Skip to main content

Consciousness Regained

The Scientific Restoration of Mind and Brain

  • Chapter
The Life Cycle of Psychological Ideas

Part of the book series: Path in Psychology

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Adelman, G.& Smith, B. (1995). Francis O. Schmitt. In Biographical Memiors. Vol. (pp. 524–545) National Academy of Sciences. Washington D.C.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aldophs, R. (2003). Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscienc, 4, 165–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adrian, E. D. (1972). Forty years’ progress in neurophysiology. In J-P Cordeau, & P Gloor. (Eds.), Recent contributions to neurophysiology: International symposium in neurosciences in honor of Herbert H. Jasper. Amsterdam. (pp. 3–7). Elsevier Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. (1986). The cognitive revolution in psychology. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. (1988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. (1997). In the theater of consciousness: The workspace of the mind. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B (2002). The conscious access hypothesis: Origins and recent evidence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 47–52.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barnard, W. G. (2002). The Varieties of Religious Experience: Reflections on its enduring value. In M. Ferrari, (Ed.), The varieties of religious experience: Centenary essays. Special Issue. Journal of Consciousness Studie, 9, 57–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E. A. (1999). Plasticity, localization and language development In H. Tager-Flusberg (Ed.), Neurodevelopmental disorders (214–253). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beaulieu, A. (2002). A space for measuring mind and brain: Interdisciplinary and digital tools in the development of brain mapping and functional imaging, 1980–1990. Brain and Cognition, 49, 13–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-David, J. & Collins, R. (1966). Social factors in the origins of a new science: The case of psychology. American Sociological Review, 31, 451–465.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blumenthal, A. L. (1979). WilhelmWundt, the founding father we never knew. Contemporary Psychology, 24, 547–550.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blank, R. H. (1999). Brain policy: How the new neuroscience will change our lives and our politics. Washington, D. C.: Georgetown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M.& Books-Gunn, J. (1979). Social cognition and the acquisition of self. NewYork: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cabeza, R. & Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 1–47.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chalmers, D. (1995). “Facing Up to the Problem of Consciousness.“ Journal of Consciousness Studies, 2, 200–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, J. (1994). ‘Toward a scientific basis for consciousness: University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona, April 12th–17th 1994. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 1, 152–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, E. (2003). Neural Darwinism: A framework for transforming and reinvigorating undergraduate education. Library Roundtable Address, The Neurosciences Institute, San Diego, CA. (February 5).

    Google Scholar 

  • Connolly, W. E. (1973). Theoretical self-consciousness. Polity, 6, 5–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corballis, M. C. (2002). From hand to mouth: The origins of language. (Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crick, F. & Koch, C. (2003). A framework for consciousness. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 119–126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dalton, T. C. (1998), Myrtle McGraw’s neurobehavioral theory of development. Developmental Review, 18 472–503.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalton, T. C. (1999). The ontogeny of consciousness: John Dewey and Myrtle McGraw’s contribution to a science of mind. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6, 3–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalton, T. C. (2000). The developmental roots of consciousness and emotional experience. Consciousness and Emotion, 1, 57–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalton, T. C. (2002). Becoming John Dewey: Dilemmas of a philosopher and naturalist. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalton, T. C. and Bergenn, V. W. (Eds.). (1995). Beyond heredity and environment: Myrtle McGraw and the maturation controversy. (Boulder, CO: Westview Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalton, T. C. and Bergenn, V. W. (1996). John Dewey, Myrtle McGraw and Logic: An unusual collaboration in the 1930s, Studies in History and Philosophy of Science, 27, 69–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. (2000). A neurobiology for consciousness. In T. Metzinger, (Ed.), Neural correlates of consciousness: Empirical and conceptual questions (pp. 11–120). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. (2003). Looking for Spinoza: Joy, Sorrow and the feeling brain. New York: Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danziger, K. (1979). The positivist repudiation ofWundt. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 15, 205–226.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dewsbury, D. (1996). Introduction. In D. Dewsbury, (Ed.), Unification through divisions: Histories of the divisions of the American Psychological Association (1–8). Washington, D. C. APA Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dupuy, J. P. (1994). The mechanization of the mind: The origins of cognitive science. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. C. (1961a). The effects of use and disuse on synaptic function. In Fessard, A., Gerard, R. W. & J. Konorski, (Eds.), Brain mechanisms and learning. A symposium organized by the Council for International Organizations of Medical Sciences (pp. 335–352) Springfield, Ill: Charles C. Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. C. (1961b). General discussion. Fessard, A., Gerard, R.W. & J. Konorski. (1961). Brain mechanisms and learning. A symposium organized by the Council for International organizations of medical sciences (pp. 655–660). Springfield, Ill: Charles C. Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M. (1974). Molecular recognition in the immune and nervous systems. In F. G. Worden, J. P. Swazey & G. Adelman, (Eds.), The neurosciences: Paths of discovery (pp. (64–74) Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M. (1987). Neural Darwinism: The theory of neuronal group selection. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M. (1988). Topobiology: An introduction to molecular embryology. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M. (1989). The remembered present: A biological view of consciousness. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M. (1992). Bright air, brilliant fire: On the matter of mind. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M. (1999). A road to science: Gerald Edelman, An interview. The Chemical Intelligencer, July, pp, 18–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. & Tononi, G. (2000). A universe of consciousness. How matter become imagination. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M. & Gally, J. (2001). Degeneracy and complexity in biological systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 13763–13768.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Editors. (1996). Toward a science of consciousness—Tucson II conference report. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 3, 278–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Editors. (1997). The future of consciousness studies. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 4, 385–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eigen, M. & De Maeyer, L. C. M. (1966). Summary of two NRP work sessions on information storage and processing in iomolecular systems. In F. O. Schmitt & T. Melnechuk, (Eds.), Neurosciences reseearch symposium summaries, Vol. 1. An anthology from the neurosciences research program bulletin (pp. 238–254). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M., Karmiloff-Smith, A. Parisi, D. & Plunkett, K. (Eds.) (1998). Redefining Innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellenberger, H. F. (1970). The discovery of the unconscious: The history and evolution of dynamic psychiatry. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erdelyi, M. H. (1974). A new look at the New Look: Perceptual defense and vigilance. Psychological Review, 81, 1–25.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fessard. A. E. (1954a). Mechanisms of nervous integration and conscious experience. In E. D. Adrian, F. Bremer & H. H. Jasper (Eds.), Brain mechanisms and consciousness. A symposium organized by the Council for International Organizations of Medical Sciences (pp. 200–236). Springfield, Ill: Charles C. Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fessard, A. (1954b). General Discussion. In E. D. Adrian, F. Bremer & H. H. Jasper (Eds.), Brain mechanisms and consciousness. A symposium organized by the Council for International Organizations of Medical Sciences (pp. 507–510). Springfield, Ill: Charles C. Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finger, S. (1994). Origins of Neuroscience: A history of explorations into the brain. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finger, S. (2000). Minds behind the brain: A history of the pioneers and their discoveries. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foerster, H. von (1949). Introductory discusssion. In H. von Foerster, (Ed.), Cybernetics. Circular causal, and feedback mechanisms in biological and social systems. Transactions of the sixth conference, March 24–25, New York City (pp. 9–26). New York, NY: Josiah Macy, Jr. Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank, L. K. (1962). The beginnings of child development and family life education in the twentieth century. The Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 8, 207–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, A. (1994). The science of consciousness: Non-locality of mind. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 1, 283–284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fremont-Smith, F. (1950). The Josiah Macy, Jr. conference program. In D. Nachmansohn, (Ed.), Nerve Impulse Transactions of the First Conference, March 2–3, 1950, (pp. 7–8). New York City. New York: Josiah Macy, Jr. Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fremont-Smith, F. (1951). The Josiah Macy, Jr. Foundation conference program. In H. A. Abramson, (Ed.), Problems of consciousness: Transactions of the Second Conference, March 19–20, 1951. New York: The Josiah Macy, Jr. Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, R. (1996). Preface. In D. Dewsbury, (Ed.), Unification through divisions: Histories of the divisions of the American Psychological Association (xv–xx). Washington, D. C. APA Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C. & Frith, U. (1999). “Interacting minds: A biological basis,“ Science, 286, 1692–1695.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fuller, S. (2000). Thomas Kuhn: A philosophical history for our times. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallup, G. (1970). Chimpanzees: Self-Recognition. Scienc, 167, 341–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M. & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An Fmri investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293, 2105–2108.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenough, W. T., Black, J. E.& Wallace, C. (1993). Brain adaptation to experience: An update. In M. H. Johnson, (Ed.), Brain development and cognition: A reader (pp. 290–318). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gross, C. G. (1998). Brain, vision, memory: Tales in the history of neuroscience. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J. & Cori, C. F. (1974). Francis O. Schmitt—A profile. In F. G. Worden, J. P. Swazey & G. Adelman, (Eds.), The neurosciences: Paths of discovery (pp. xv–xxi). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harman, W. (1994). The scientific exploration of consciousness: Towards an adequate epistemology: Journal of Consciousness Studies, 1, 140–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1954). The problem of consciousness and introspection. In E. D. Adrian, F. Bremer & H. H. Jasper (Eds.), Brain mechanisms and consciousness. A symposium organized by the Council for International Organizations of Medical Sciences (pp. 402–417). Springfield, Ill: Charles C. Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1961). General discussion. Fessard, A., Gerard, R. W. & J. Konorski. (1961). Brain mechanisms and learning. Asymposium organized by the Council for International organizations of medical sciences (pp. 661–663). Springfield, Ill: Charles C. Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heims, S. J. (1991). The cybernetics group. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurley, S. (1998). Consciousness in Action (Cambridge: Harvard University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1904). “Does consciousness exist?“ In J. J. McDermott, (Ed.), The writings of William James (pp. 169–183). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jasper, H. H. (1966). Final discussion. In J. C. Eccles, (Ed.), Brain and conscious experience. Study week September 28 to October 4, 1964 of the Potificia Academia Scientiarum. (pp. 551). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jasper, H. H. (1974). Philosophy or physics—mind or molecules. In F. G. Worden, J. P. Swazey & G. Adelman, (Eds.), The neurosciences: Paths of discovery (pp. 403–422). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jasper, H. H. (1996). Herbert H. Jasper. In L. R. Squire, (Ed.), The history of neuroscience in autobiography (318–347). Washington, D. C.: Society for Neuroscience.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jasper, H. H. (1998). Historical perspective. In H. H. Jasper, L. Descarries, V. F. Castellucci & S. Rossignol, (Eds.), Consciousness at the frontiers of neuroscience (1–6). Philadelphia: Lippincott.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. & Lakoff, M. (1999). Philosophy in the Flesh. The emodied mind and its challenge to Western thought. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kast, L. (1937). Review by the president of the activities for the six years ending December 31, 1936. New York: Josiah Macy, Jr. Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lashley, K. S. (1948). The problem of serial order of behavior. In L. A. Jeffress, (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior: The Hixon Symposium (pp. 112–130). New York: Hafner.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lashley, K. S. (1954). Dynamic processes in perception. In E. D. Adrian, F. Bremer & H. H. Jasper (Eds.), Brain mechanisms and consciousness. A symposium organized by the Council for International Organizations of Medical Sciences (pp. 422–436). Springfield, Ill: Charles C. Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. (2002). Synaptic self: How our brains become who we are. New York: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Libet, B. (1966). Brain stimulation and the threshold of conscious experience. In E. D. Adrian, F. Bremer & H. H. Jasper (Eds.), Brain mechanisms and consciousness. Asymposium organized by the Council for International Organizations of Medical Sciences (pp. 165–181). Springfield, Ill: Charles C. Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Libet, B., Wright, E. W., Jr. & Gleason, C. A. (1983a). Preparation-or intention-to-act, in relation to pre-event potentials recorded at the vertex. Elecroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 56, 367–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Libet, B., Gleason, C. A Wright, E.W., Jr. & Pearl, D. K. (1983b). Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. Brain, 106, 127–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Licklider, J. C. L. (1950). The manner in which and extent to which speech can be distorted and remain intelligible. In H. von Foerster, (Ed.), Cybernetics. Circular causal, and feedback mechanisms in biological and social systems. Transactions of the sixth conference, March 23–24, New York City. New York, (pp. 58–122). NY: Josiah Macy, Jr. Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCulloch, W. S. (1948). Why the mind is in the head. In L. A. Jeffress, (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior: The Hixon Symposium (pp. 42–57). New York: Hafner.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. M. (1966). Cerebral organization and the conscious control of action. In J. C. Eccles, (Ed.), Brain and conscious experience. Study week September 28 to October 4, 1964 of the Potificia Academia Scientiarum. (pp. 422–440). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • McIntosh, A. R. (2000). Towards a nework theory of cognition. Neural Network, 13, 861–870.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magoun, H. W. (1974). The role of research institutes in the advancement of neuroscience: Ranson’s Institute of Neurology, 1928–1942. In F. G. Worden, J. P. Swazey & G. Adelman, (Eds.), The neurosciences: Paths of discovery (pp. 515–527). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malcom, N. (1967). Wittgenstein. In P. Edwards (Ed.), The Encyclopedia of Philosophy, Vol. 8. New York: Macmillan/Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. A. (1975). Consciousness: Respectable, useful and probably necessary. In R. Solso, (Ed.), Information Processing and cognition: The Loyola Symposium. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metzinger, T. (2000). Introduction. In T. Metzinger, (Ed.), Neural correlates of consciousness: Empirical and conceptual questions (pp. 1–12). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merzenich, M. M. & deCharms, R. C. (1996). Neural representations, experience and change. In R. llinais and P. Churchland, (Eds.), The mind-brain continuum (pp. 61–81). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merzenich, M. M. (2001). Cortical plasticity contributing to child development. In J. L. McCleland & R. S. Siegler, (Eds.), Mechanisms of cognitive development: Behavioral and neural perspectives (pp. 67–96). Mahwah, N. J.: Lawerence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (2003). The cognitive revolution: A historical perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 141–144.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Montague, P. R., Berns, G. S., Cohen, J. D., McClure, S. M., Pagnoni, G., Dhamala, M., Wiest, M. C., Igor Karpov, King, R. D., Apple, N. & Fisher, R. E. (2002). Hyperscanning: Simultaneous fMRI during Linked Social Interactions. Neuroimag, 16, 1159–1164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mountcastle, V. B. (1966). Final discussion. In J. C. Eccles, (Ed.), Brain and conscious experience. Study week September 28 to October 4, 1964 of the Potificia Academia Scientiarum. (pp. 548–550). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mutalik, P. (1998). Sweet music in Bremen.: Second ASSC annual conference, 19–21 June, 1998. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 5, 734–738.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neville, H. J. (1993). Neurobiology of cognitive and language processing: Effects of early experience. In M. H. Johnson (Ed.), Brain development and cognition (pp. 424–448). Cambridge, MA: Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norberg, A. L. (1988). Oral history interview with J. C. R. Licklider, 28 October 1988, Cambridge, Massachusetts. Charles Babbage Institute. Center for the history of information technology. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overton, W. F. (1998). Developmental psychology: Philosophy, concepts and methodology. In W. Damon, (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. I 5th ed. (pp. 107–188). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paul, Pope VI. (1966). The audience and address. In J. C. Eccles, (Ed.), Brain and conscious experience. Study week September 28 to October 4, 1964 of the Potificia Academia Scientiarum. (pp. xix–xxi). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paul, Pope John II. (1990). Message of his Holiness, John Paul II. In R. J. Russell, W. Stoeger & G. V. Coyne, (Eds.), John Paul II on science and religion. Reflections on the new view from Rome, (pp. 1–14). Rome: Vatican Observatory Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penfield, W. (1972). Herbert Jasper. In J-P Cordeau, & P Gloor. (Eds.), Recent contributions to neurophysiology: International symposium in neurosciences in honor of Herbert H. Jasper. Amsterdam. (pp. 9–12). Elsevier Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penfield, W. (1974). The mind and the brain. In F. G. Worden, J. P. Swazey & G. Adelman, (Eds.), The neurosciences: Paths of discovery (pp. 437–454). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D. & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 4, 515–526.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran, V. S. (1998). Phantoms in the brain: Probing the mysteries of the human mind. New York: William Morrow and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapaport, D. (1951). Consciousness: A psychopathological and psychodynamic view. Discussion. In H. A. Abramson, (Ed.), Problems of consciousness: Transactions of the Second Conference, March 19–20 (pp. 28–57). New York: The Josiah Macy, Jr. Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rappleye, W. (1955). The Josiah Macy, Jr. Foundation: Twentieth Anniversary Review, 1930–1955. New York: The Josiah Macy, Jr. Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. London: Hutchinson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenfield, I. (1986). Neural Darwinism: A new approach to memory and perception. New York Review of Books, October 9, pp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sachs, O. (1994). A new vision of mind. In O. Sporns & G. Tononi, (Eds.), Selectionism and the brain (pp. 101–121). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlosberg, H. (1951). Consciousness: Psychopathological and PsychodynamicViews: Discussion. In H. Abramson, (Ed.), Problems of Consciousness. Transactions of the second conference. March 19–20, (pp. 28–57). New York: Josiah Macy, Jr. Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, F. O. (1992). The neurosciences research program: A brief history. In F. Samson & G. Adelman, (Eds.), The neurosciences: Paths of discovery II, (pp. 1–21). Boston: Birkhäuser.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, F. O. (1990). The never-ceasing search. Philadelphia: American Philosophical Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, J. M. & Begley, S. (2002). The mind and the brain: Neuroplasticity and the power of mental force. New York: Harper-Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperry, R.W. (1974). In search of psyche. In F. G. Worden, J. P. Swazey & G. Adelman, (Eds.), The neurosciences: Paths of discovery (pp. 424–434). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Srinvasan, R., Russell, D. P., Edelman, G. M., & Tononi, G. (1999). Increased synchronization of neuromagnetic responses during conscious perception. The Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 5435–5448.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stigler, J. W. Breakthroughs in using individual differences to study learning: Comments on Goldin-Meadow, Haier, McCllelland, Merzenich, and Siegler. In J. L. McCleland & R. S. Siegler, (Eds.), Mechanisms of cognitive development: Behavioral and neural perspectives (pp. 149–160.). Mahwah, N. J.: Lawerence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutherland, K. (1994). Consciousness—Its place in contemporary science. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 1, 285–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swazey, J. P. (1974). Forging a neuroscientific community: A brief history of the neurosciences research program. In F. G.Worden, J. P. Swazey & G. Adelman, (Eds.), The neurosciences: Paths of discovery (pp. 528–546). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swazey, J. P. & Worden, F. G. (1974). On the nature of research in neuroscience. In F. G. Worden, J. P. Swazey & G. Adelman, (Eds.), The neurosciences: Paths of discovery (pp. 568–587). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, C. (1989). The sources of the self: The making of modern identity. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, C. (1994). Reply and rearticulation. In J. Tulley, (Ed.), Philosophy in an age of pluralism: The philosophy of Charles Taylor in question (pp. 213–257). NewYork: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, E. (2002). William James and depth psychology. In M. Ferrari, (Ed.), The varieties of religious experience: Centenary essays. Special Issue. Journal of Consciousness Studie, 9, 11–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E. & Smith, L. (1998). Dynamic systems theory. In R. Lerner, (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 1. (pp. 563–634), New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorpe, W. H. (1966). Ethology and consciousness. In J. C. Eccles, (Ed.), Brain and conscious experience. Study week September 28 to October 4, 1964 of the Potificia Academia Scientiarum. (pp. 470–495). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tononi, G. & Edelman, G. (1998). Consciousness and complexity. Science, 282, 1846–1851.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiener, N. (1948). Cybernetics. (New York: JohnWiley and Sons).

    Google Scholar 

  • Windle, W. F. (1979). The pioneering role of Clarence Luther Herrick in American Neuroscience. New York: Exposition Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. R. & Zelazo, P. D. (1998). The emergence of consciousness. In H. H. Jasper, L. Descarries, V. F. Castellucci & S. Rossignol, (Eds.), Consciousness at the frontiers of neuroscience (1–6). Philadelphia: Lippincott.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D. (2000). Self-reflection and the development of consciously controlled processing. In P. Mitchell & K. Riggs (Eds.), Children’s reasoning and mind (pp. 169–189) New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2005 Springer Science + Business Media, Inc.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Dalton, T.C., Baars, B.J. (2005). Consciousness Regained. In: Dalton, T.C., Evans, R.B. (eds) The Life Cycle of Psychological Ideas. Path in Psychology. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/0-306-48010-7_9

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/0-306-48010-7_9

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-0-306-47998-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-0-306-48010-2

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics