Skip to main content

Stimulus Control and Generalization

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Autism Service Delivery

Part of the book series: Autism and Child Psychopathology Series ((ACPS))

Abstract

The ability to identify possible sources of stimulus control, and to increase or decrease them, as necessary, is essential to effective autism service delivery. Thus, familiarity with the fundamentals of stimulus control should hold special significance for applied behavior analysts working in the autism field. This chapter provides an overview of some major areas of basic laboratory-based stimulus control research in behavior analysis, with particular attention paid to topics of potential relevance in the application of stimulus control procedures. Defining characteristics of three-, four-, and five-term contingencies are described here, issues in establishing these forms of stimulus control are discussed, and the nature of the stimulus classes that can result in each case are explored. While emphasizing behavior–environment interactions throughout, the literature reviewed here ranges from analysis of each element of an n-term contingency to syntheses of complex human repertoires. This inductive and translational approach stands to be enhanced, however, by appropriate integration of laboratory findings with applications in autism service delivery. The experimental behavior-analytic literature has important lessons to offer those practitioners wishing to better understand the stimulus control principles that can improve intervention effectiveness.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 169.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Andronis, P. T. (1983). Symbolic aggression by pigeons: Contingency coadduction. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Annett, J. M., & Leslie, J. C. (1995). Stimulus equivalence classes involving olfactory stimuli. The Psychological Record, 45, 439–450.

    Google Scholar 

  • April, L. B., Bruce, K., & Galizio, M. (2011). Matching- and nonmatching-to-sample concept learning in rats using olfactory stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 96, 139–154.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arntzen, E., & Holth, P. (1997). Probability of stimulus equivalence as a function of training design. The Psychological Record, 47, 309–320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arntzen, E., & Holth, P. (2000). Equivalence outcome in single subjects as a function training structure. The Psychological Record, 50, 603–628.

    Google Scholar 

  • Astley, S. L., & Wasserman, E. A. (1996). Nonsimilarity-based categorization. In T. R. Zentall & P. M. Smeets (Eds.), Stimulus Class Formation in Humans and Animals (pp. 111–133). New York: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Augustson, K. G., & Dougher, M. J. (1992). Teaching conditional discrimination to young children: Some methodological successes and failures. Experimental Analysis of Human Behavior Bulletin, 9, 21–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Augustson, E. M., & Dougher, M. J. (1997). The transfer of avoidance evoking functions through stimulus equivalence classes. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 28, 181–191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Augustson, E. M., Dougher, M. J., & Markham, M. R. (2000). Emergenceof conditional stimulus relations and transfer of respondent eliciting functions among compound stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 745–770.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baer, D. M., Peterson, R. F., & Sherman, J. A. (1967). The development of imitation by reinforcing behavior similarity to a model. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10, 405–416.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, A. (1973). Postdiscrimination gradients of human subjects on a tone continuum. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 101, 337–342.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, D. (1994). Stimulus equivalence and relational frame theory. The Psychological Record, 44, 91–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Staunton, C., Whelan, R., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Commins, S., Walsh, D., Steward, I., Smeets, P. M., & Dymond, S. (2005). Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 84, 417–433.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barros, R., Galvao, O., & McIlvane, W. J. (2002). Generalized identity matching-to-sample in cebus paella. The Psychological Record, 52, 411–460.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentall, R. P., Jones, R. M., & Dickens, D. W. (1999). Errors and response latencies as a function of nodal distance in 5-member equivalence classes. The Psychological Record, 49, 93–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berryman, R., Cumming, W. W., & Nevin, J. A. (1963). Acquisition of delayed matching in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 6, 101–107.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bickel, W. K., & Etzel, B. C. (1985). The quantal nature of controlling stimulus-response relations as measure in tests of stimulus generalization. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 44, 245–270.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bloomfield, T. M. (1967). A peak shift on a line-tilt continuum. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10, 361–366.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blough, D. S. (1959). Generalization and preference on a stimulus-intensity continuum. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2, 307–317.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blough, D. S. (1984). Form recognition in pigeons. In H. L. Roitblat, T. G. Bever & H. S. Terraced (Eds.), Animal cognition (pp. 277–289). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bousfield, W. A. (1953). The occurrence of clustering in recall of randomly arranged associates. Journal of General Psychology, 49, 229–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bush, K. M. (1993). Stimulus equivalence and cross-modal transfer. The Psychological Record, 43, 567–584.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bush, K. M., Sidman, M., & de Rose, T. (1989). Contextual control of emergent equivalence relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 29–45.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carr, D., & Blackman, D. E. (2001). Relations among equivalence, naming, and conflicting baseline control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 75, 55–76.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carr, D., Wilkinson, K. M., Blackman, D., & McIlvane, W. J (2000). Equivalence classes inindividuals with minimal verbal repertoires. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 74, 101–114.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carrigan, P. F. Jr., & Sidman, M. (1992). Conditional discrimination and equivalence relations: A theoretical analysis of control by negative stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 459–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, D. E., & Eckerman, D. A. (1975). Symbolic matching by pigeons: Rate of learning complex discriminations predicted from simple discriminations. Science, 187, 662–664.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, D. E., & Werner, T. J. (1978). Complex learning and information processing by pigeons: A critical analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 29, 565–601.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Catania, A. C. (1998). Learning (4th ed.). Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catania, A. C., & Cerutti, D. (1986). Some nonverbal proprerties of verbal behavior. In T. Thompson & M. D. Zeiler (Eds.), Analysis and integration of behavioral units (pp. 185–211). Hllsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cerutti, D. T., & Rumbaugh, D. M. (1993). Stimulus relations in comparative primate perspective. The Psychological Record, 43, 811–821.

