Skip to main content

On the Nature of Language Production – Towards a General Model

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Developing Language and Literacy

Part of the book series: Literacy Studies ((LITS,volume 23))

Abstract

This chapter is a sketch towards a general model of the language production process in different modalities, with special reference to the dynamic interaction between language and thought. The model, it is argued, must take the temporal, social and cognitive organization of the language production process into account.

Special attention is given the external representation and its affordances, that is, what the spoken or written representation offers the speaker or writer to do. For the purpose of demonstration, three different situations of language production are subjected to contrastive analysis: a predominantly monological discourse in a spoken and a written condition derived from a narrative task; a genuinely dialogical spontaneous spoken discourse; and the writing of a poem.

Further, the paper proposes a partly new conceptual approach: that of seeing language production basically as a process of drafting – in writing as well as in speech. In effect, text-writing can be said typically to be a solitary drafting process where revisiting and reinterpreting one’s own previous discourse is essential to developing the text, whereas spoken conversation is typically a joint drafting process where attention to one’s co-speaker’s reactions is essential to developing the discourse.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Alves, R., Castro, S.-L., de Sousa, L., & Strömqvist, S. (2007). Influence of typing skill on pause-execution cycles in written composition. In M. Torrance, L. van Waes, & D. Galbraith (Eds.), Writing and cognition: Research and applications (pp. 55–65). Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1987). The psychology of written composition. Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H. (2006). Context and common ground. I. In K. Brown (Ed.), Encyclopedia of language & linguistics (Vol. 3, 2nd ed., pp. 105–108). Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H., Tree, F., & Jean, E. (2002). Using uh and um in spontaneous speaking. Cognition, 84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flower, L., & Hayes, J. R. (1981). The pregnant pause: An inquiry into the nature of planning. Research in the Teaching of English, 229–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1975). The language of thought. Crowell Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fromkin, V. A. (1973). Speech errors as linguistic evidence. Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gärdenfors, P. (2000). Conceptual spaces – The geometry of thought. Bradford Books, MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1969). The ecological approach to visual perception. Hughton-Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman-Eisler, F. (1968). Psycholinguistics: Experiments in spontaneous speech. Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gumperz, J., & Levinson, S. (Eds.). (1996). Rethinking linguistic relativity: Studies in the social and cultural foundations of Language. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jefferson, G. (1974). Error correction as an interactional resource. Language in Society, 3(2), 181–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jefferson, G., Schegloff, E. A., & Sacks, H. (1977). The preference for self-corrections in the organization of repair in conversation. Language, 53, 361–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kellogg, R. T. (2008). Training writing skills: A cognitive development perspective. Journal of Writing Research, 1, 1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R. (1987). Foundations of cognitive grammar, vol. 1: Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. (1983). Monitoring and self-repair in speech. Cognition, 14(1), 41–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. C. (1997). Language and cognition: The cognitive consequences of spatial description in Guugu Yimithirr. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 7(1), 98–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindblom, B., & Öhman, S. (Eds.). (1979). Frontiers of speech communication research: Festschrift for Gunnar Fant. Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, A. (2015). Reacher said nothing: Lee Child and the making of make me. Penguin Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, P., & Oppenheimer, D. (2014). The Pen is mightier than the keyboard: Advantages of Longhand over laptop note taking. Psychological Science, 25(6), 1159–1168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myhill, D. (2008). Towards a linguistic model of sentence development in writing. Language and Education, 22(5), 271–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D., & Tolchinsky, L. (2002). Developing linguistic literacy: A comprehensive model. Journal of Child Language, 29(2), 417–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, H., Schegloff, A., & Jefferson, G. (1972). A Simplest Systematics for the Organization of Turn-Taking for Conversation. Language, 50(4, Part 1), 696–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shannon, C. E. (1951). Prediction and entropy of printed English. The Bell System Technical Journal, 30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. (1996). From “thought and language” to “thinking for speaking”. In J. Gumperz & S. Levinson (Eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity: Studies in the social and cultural foundations of language (pp. 70–96). Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I. (2002). Cognitive and communicative consequences of linguistic diversity. In S. Strömqvist (Ed.), The diversity of languages and language learning (pp. 7–23). Lund University, Centre for Languages and Literature.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strömqvist, S. (2009). SprÅkets öga. Om vägarna mellan tankar och ord [The eye of language. On the paths between thoughts and words]. Studentlitteratur.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strömqvist, S., Nordqvist, Å., & Wengelin, Å. (2004). Writing the frog story – Developmental and cross-modal perspectives. In S. Strömqvist & L. Verhoeven (Eds.), Relating events in narrative – Typological and contextual perspectives (pp. 359–394). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Strömqvist, S., Holmqvist, K., Johansson, V., Karlsson, H., & Wengelin, Å. (2006). What key-logging can reveal about writing. In K. Sullivan & E. Lindgren (Eds.), Computer key-stroke logging and writing: Methods and applications (pp. 45–72). Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suwa, M., & Tversky, B. (1997). What do architects and students perceive in their design sketches?: A protocol analysis. Design Studies, 18(4), 385–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torrance, M., Johansson, R., Johansson, V., & Wengelin, Å. (2016). Reading during the composition of multi-sentence texts: An eye-movement study. Psychological Research, 80(5), 729–743.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Noort, P. D. M., Struys, E., Bosch, P., Jaswetz, L., Perriard, B., Yeo, S., et al. (2019). Does the bilingual advantage in cognitive control exist and if so, what are its modulating factors? A systematic review. Behavioral Sciences, 9, 27. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9030027

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought and reality. Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. M.I.T. Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, C., & Stevens, K. (1972). Emotions and speech: Some acoustical correlates. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 52, 1238–1250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I want to extend a special thanks to Victoria Johansson, Herbert Clark and Dan Slobin for inspiring discussions of many of the issues addressed in this paper. Also, a warm thanks to the members of the Linneaus excellence centre “Thinking in Time – Cognition, Communication and Learning” at Lund university for fruitful feedback.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sven Strömqvist .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2022 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Strömqvist, S. (2022). On the Nature of Language Production – Towards a General Model. In: Levie, R., Bar-On, A., Ashkenazi, O., Dattner, E., Brandes, G. (eds) Developing Language and Literacy. Literacy Studies, vol 23. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-99891-2_7

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-99891-2_7

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-99890-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-99891-2

  • eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics