Skip to main content
Log in

Visually guided, aimed movements can be unaffected by stimulus–response uncertainty

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

    We’re sorry, something doesn't seem to be working properly.

    Please try refreshing the page. If that doesn't work, please contact support so we can address the problem.

Abstract

Response times generally increase linearly with the logarithm of the number of potential stimulus–response alternatives (e.g., Hick’s law). The ubiquity and theoretical importance of this generalization make exceptions particularly interesting. Recently, Kveraga et al. (Exp Brain Res 146:307, 2002) added a third to the two previously known exceptions, demonstrating that saccade latencies were unaffected by stimulus–response uncertainty. They suggest that visually guided saccades are exceptional, because these movements can be automatically selected using a privileged pathway: the topographically organized regions in superior colliculus that convert spatially coded visual activity into spatially coded motor commands. We report that visually guided, aimed hand movements also are unaffected by both stimulus–response uncertainty and stimulus–response repetition. A second experiment demonstrated that this lack of an uncertainty effect persists for equiluminant stimuli. This result suggests that posterior parietal cortex is not the privileged pathway eliminating stimulus–response uncertainty for hand movements. Because hand movements are not guided by mechanisms in the superior colliculus, our results cast doubt on the privileged-pathway hypothesis, at least for hand movements. Instead, the absence of stimulus–response uncertainty may occur only in tasks that do not require the stimulus to be associated with a response effector and that have high stimulus–response compatibility.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For both eye movements and hand movements, this analysis presupposes that the movement can be planned without reference to the initial position of the hand or the eye.

  2. When they studied anti-saccades, eye movements made in the direction opposite where the target appeared, Kveraga et al. (2002) found a large uncertainty effect.

  3. In this and several other papers (Berryhill et al. 2004, 2005), these authors emphasize the importance of the comparison between the no-uncertainty condition, N = 1, and the conditions normally studied in these experiments, with N > 1. Such a comparison is difficult to make convincingly because these conditions potentially differ in much more than the quantitative level of S-R uncertainty: the step from one to two also involves the transition from simple to choice reaction time. In the simple reaction time case, the stimulus can function simply as a “Go” signal: there is no necessity for the participant to identify the stimulus or for it to be processed at all. Also, in experiments such as this, with stimuli arrayed about the fixation, participants may be tempted to ignore the indicated fixation point and foveate the single possible stimulus location, when the response is not an eye movement; of course, when the response is an eye movement and so eye position before the stimulus onset is being monitored, participants cannot use this strategy to reduce the latency when N = 1. An additional issue is that, unless there is a variable fore period or catch trials, participants may anticipate the onset of the stimulus. Any of these three differences between simple and choice reaction time would lead to faster responses for N = 1 than might otherwise be expected, as has been found in virtually every experiment reporting data of this type. The fact that there is no difference in these conditions with saccades or smooth pursuit eye movements probably reflects more about differences in the eye-movement control system or participants’ strategies than it does about the response–selection processes that are typically the focus in studies examining uncertainty in choice-reaction time tasks.

  4. One interesting exception to this generalization about previous designs is the experiment reported by Pellizer and Hedges (2003). Although we feel that much can be learned from the radically different design used in this paper, we decided not to adopt it because it involves randomizing the sets of possible target locations from trial to trial rather than blocking these sets as is traditionally done. Until we have had an opportunity to convince ourselves that there are absolutely no important implications of this choice, we felt that adopting such a design would unnecessarily complicate the comparisons that we wish to make with previous research.

  5. At each point, data triples, consisting of x-position, y-position, and direction of movement in the xy-plane, were extracted from the trajectories recorded for a participant’s 384 movements using cubic-spline interpolation. The 48 triples for each target were then fit using a multivariate normal distribution. Based on the eight fits, estimates were computed for the probability of a movement to each target being correctly classified or incorrectly associated with one of the seven other locations.

  6. In Fitts’ law, the index of difficulty is defined as log2(2 × D/W). Using this index, the difficulty of the movements used in this experiment ranges from 4.2 to 4.6.

  7. However, a post-hoc comparison of the results in Proctor and Wang (1997) Experiment 1B and Experiment 2 leads to the opposite result for a comparison of uni- versus bi-manual keypress movements. This appears to be an area that could use further exploration.

References

  • Albano JE (1996) Adaptive changes in saccade amplitude: oculocentric or orbitocentric mapping? Vision Res 36:2087–2098

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson JR, Bothell D, Byrne MD, Douglass S, Lebiere C, Qin Y (2004) An integrated theory of the mind. Psychol Rev 111:1036–1060

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berryhill M, Kveraga K, Hughes HC (2004) Smooth pursuit under stimulus–response uncertainty. Cogn Brain Res 19:100–102

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berryhill M, Kveraga K, Hughes HC (2005) Effects of directional uncertainty on visually-guided joystick pointing. Percept Mot Skills 100:267–274

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brainard DH (1997) The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis 10:433–436

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Buneo CA, Jarvis MR, Batista AP, Andersen RA (2002) Direct visuomotor transformations for reaching. Nature 416:632–636

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Carlton LG (1981) Visual information: the control of aiming movements. Q J Exp Psychol A Hum Exp Psychol 33:87–93

    Google Scholar 

  • Christie LS, Luce RD (1956) Decision structures and time relations in simple choice behavior. Bull Math Biophys 18:89–112

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dassonville P, Lewis SM, Foster HE, Ashe J (1999) Choice and stimulus–response compatibility affect duration of response selection. Cogn Brain Res 7:235–240

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fitts PM (1954) The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. J Exp Psychol 47:381–391

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Flanders M, Tillery SIH, Soechting JF (1992) Early stages in a sensorimotor transformation. Behav Brain Sci 15:309–320

    Google Scholar 

  • Glover SR, Dixon P (2001) Dynamic illusion effects in a reaching task: evidence for separate visual representations in the planning and control of reaching. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 27:560–572

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Glover SR, Dixon P (2002) Dynamic effects of the Ebbinghaus illusion in grasping: support for a planning/control model of action. Percept Psychophys 64:266–278

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Graves RA (1996) Luminance and color effects on localization of briefly flashed visual stimuli. Vis Neurosci 13:567–573

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald AG (1970) Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: with special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. Psychol Rev 77:73–90

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Heath M (2005) Role of limb and target vision in the online control of memory-guided reaches. Motor Control 9:281–311

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hick WE (1952) On the rate of gain of information. Q J Exp Psychol 4:11–26

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyman R (1953) Stimulus information as a determinant of reaction time. J Exp Psychol 45:188–196

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kornblum S (1969) Sequential determinants of information processing in serial and discrete choice reaction time. Psychol Rev 76:113–131

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kornblum S, Hasbroucq T, Osman A (1990) Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus–response compatibility–A model and taxonomy. Psychol Rev 97:253–270

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Krzanowski WJ (1988) Principles of multivariate analysis. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK

    Google Scholar 

  • Kveraga K, Berryhill M, Hughes HC (2006) Directional uncertainty in visually guided pointing. Percept Mot Skills 102:125–132

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kveraga K, Boucher L, Hughes HC (2002) Saccades operate in violation of Hick’s law. Exp Brain Res 146:307–314

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lacouture Y, Marley AAJ (1991) A connectionist model of choice and reaction time in absolute identification. Connect Sci 3:401–433

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laming DRJ (1966) A new interpretation of relation between choice-reaction time and number of equiprobable alternatives. Br J Math Stat Psychol 19:139–149

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Leonard JA (1959) Tactual choice reactions: I. Q J Exp Psychol 11:76–83

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Livingstone M, Hubel D (1988) Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth—Anatomy, physiology, and perception. Science 240:740–749

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Marsden CD, Merton PA, Morton HB, Adam J (1978) Long loop mechanisms. In: Desmedt JE (ed) Cerebral motor control in man. Karger, Basel, pp 334–341

    Google Scholar 

  • Maxwell SE, Delaney HD (2004) Designing experiments and analyzing data: a model comparison perspective. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Merkel J (1885) Die zeitlichen Verhaltnisse der Willensthatigkeit. Philosophical Studies 32:73–127. Cited in Teicher and Krebs (1974)

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer DE, Abrams RA, Kornblum S, Wright CE, Smith JEK (1988) Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. Psychol Rev 95:340–370

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer DE, Kieras DE (1997a) A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: part 1. Basic mechanisms. Psychol Rev 104:3–65

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer DE, Kieras DE (1997b) A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: part 2. Accounts of psychological refractory-period phenomena. Psychol Rev 104:749–791

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer DE, Smith JEK, Kornblum S, Abrams RA, Wright CE (1990) Speed-accuracy tradeoffs in aimed movements: toward a theory of rapid voluntary action. In: Jeannerod M (ed) Attention and performance XIII: motor representation and control. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, Chap. 6, pp 173–226

  • Milner AD, Goodale MA (1995) The visual brain in action. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK

    Google Scholar 

  • Mowbray GH, Rhoades MV (1959) Choice reaction times for skilled responses. Q J Exp Psychol 11:16–23

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pellizer G, Hedges JH (2003) Motor planning: effect of directional uncertainty with discrete spatial cues. Exp Brain Res 150:276–289

    Google Scholar 

  • Pitzalis S, Di Russo F, Spinelli D (2005) Loss of visual information in neglect: the effect of chromatic–versus luminance-contrast stimuli in a ‘‘what’’ task. Exp Brain Res 163:527–534

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor RW, Wang H (1997) Set- and element-level stimulus-response compatibility effects for different manual response sets. J Mot Beh 29:351–365

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Proteau L, Girouard Y (1984) Motor programming: does the choice of the limb which is to carry out the response imply a delay? J Mot Beh 16:302–312

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum DA (1980) Human movement initiation: specification of arm, direction, and extent. J Exp Psychol Gen 109:444–474

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schiller PH, Stryker M (1972) Single-unit recording and stimulation in superior colliculus of the alert rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol 35:915–924

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Semjen A, Requin J, Fiori N (1978) The interactive effect of foreperiod duration and response-movement characteristics upon choice-reaction time in a pointing task. J Hum Mov Stud 4:108–118

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith GA, Carew M (1987) Decision time unmasked: individuals adopt different strategies. Aust J Psychol 39:339–351

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snyder LH, Batista AP, Andersen RA (2000) Intention-related activity in the posterior parietal cortex: a review. Vision Res 40:1433–1441

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks DL, Hartwich-Young R (1989) The deep layers of the superior colliculus. In: Wurtz RH, Goldberg ME (eds) The neurobiology of saccadic eye movements, vol 3. Reviews of Oculomotor Research, Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 213–256

  • Stuphorn V, Bauswein E, Hoffmann K-L (2002) Neuons in the primate superior colliculus coding for arm movements in gaze-related coordinates. J Neurophysiol 83:1283–1299

    Google Scholar 

  • Teichner WH, Krebs MJ (1974) Laws of visual choice reaction time. Psychol Rev 81:75–98

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • ten Hoopen G, Akerboom S, Raaymakers E (1982) Vibrotactual choice reaction time, tactile receptor systems and ideomotor compatibility. Acta Psychol 50:143–157

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Usher M, McClelland JL (2001) The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. Psychol Rev 108:550–592

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wagenaar WA (1969) Note on the construction of digram-balanced Latin squares. Psychol Bull 72:384–386

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welford AT (1960) The measurement of sensory-motor performance: survey and reappraisal of tweleve years progress. Ergonomics 3:189–230

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welford AT (1968) Fundamentals of skill. Methuen, London, UK

  • Werner W, Dannenberg S, Hoffmann K-P (1997a) Arm-movement-related neurons in the primate superior colliculus and underlying reticular formation: comparison of neuronal activity with EMGs of muscles of the shoulder, arm and trunk during reaching. Exp Brain Res 115:191–205

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Werner W, Dannenberg S, Hoffmann K-P (1997b) Anatomical distribution of arm-movement-related neurons in the primate superior colliculus and underlying reticular formation in comparison with visual and saccadic cells. Exp Brain Res 115:206–216

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Woodworth RS (1899) The accuracy of voluntary movement. Psychol Rev 3(2, Whole No. 13)

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Sylvan Kornblum for many useful discussions concerning the stimulus–response uncertainty effects. We also are grateful to Nina Macdonald, Robert W. Proctor, and an anonymous reviewer for useful comments on earlier versions of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Charles E. Wright.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Wright, C.E., Marino, V.F., Belovsky, S.A. et al. Visually guided, aimed movements can be unaffected by stimulus–response uncertainty. Exp Brain Res 179, 475–496 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-006-0805-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-006-0805-z

Keywords

Navigation