    Google Scholar 

  • Challies, D. M., Hunt, M., Garry, M., & Harper, D. N. (2011). Whatever gave you that idea? False memories following equivalence training: A behavioral account of the misinformation effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 96, 343–362.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chatlosh, D. L., & Wasserman, E. A. (1987). Delayed temporal discrimination in pigeons: A comparison of two procedures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 47, 299–309.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Church, R. M. (1980). Short-term memory for time intervals. Learning and Motivation, 11, 208–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Church, R. M. (1984). Properties of the internal clock. In J. Gibbon & L. Allen (Eds.), Timing and time perception (Vol. 438). New York: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, L. R., Looney, T. A., Brady, J. H., & Aucella, A. F. (1976). Differential sample response schedules in the acquisition of conditional discriminations by pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 26, 301–314.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, L. R., Brady, J., & Lowry, M. (1981). The role of differential responding in matching-to-sample and delayed matching performance. In M. L. Commons & J. A. Nevin (Eds.), Quantitative analysis of behavior: Vol. I. Discriminative properties of reinforcement schedules (pp. 345–364). Cambridge: Ballinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Columbus, Ohio: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cumming, W. W., & Berryman, R. (1961). Some data on matching behavior in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4, 281–284.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cumming, W. W., & Berryman, R. (1965). The complex discriminated operant: Studies of matching-to-sample and related problems. In D. I. Mostofsky (Ed.), Stimulus generalization (pp. 284–330). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Amato, M. R., & Colombo, M. (1989). On the lmits of the matching concept in mondeys (Cebus apella). Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 225–236.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • D’Amato, M. R., Salmon, D. P., & Colombo, M. (1985a). Extent and limits of the matching concept in monkeys (Cebus paella). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 11, 35–51.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • D’Amato, M. R., Salmon, D. P., Loukas, E., & Tomie, A. (1985b). Symmetry and transitivity of conditional relations in monkeys (Cebus apella) and pigeons (Columba livia). Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 44, 35–47.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davenport, R. K., & Rogers, C. M. (1970). Intermodal equivalence of stimuli in apes. Science, 168, 279–280.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davison, M., & McCarthy, D. (1988). The matching law: A research review. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davison, M., & Nevin, J. A. (1999). Stimuli, reinforcers, and behavior: An integration. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 71, 439–482.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Debert, P., Matos, A. M., & McIlvane, W. J. (2007). Conditinal relations with compound abstract stimuli using a go/no-go procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 87, 89–96.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Debert, P., Huziwara, E. M., Faggiani, R.B., de Mathis, M. E. S., & McIlvane, W. J. (2009). Emergent conditional relations in a go-no go procedure: Figure-ground and stimulus-position compound relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 92, 233–243.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeGrandpre, R. J., Bickel, W. K., & Higgins, S. T. (1992). Emergent equivalence relations between interoceptive (drug) and exteroceptive (visual) stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 9–18.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Rose, J. C., McIlvane, W. J., Dube, W. V., Galpin, V. C., & Stoddard, L. T. (1988a). Emergent simple discrimination established by indirect relation to differential consequences. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 1–20.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Rose, J. C., McIlvane, W. J., Dube, W. V., & Stoddard, L. T. (1988b). Stimulus class formation and functional equivalence in moderately retarded individuals’ conditional discrimination. Behavioural Processes, 17, 167–175.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Derrickson, J. G., Neef, N. A., & Cataldo, M. E. (1993). Effects of signaling invasive procedures on a hospitalized infant’s affective behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 133–134.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Devany, J. M., Hayes, S. C., & Nelson, R. O. (1986). Equivalence class formation in language-able and language-disabled children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 243–257.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dews, P. B. (1970). Drugs in psychology. A commentary on T. Thompson & C.R. Schuster’s Behavioral Pharmacology. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 13, 395–406.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dinsmoor, J. A. (1983). Observing and conditioned reinforcement. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6, 693–728.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dinsmoor, J. A. (1995a). Stimulus control: Part I. The Behavior Analyst, 18, 51–68.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dinsmoor, J. A. (1995b). Stimulus control: Part II. The Behavior Analyst, 18, 253–269.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dinsmoor, J. A., Mueller, K. L., Martin, L. T., & Bowe, C. A. (1982). The acquisition of observing. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 38, 249–263.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, M. H., & Spradlin, J. E. (1976). Establishing stimulus equivalence among retarded adolescents. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 21, 144–164.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dougher, M. J., & Markham, M. R. (1994). Stimulus equivalence, functional equivalence, and the transfer of function. In S. C. Hayes, L. J. Hayes, M. Sato, & K. Ono (Eds.), Behavior analysis of language and cognition (pp. 71–91). Reno: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dougher, M. J., Augustson, E. M., Markham, M. R., Wulfert, E., & Greenway, D. E. (1994). The transfer of respondent eliciting and extinction functions through stimulus equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 62, 331–351.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dougher, M. J., Perkins, D. R., Greenway, D., Koons, A., & Chiasson, C. (2002). Contextual control of equivalence-based transformation of functions. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 63–93.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dube, W. V., & McIlvane, W. J. (1995). Stimulus-reinforcer relations and emergent matching to sample. Psychological Record, 45, 591–612.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dube, W. V., & McIlvane, W. J. (1996). Some implications of a stimulus control topography analysis for emergent stimulus classes. In T. R. Zentall & P. M. Smeets (Eds.), Stimulus class formation in humans and animals (pp. 197–218). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dube, W. V., McIlvane, W. J., Mackay, H. A., & Stoddard, L. T. (1987). Stimulus class membership established via stimulus-reinforcer relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 47, 159–175.