levels or more modules. Hence, this might call for more specialised regions in primates.

In this review, we introduce two basal organizational principals: (a) “core and shell” principle and (b) network-specific convergence. Herewith, our review scratches on a general principle in information processing, which might change from rodents to the human being due to complexity of informational flow. The analysis of the presence of such convergences, however, might help to step forward to the understanding of movement disorders such as Parkinson’s disease or dystonia, where a dysfunction in the interplay between these two subcortical systems is highly probable.