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dube, W. V., McIlvane, W. J., Maguire, R. W., Mackay, H. A., & Stoddard. L. T. (1989). Stimulus class formation and stimulus-reinforcer relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 47, 159–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dube, W. V., McIlvane, W. J., & Green, G. (1992). An analysis of generalized identity matching-to-sample test procedures. The Psychological Record, 42, 17–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dube, W. V., Callahan, T. D., & McIlvane, W. J. (1993). Serial reversals of concurrent auditory discriminations in rates. The Psychological Record, 43, 429–440.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dube, W. V., Dickson, C. A., Balsamo, L. M., O’Donnell, K. L., Tomanari, G. Y., Farren, K. M., Wheeler, E. E., & McIlvane, W. (2010). Observing behavior and atypically restricted stimulus control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 94, 297–313.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dugdale, N. A., & Lowe, C. F. (1990). Naming an dsitmulus equivalence. In D. E. Blackman & H. Lejeune (Eds.), Behaviour analysis in theory and practice: Contributions and controversies (pp. 115–138). Hove: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dymond, S., & Barnes, D. (1995). A transformation of self-discrimination response functions in accordance with the arbitrarily applicable relations of sameness, more than, and less than. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64, 163–184.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eckerman, C. O. (1969). Probability of reinforcement and the development of stimulus control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 551–559.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eckerman, D. A. (1970). Generalization and response mediation of a conditional discrimination. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 13, 301–316.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eckerman, D. A., Lanson, R. N., & Cumming, W. W. (1968). Acquisition and maintenance of matching without a required observing response. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 435–441.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Erjavec, M., & Horne, P. J. (2008). Determinants of imitation of hand-to-body gestures in 2- and 3-year-old children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 89, 183–207.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Erjavec, M., Lovett, V. E., & Horne, P. J. (2009). Do infants show generalized imitation of gestures? II. The effects of skills training and multiple exemplar matching training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 91, 355–376.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Estavez, A. F., Fuentes, L. J., Overmier, J. B., & Gonzalez, C. (2003). Differential outcomes effect in children and adults with down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 108, 108–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farthing, G. W., & Opuda, M. J. (1974). Transfer of matching-to-sample in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 21, 199–213.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferster, C. B. (1960). Intermittent reinforcement of matching to sample in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 3, 259–272.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferster, C. B., & Appel, J. B. (1961). Punishment of Sdelta responding in matching to sample by time out from positive reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Analysis, 4, 45–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fields, L., & Garuto, M. (2009). Optimizing linked perceptual class formation and transfer of function. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 91, 225–251.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fields, L., & Moss, P. (2008). Formation of partially and fully elaborated generalized equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 90, 135–168.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fields, L., Adams, B. J., Verhave, T., & Newman, S. (1990). The effects of nodality on the formation of equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, 345–358.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fields, L., Newman, S., Adams, B. J., & Vehave, T. (1992). The expansion of equivalence classes through simple discrimination training and fading. The Psychological Record, 42, 3–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fields, L., Adams, B. J., & Verhave, T. (1993). The effects of equivalence class structure on test performances. The Psychological Record, 43, 693–712.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fields, L., Reeve, K. F., Adams, B. J., Brown, J. L., & Verhave, T. (1997a). Predicting the extension of equivalence classes from primary generalization gradients: The merger of equivalence classes and perceptual classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 68, 67–91.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fields, L., Reeve, K. F., Rosen, D., Varelas, A., Adams, B. J., & Belanich, J. (1997b). Using simulutaneous protocol to study equivalence class formation: The facilitating effect of nodal number and size of previously established equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 47, 661–686.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fields, L., Hobbie-Reeve, S. A., Adams, B. J., & Reeve, K. F. (1999). Effects of training directionality and class size on equivalence class formation by adults. The Psychological Record, 49, 703–724.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fields, L., Matneja, P., Varelas, A., Belanich, J., Fitzer, A., & Shamoun, K. (2002). The formation of linked perceptual classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 271–290.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fields, L., Arntzen, E., Nartey, R. K., & Eilifsen, C. (2012). Effects of a meaningful, a discriminative, and a meaningless stimulus on equivalence class formation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 97, 163–181.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Galizio, M., Stewart, K., & Pilgrim, C. (2001). Clustering in artificial categories: An equivalence analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 609–614.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galizio, M., Stewart, K., & Pilgrim, C. (2004). Typicality effects in contingency-shaped generalized equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 82, 253–273.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garotti, M., de Souza, G., de Rose, J. C., Molina, R. C., & Gil, M. S. (2000). Reoganization of equivalence classes after reversal of baseline relations. The Psychological Record, 50, 35–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gatch, M. B., & Osborne, J. G. (1989). Transfer of contextual stimulus function via equivalence class development. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 369–378.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gewirtz, J. L., & Stengle, K. G. (1968). Learning of generalized imitation as the basis for identification. Psychological Review, 5, 374–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldiamond, I. (1962). Perception. In A. J. Bachrach (Ed.), Experimental foundations of clinical psychology (pp. 280–340). New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, M., & Shapiro, S. (1979). Matching-to-sample and oddity-to-sample in goldfish. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 31, 259–266.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Green, G. (1990). Differences in development of visual and auditory-visual equivalence relations. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 95, 260–270.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Green, G., & Saunders, R. (1998). Stimulus equivalence. In K. A. Lattal & M. Perone (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in human operant behavior (pp. 229–262). New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley. (Re-published (1989). Los Altos, CA: Peninsula).

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, G., Sigurdardottir, Z. G., & Saunders, R. R. (1991). The role of instructions in the transfer of ordinal functions through equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 55, 287–304.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griffee, K., & Dougher, M. J. (2002). Contextual control of stimulus generalization and stimulus equivalence in hierarchical categorization. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 433–447.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guinther, P. M., & Dougher, M. J. (2010). Semantic false memories in the form of derived relational intrusions following training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 93, 329–347.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guttman, N. (1959). Generalization gradients around stimuli associated with different reinforcement schedules. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 335–340.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guttman, N. (1965). Effects of discrimination formation on generalization measured from a positive-rate baseline. In D. I. Mostofsky (Ed.), Stimulus generalization (pp. 210–217). Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guttman, N., & Kalish, H. I. (1956). Discriminability and stimulus generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 79–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hanson, H. M. (1959). Effects of discrimination training on stimulus generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 321–334.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, H. F. (1949). The formation of learning sets. Psychological Review, 56, 51–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, J. M. (1991). Stimulus control. In I. H. Iversen & K. A. Lattal (Eds.), Experimental analysis of behavior: Part I (pp. 251–299). New York: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, J. M., & Beecher, M. D. (1969). Control of responding by the location of an auditory stimulus: Role of rise time of the stimulus. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 217–227.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, R. J., & Green, G. (1990). Development of conditional and equivalence relations without differential consequences. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54, 225–237.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C. (1989). Nonhumans have not yet shown stimulus equivalence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 385–392.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, L. J., Tilley, K. L., & Hayes, S. C. (1988). Extending equivalence class membership to gustatory stimuli. The Psychological Record, 38, 473–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, L. J., Thompson, S., & Hayes, S. C. (1989). Stimulus equivalence and rule following. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 275–291.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Kohlenberg, B. S., & Hayes, L. J. (1991). The transfer of specific and general consequential functions through simple and conditional equivalence relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 119–137.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Gifford, E. V., & Wilson, K. G. (1996). Stimulus classes and stimulus relations: Arbitrarily applicable relational responding as an operant. In T. R. Zentall & P. M. Smeets (Eds.), Stimulsu class formation in humans and animals (pp. 279–299). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hearst, E. (1971). Contrast and stimulus generalization following prolonged discrimination training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 15, 355–363.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hearst, E., Koresko, M. B., & Poppen, R. (1964). Stimulus generalization and the response-reinforcement contingency. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 369–380.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herman, L. M., & Gordon, J. A. (1974). Auditory delayed matching in the bottlenose dolphin. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 21, 19–26.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herman, L. M., & Thompson, R. K. R. (1982). Symbolic, identity and probe delayed matching of sounds by the bottlenose dolphin. Animal Learning & Behavior, 10, 22–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrick, R. M., Myers, J. L., & Korotkin, A. L. (1959). Changes in Sd and in S-delta rates during the development of an operant discrimination. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 52, 359–364.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J. (1970). On the law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 13, 243–266.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J. (1979). Acquisition, generalization and discrimination reversal of a natural concept. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 5, 116–129.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J., Loveland, D. H., & Cable, C. (1976). Natural concepts in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2, 285–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hogan, D. E., & Zentall, T. R. (1977). Backward associations in the pigeon. American Journal of Psychology, 90, 3–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holland, J. G. (1958). Human vigilance. Science, 128, 61–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, P. W. (1979). Transfer of matching performance in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 31, 103–114.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holt, G. L., & Shafer, J. N. (1973). Function of intertribal interval in matching-to-sample. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 19, 181–186.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Honig, W. K., & Urcuioli, P. J. (1981). The legacy of Guttman and Kalish (1956): 25 years of research on stimulus generalization. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 36, 405–445.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Honig, W. K., Boneau, C. A., Burnstein, K. R., & Pennypacker, H. S. (1963). Positive and negative generalization gradients obtained after equivalent training conditions. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 56, 111–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horne, P. J., & Erjavec, M. (2007). Do infants show generalized imitation of gestures? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 87, 63–87.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horne, P. J., & Lowe, C. F. (1996). On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 185–241.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horne, P. J., Lowe, C. F., & Randle, V. R. L. (2004). Naming and categorization in young children: II. Listener behavior training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 81, 267–288.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horne, P. J., Hughes, J. C., & Lowe, C. F. (2006). Naming and categorization in young children: IV: Listener behavior training and transfer of function. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 85, 247–273.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horne, P. J., & Erjavec, M., & Lovett, V. E. (2009). The effects of modeling, local stimulus enhancement, and affordance demonstration on the production of object-directed actions in 6-month-old infants. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27, 269–281.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hove, O. (2003). Differential probability of equivalence class formation following a one-to-many versus a many-to-one training structure. The Psychological Record, 53, 617–634.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irwin, R. J., & McCarthy, D. (1998). Psychophysics. In K. A. Lattal & M. Perone (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in human operant behavior (pp. 291–321). New York: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Iversen, I. H., Sidman, M., & Carrigan, M. (1986). Stimulus definition in conditional discriminations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45, 297–304.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iwai, E., Yagimuna, S., & Mishkin, M. (1986). Acquisition of discrimination learning of patterns identical in configuration in Macaques (Macaca mulatta and M. fuscata). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 100, 30–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, W. J., & Pegram, G. V. (1970). Acquisition, transfer and retention of matching by rhesus monkeys. Psychological Reports, 27, 839–846.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, H. M. (1965). Generalization gradients and the concept of inhibition. In D. I. Mostofsky (Ed.), Stimulus generalization (pp. 55–61). Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, H. M., & Harrison, R. H. (1960). Effect of discrimination training on auditory generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 246–253.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, H. M., & Harrison, R. H. (1962). Generalization gradients of inhibition following auditory discrimination learning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5, 435–441.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, H. M., & Sainsbury, R. S. (1970). Discrimination learning with the distinctive feature on positive or negative trials. In D. I. Mostofsky (Ed.), Attention: Contemporary theory and analysis (pp. 239–273). New York.: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, C., & Sidman, M. (1993). Conditional discrimination and equivalence relations: Control by negative stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59, 333–347.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, C., Meleshkevich, O., & Dube, W. V. (2014). Merging separately established stimulus classes with outcome-specific reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 101, 38–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kastak, D., & Schusterman, R. (1994). Transfer of visual identity matching-to-sample in two California sea lions (Zalophus califonianus). Animal Learning & Behavior, 22, 427–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kastak, C. R., Schusterman, R. J., & Kastak, D. (2001). Equivalence classification by California sea lions using class-specific reinforcers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 76, 131–158.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelleher, R. T., Riddle, W. C., & Cook, L. (1962). Observing responses in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5, 3–13.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, F. S., & Schoenfeld, W. N. (1950). Principles of psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendall, S. B., & Mills, W. A. (1979). Attention in the pigeon: Testing for excitatory and inhibitory control by the weak elements. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 31, 421–431.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, C. H. (1991). Equivalence class formation influenced by the number of nodes separating stimuli. Behavioral Processes, 24, 219–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, C. H., & Laitinen, R. (1988). Second-order conditional control of symmetric and transitive stimulus relations: The influence of order effects. The Psychological Record, 38, 437–446.

    Google Scholar 

  • Killeen, P. R. (1991). Behavior’s time. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 27, 295–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Killeen, P. R., & Fetterman, J. G. (1988). A behavioral theory of timing. Psychological Review, 95, 274–295.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kohlenberg, B. S., Hayes, S. C., & Hayes, L. J. (1991). The transfer of contextual control over equivalence classes through equivalence classes: A possible model of social stereotyping. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 505–518.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lane, S. D., Clow, J. K., Innis, A., & Critchfield, T. S. (1998). Generalization of cross-modal stimulus equivalence classes: Operant processes as components in human category formation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 70, 267–280.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lazar, R. M. (1977). Extending sequence-class membership with matching to sample. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27, 381–392.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lazar, R. M., Davis-Lang, D., & Sanchez, L. (1984). The formation of visual stimulus equivalences in children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 41, 251–266.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lazar, R. M., & Kotlarchyk, B. J. (1986). Second-order control of sequence-class equivalences in children. Behavioural Processes, 13, 205–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipkens, R., Kop, P. F. M., & Matthijs, W. (1988). A test of symmetry and transitivity in the conditional discrimination performances of pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 49, 395–409.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lovaas, O. I., Koegel, R. L., & Schreibman, L. (1979). Stimulus overselectivity in autism: A review of research. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 1236–1254.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, C. F., Horne, P. J., Harris, F. D. A., & Randle, V. R. L. (2002). Naming and categorization in young children: Vocal tact training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 527–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, C. F., Horne, P. J., & Hughes, J. C. (2005). Naming and categorization in young children: III: Vocal tact training and transfer of function. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 83, 47–65.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkron, B. (1984). Coding responses and the generalization of matching to sample in children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 42, 1–18.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkron, B. (1988). Generalization of delayed identity matching in retarded children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 163–172.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkron, B. (1998). Some logical functions of joint control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 69, 327–354.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, D. C., & Green, G. (1991). Development and crosmodal transafer of contextual control of emergent stimulus relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 139–154.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mackay, H. A. (1991). Conditional stimulus control. In I. H. Iversen & K. A. Lattal (Eds.), Experimental analysis of behavior: Part I (pp. 301–350). New York: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackay, H. A., & Sidman, M. (1984). Teaching new behavior via equivalence relations. In P. H. Brooks, R. Sperber, & C. McCauley (Eds.), Learning and cognition in the mentally retarded (pp. 493–513). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackintosh, N. J. (1977). Stimulus control attentional factors. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 481–513). NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manabe, K., Kawashima, T., & Staddon, J. E. R. (1995). Differential vocalization in budgerigars: Towards an experimental analysis of naming. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 63, 111–126.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mandell, C., & Sheen, V. (1994). Equivalence class formation as a function of the pronounceability of the sample stimuli. Behavioral Processes, 32, 29–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marcucella, H. (1974). Signalled reinforcement in differential-reinforcement-of-low-rate schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 22, 381–390.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Markham, M. R., & Dougher, M. J. (1993). Compound stimuli in emergent relations: Extending the scope of stimulus equivalence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 60, 529–542.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Markham, M. R., Dougher, M. J., & Augustson, E. A. (2002). Transfer of operant discrimination and respondent elicitation via emergent relations of compound stimuli. The Psychological Record, 52, 325–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, D., & Davison, M. (1979). Signal probability, reinforcement, and signal detection. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 32, 373–386.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McIlvane, W. J., & Dube, W. V. (2003). Stimulus control topography coherence theory: Foundations and extensions. The Behavior Analyst, 26, 195–213.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McIlvane, W. J., & Stoddard, L. T. (1981). Acquisition of matching-to-sample performances in severe retardation: Learning by exclusion. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 25, 33–48.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McIlvane, W. J., & Stoddard, L. T. (1985). Complex stimulus relations and exclusion in severe mental retardation. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 5, 307–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McIlvane, W. J., Kledaras, J. B., Munson, L. C., King, K. A., de Rose, J. C., & Stoddard, L. T. (1987). Controlling relations in conditional discrimination and matching by exclusion. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 48, 187–208.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McIlvane, W. J., Dube, W. V., Kledaras, J. B., Iennaco, F. M., & Stoddard, L. T. (1990). Teaching relational discrimination to individuals with mental retardation: Some problems and possible solutions. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 95, 283–296.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McIlvane, W. J., Kledaras, J. B., Lowry, M., & Stoddard, L. T (1992). Studies of exclusion in individuals with severe mental retardation. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 13, 509–532.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McIlvane, W. J., Kledaras, J. B., Callahan, T. C., & Dube, W. V. (2002). High probability stimulus control topographies with delayed S+ onset in a simultaneous discrimination procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 77, 189–198.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McIntire, K. D., Cleary, J., & Thompson, T. (1987). Conditional relations by monkeys: Reflexivity, symmetry, and transitivity. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 47, 279–285.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McIntire, K. D., Cleary, J., & Thompson, T. (1989). Reply to Saunders and to Hayes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 393–396.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Minster, S. T., Jones, M., Elliffe, D., & Muthukumaraswamy, S. D. (2006). Stimulus equivalence: Testing Sidman’s (2000) theory. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 85, 371–391.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, J. (2003). Explanation and description in traditional neobehaviorism, cognitive psychology, and behavior analysis. In K. Lattal & P. N. Chase (Eds.) Behavior theory and philosophy (pp. 13–39). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moxon, P. D., Keenan, M., & Hine, L. (1993). Gender-role stereotyping and stimulus equivalence. The Psychological Record, 43, 381–393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. R., & Wasserman, E. A. (1978). Temporal factors influencing the pigeon’s successive matching-to-sample performance: Sample duration, intertrial interval and retention interval. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 30, 153–162.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nevin, J. A. (1967). Effects of reinforcement scheduling on simultaneous discrimination performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10, 251–260.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nevin, J. A., & Liebold, K. (1966). Stimulus control of matching and oddity in a pigeon. Psychonomic Science, 5, 351–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nevin, J. A., Cumming, W. W., & Berryman, R. (1963). Ratio reinforcement of matching behavior. Journal of the Experimental of Behavior, 6, 149–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oden, D. L., Thompson, R. K. R., & Premack, D. (1988). Spontaneous transfer of matching by infant chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 14, 140–145.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Donnell, J., & Saunders, K. J. (2003). Equivalence relations in individuals with language limitations and mental retardation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 80, 131–157.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Overman, W. H., & Doty, R. W. (1980). Prolonged visual memory in macaques and man. Neuroscience, 5, 1825–1831.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pena, T., Pitts, R. C., & Galizio, M. (2006). Identity matching-to-sample with olfactory stimuli in rats. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 85, 203–221.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, D. R., Dougher, M. J., & Greeway, D. E. (2007). Contextual control by function and form of transfer of functions. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 88, 87–102.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, G. B. (1984). How expectancies guide behavior. In H. L. Roitblat, T. G. Bever, & H. S. Terrace (Eds.), Animal cognition (pp. 135–148). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, R. F., & Whitehurst, G. J. (1971). A variable influencing the performance of generalized imitative behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4, 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, G. B., Wheeler, R. L., & Armstrong, G. D. (1978). Expectancies as mediators in the differential-reward conditional discrimination performance of pigeons. Animal Learning & Behavior, 6, 279–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pilgrim, C., & Galizio, M. (1990). Relations between baseline contingencies and equivalence probe performances. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54, 213–224.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pilgrim, C., & Galizio, M. (1995). Reversal of baseline relations and stimulus equivalence: I: Adults. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 63, 225–238.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pilgrim, C., & Galizio, M. (1996). Stimulus equivalence: A class of correlations, or a correlation of classes? In T. R. Zentall & P. M. Smeets (Eds.), Stimulus class formation in humans and animals (pp. 173–195). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Pilgrim, C., Chambers, L., & Galizio, M. (1995). Reversal of baseline relations and stimulus equivalence: II. Children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 63, 239–254.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pilgrim, C., Jackson, J., & Galizio, M. (2000). Acquisition of arbitrary conditional discriminations by young typically developing children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 73, 177–193.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pilgrim, C., Click, R., & Galizio, M. (2011). A developmental analysis of children’s equivalence-class formation and disruption. Acta Psicologica, 1, 55–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poole, J., & Lander, D. G. (1971). The pigeon’s concept of pigeon. Psychonomic Science, 25, 157–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Porter, D., & Neuringer, A. (1984). Music discriminations by pigeons. Journal of Experimetnal Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 10, 138–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poulson, C. L., Kymissis, E., Reeve, K. F., Andreatos, M., & Reeve, L. (1991). Generalized vocal imitation in infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 267–279.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Randell, T., & Remington, B. (1999). Equivalence relations between visual stimuli: The functional role of naming. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 71, 395–415.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ray, B. A., & Sidman, M. (1970). Reinforcement schedules and stimulus control. In W. N. Schoenfeld (Ed.), The theory of reinforcement schedules (pp. 187–214). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rehfeldt, R. A., & Root, S. (2004). The generalization and long-term retention of equivalence relations in adults with mental retardation. The Psychological Record, 54, 173–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, G. S. (1961). Attention in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4, 203–208.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rilling, M. (1977). Stimulus control and inhibitory processes. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 432–480). NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rilling, M., Caplan, H., Howard, R. C., & Brown, C. H. (1975). Inhibitory stimulus control following errorless discrimination learning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 24, 121–133.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, W. A., & Grant, D. S. (1976). Studies of short-term memory in the pigeon using the delayed matching to sample procedure. In D. L. Medin, W. A. Roberts, & R. T. Davis (Eds.), Processes of Animal Memory (pp. 79–112). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roche, B., & Barnes, D. (1997). A transformation of respondently conditioned stimulus function in accordance with arbitrarily applicable relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 67, 275–301.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roche, B., Barnes, D., & Smeets, P. M. (1997). Incongruous stimulus pairing and conditional discrimination training: Effects on relational responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 68, 143–160.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rodewald, H. K. (1974a). All-or-none acquisition in matching-to-sample and a test of two models. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 22, 53–60.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rodewald, H. K. (1974b). Symbolic matching-to-sample by pigeon. Psychological Reports, 34, 987–990.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. B. (1975). Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573–605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, A. P., & Terrace, H. S. (1975). On the minimal conditions for the development of a peak-shift and inhibitory stimulus control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 23, 385–414.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, R. A., Kamil, A. C., & Mack, R. (1972). The effects of fixed-ratio sample requirements on matching to sample in the pigeon. Psychonomic Science, 26, 291–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Santi, A. (1978). The role of physical identity of the sample and the correct comparison stimulus in matching-to-sample paradigms. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 29, 511–516.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, K. J. (1989). Naming in conditional discrimination and stimulus equivalence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 379–384.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, R. R., & Green, G. (1992). The nonequivalence of behavioral and mathematical equivalence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 57, 227–241.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, R. R., & Green, G. (1999). A discrimination analysis of training-structure effects on stimulus equivalence outcomes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 72, 117–137.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, K. J., & Spradlin, J. E. (1989). Conditional discrimination in mentally retarded adults: The effect of training the component simple discriminations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 1–12.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, K. J., & Spradlin, J. E. (1990). Conditional discrimination in mentally retarded adults: The development of generalized skills. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54, 239–250.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, K. J., & Spradlin, J. E. (1993). Conditional discrimination in mentally retarded subjects: Programming acquisition and learning set. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 60, 571–585.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, K. J., & Williams, D. C. (1998a). Do parakeets exhibit derived stimulus control? Some thoughts on experimental control procedures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 70, 321–324.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, K. J., & Williams, D. C. (1998b). Stimulus-control procedures. In K. A. Lattal & M. Perone (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in human operant behavior (pp. 193–228). New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, R. R., Saunders, K. J., Kirby, K. C., & Spradlin, J. E. (1988a). The merger of equivalence classes by unreinforced conditional selection of comparison stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 145–162.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, R. R., Wachter, J. A., & Spradlin, J. E. (1988b). Establishing auditory stimulus control over an eight-member equivalence class via conditional discrimination procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 49, 95–115.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, K. J., Saunders, R. R., Williams, D. C., & Spradlin, J. E. (1993). An interaction of instructions and training design on stimulus class formation: Extending the analysis of equivalence. The Psychological Record, 43, 725–744.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, K. J., Williams, D. C., & Spradlin, J. E. (1995). Conditional discrimination by adults with mental retardations: Establishing relations between physically identical stimuli. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 99, 558–563.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, R. R., Drake, K. M., & Spradlin, J. E. (1999). Equivalence class establishment, expansion, and modification in preschool children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 71, 195–214.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Rumbaugh, D. M., Smith, S. T., & Lawson, J. (1980). Reference: The linguistic essential. Science, 210, 922–925.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schoenfeld, W. N., & Cumming, W. W. (1963). Perception and behavior. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of science (Vol. 5, pp. 213–252). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schreibman, L. (1997). The study of stimulus control in autism. In D. M. Baer & E. M. Pinkston (Eds.), Environment and behavior (pp. 203–209). Boudler: Westview.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schusterman, R. J., & Kastak, D. (1993). A California sea lion (Zalophus californianus) is capable of forming equivalence relations. Psychological Record, 43, 823–839.

    Google Scholar 

  • Serna, R. W. (1991). Interchangeability of stimulus terms in five-term contingencies. Experimental Analysis of Human Behavior Bulletin, 9, 2–3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Serna, R. W., Dube, W. V., & McIlvane, W. J. (1997). Assessing same/different judgments inindividuals withsever intellectural disabilities: A status report. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 18, 343–368.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shahan, T. A. (2013). Attention and conditioned reinforcement. In G. J. Madden (Ed.), APA handbook of behavior analysis (pp. 387–410). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimp, C. P., Sabulsky, S. L., & Childers, L. J. (1989). Preference for starting and finishing behavior patterns. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 341–352.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1971). Reading and auditory-visual equivalences. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 14, 5–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1977). Teaching some basic prerequisites for reading. In P. Mittler (Ed.), Research to practice in mental retardation: Vol. 2 Education and training (pp. 353–360). Baltimore: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1979). Remarks. Behaviorism, 7, 123–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1986). Functional analysis of emergent verbal classes. In T. Thompson & M. D. Zeiler (Eds.), Analysis and integration of behavioral units (pp. 213–245). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1987). Two choices are not enough. Behavior Analysis, 22, 11–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1992). Equivalence relations: Some basic considerations. In S. C. Hayes, & L. J. Hayes (Eds.), Understanding verbal relations (pp. 15–27). Reno: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behavior: A research story. Boston: Authors Cooperative.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (2000). Equivalence relations and the reinforcement contingency. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 74, 127–146.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M., & Cresson, O. (1973). Reading and crossmodal transfer of stimulus equivalences in severe retardation. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 77, 515–523.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M., & Stoddard, L. T. (1966). Programming perception and learning for retarded children. In N. R. Ellis (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation (Vol. 2, pp. 151–208). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M., & Stoddard, L. T. (1967). The effectiveness of fading in programming a simultaneous form discrimination for retarded children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10, 3–15.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M., & Tailby, W. (1982). Conditional discrimination vs. matching to sample: An expansion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 5–22.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M., Rauzin, R., Lazar, R., Cunningham, S., Tailby, W., & Carrigan, P. (1982). A search for symmetry in the conditional discriminations of rhesus monkeys, baboons, and children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 23–44.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M., Kirk, B., & Willson-Morris, M. (1985). Six-member stimulus classes generated by conditional-discrimination procedures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, 21–42.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M., Willson-Morris, M., & Kirk, B. (1986). Matching-to-sample procedures and the development of equivalence relations: The role of naming. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 6, 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M., Wynne, C. K., Maquire, R. W., & Barnes, T. (1989). Functional classes and equivalence relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 261–274.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sigurdardottir, Z. G., Green, G., & Saunders, R. R. (1990). Equivalence classes generated by sequence training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, 47–63.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1931). The concept of the reflex in the description of behavior. Journal of General Psychology, 5, 427–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1935a). The generic nature of the concepts of stimulus and response. Journal of General Psychology, 12, 40–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1935b). Two types of conditioned reflex and a pseudotype. Journal of General Psychology, 12, 66–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1969). The experimental analysis of behavior. Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis (pp. 75–104). Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1972). Experimental psychology. Cumulative Record (pp. 295–313). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spradlin, J. E., & Saunders, R. (1986). The development of stimulus classes using match-to-sample procedures: Sample classification versus comparison classification. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 6, 41–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spradlin, J. E., Cotter, V. W., & Baxley, N. (1973). Establishing a conditional discrimination without direct training: A study of transfer with retarded adolescents. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 77, 556–566.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spradlin, J. E., Saunders, K. J., & Saunders, R. R. (1992). The stability of equivalence classes. In S. C. Hayes & L. J. Hayes (Eds.), Understanding verbal relations (pp. 29–42). Reno: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steele, D., & Hayes, S. C. (1991). Stimulus equivalence and arbitrarily applicable relational responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 519–555.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, I., Barnes-Holmes, D., Roche, B., & Smeets, P. M. (2002). A functional-analytic model of analogy: A relational frame analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 375–396.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stoddard, L. T., & McIlvane, W. J. (1989). Establishing auditory stimulus control in profoundly retarded individuals. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 10, 141–151.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1977). An implicit technology of generalization. Journal of applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 349–367.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stromer, R., McIlvane, W. J., Dube, W. V., & Mackay, H. A. (1993a). Assessing control by elements of complex stimuli in delayed matching to sample. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59, 83–102.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stromer, R., McIlvane, W. J., & Serna, R. W. (1993b). Complex stimulus control and equivalence. The Psychological Record, 43, 585–598.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sundberg, M. L., & Partington, J. W. (1998). Teaching language to children with autism or other developmental disabilities. Pleasant Hill: Behavior Analysts, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S. (1963a). Discrimination learning with and without ‘errors’. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 6, 1–27.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S. (1963b). Errorless transfer of a discrimination across two continua. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 6, 223–232.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, M. (1970). Discrimination of auditory intensities by rates. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 13, 145–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, D. R. (1969). The use of operant conditioning techniques to investigate perceptual processes in animals. In R. M. Gibert & N. S. Sutherland (Eds.), Animal discrimination learning (pp. 1–33). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tonneau, F. (2001). Equivalence relations: A critical analysis. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 2, 1–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trapold, M. (1970). Are expectancies based upon different positive reinforcing events discriminably different? Learning & Motivation, 1, 129–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Urcuioli, P. J. (1984). Overshadowing in matching-to-sample: Reduction in sample-stimulus control by differential sample behaviors. Animal Learning & Behavior, 12, 256–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Urcuioli, P. J. (1985). On the role of differential sample behaviors in matching-to-sample. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 11, 502–519.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Urcuioli, P. J. (2001). Categorization and acquired equivalence. In. R. G. Cook (Ed.), Avian visual cognition (a cyberbook). Comparative Cognition Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Urcuioli, P. J. (2013). Stimulus control and stimulus class formation. In G. J. Madden (Ed.), APA handbook of behavior analysis (pp. 361–386). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Urcuioli, P. J., & Nevin, J. A. (1975). Transfer of hue matching in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 24, 149–155.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vaughn, W. Jr.. (1988). Formation of equivalence sets in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 14, 36–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wasserman, E. A., & DeVolder, C. L. (1993). Similarity- and nonsimilarity-based conceptualization in children and pigeons. Psychological Record, 43, 779–793.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wasserman, E. A., DeVolder, C. L., & Coppage, D. J. (1992). Nonsimilarity-based conceptualization in pigeons via secondary or mediated generalization. Psychological Science, 3, 374–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watanabe, S., Sakamoto, J., & Wakita, M. (1995). Pigeons’ discrimination of paintings by Monet and Picasso. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 63, 165–174.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watt, A., Keenan, M., Barnes, D., & Cairns, E. (1991). Social categorization and stimulus equivalence. The Psychological Record, 41, 33–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weisberg, P., & Rovee-Collier, C. (1998). Behavioral processes of infants and young children. In K. A. Lattal & M. Perone (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in human operant behavior (pp. 325–370). New York: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, B., & Laties, V. G. (1969). Behavioral pharmacology and toxicology. Annual Review of Pharmacology, 9, 297–326.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, S. J., & Schindler, C. W. (1981). Generalization peak shift in rats under conditions of positive reinforcement and avoidance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 35, 175–185.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wetherby, B., Karlan, G. R., & Spradlin, J. E. (1983). The development of derived stimulus relations through training in arbitrary-matching sequences. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 40, 69–78.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, K. G. (2013). Remembering and forgetting. In G. J. Madden (Ed.), APA handbook of behavior analysis (pp. 411–438). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkie, D. M., & Masson, M. E. (1976). Attention in the pigeon: A reevaluation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 26, 207–212.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, K. M., & McIlvane, W. J. (2001). Methods for studying symbolic behavior and category formation: Contributions of stimulus equivalence research. Developmental Review, 21, 355–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, D. C., & Saunders, K. J. (1997). Methodological issues in the study of drug effect on cognitive skills in mental retardation. In N. W. Bray (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation (Vol. 21, pp. 147–185). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wirth, O., & Chase, P. N. (2002). Stability of functional equivalence and stimulus equivalence: Effects of baseline reversals. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 77, 29–47.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, A. A. (1997). Concept learning and learning strategies. Psychological Science, 8, 119–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, A. A., Cook, R. G., Rivera, J. J., Sands, S. F., & Delius, J. D. (1988). Concept learning by pigeons: Matching-to-sample with trial-unique video picture stimuli. Animal Learning & Behavior, 16, 436–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wulfert, E., & Hayes, S. C. (1988). Transfer of a conditional ordering response through conditional equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 125–144.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wulfert, E., Dougher, M. J., & Greenway, D. E. (1991). Protocol analysis of the correspondence of verbal behavior and equivalence class formation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 489–504.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wulfert, E., Greenway, D. E., & Dougher, M. J. (1994). Third-order equivalence classes. The Psychological Record, 44, 411–439.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wyckoff, L. B. Jr. (1969). The role of observing responses in discrimination learning. In D. P. Hendry (Ed.), Conditioned reinforcement (pp. 237–260). Homewood: Dorsey Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zentall, T. R., & Hogan, D. E. (1974). Abstract concept learning in the pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, 393–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zentall, T. R., & Urcuioli, P. J. (1993). Emergent relations in the formation of stimulus classes by pigeons. The Psychological Record, 43, 795–810.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zentall, T. R., Edwards, C. A., Moore, B. S., & Hogan, D. E. (1981). Identity: The basis for both matching and oddity learning in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 7, 70–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, J., & Ferster, C. B. (1963). Intermittent punishment of S-delta responding in matching to sample. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 6, 349–356.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zygmont, D. M., Lazar, R. M., Dube, W. V., & McIlvane, W. J. (1992). Teaching arbitrary matching via sample stimulus-control shaping to young children and mentally retarded individuals: A methodological note. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 57, 109–117.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Carol Pilgrim .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Pilgrim, C. (2015). Stimulus Control and Generalization. In: DiGennaro Reed, F., Reed, D. (eds) Autism Service Delivery. Autism and Child Psychopathology Series. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-2656-5_2

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics