Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Syst. Neurosci., 03 July 2014
Volume 8 - 2014 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2014.00124

Approach, avoidance, and their conflict: the problem of anchoring

  • 1Department of Psychology and Brain Health Research Centre, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
  • 2Department of Psychology, City University London, London, UK

To understand the neurobiology of individual differences in approach and avoidance behavior, we must anchor constructs at the behavioral level to the long-term global sensitivities of the neural systems that give rise to the observed stable patterns of behavior. We will argue that this requires not only appropriate data at both the neural and behavioral levels but also appropriate account to be taken of interactions at the intervening level of the conceptual nervous system (Hebb, 1949; Gray, 1975). In particular, in accounting for approach and avoidance behavior we must include consideration of the distinction between valuation and motivation (Corr and McNaughton, 2012), of interactions between the approach system and the avoidance system (Gray and Smith, 1969), and of their interaction with a distinct additional system that is activated by approach-avoidance conflict (Gray, 1977; summarized in Corr, 2013).

But first we need to ask why would we expect there to be traits linked to global approach and avoidance systems? Simple animals (with little or no brain) can produce approach and avoidance behavior (toward benefits and ultimately reproduction; and away from dangers and ultimately failure to reproduce) via multiple independent rules of thumb (Krebs et al., 1983). But we can expect more complex brains to have largely integrated these simple elements into systems more generally dedicated to approach or avoidance “because this is how [a few] genes can build a complex system that will produce appropriate but flexible behavior to increase fitness. … Rather than just pre-programmed movements such as tropisms and taxes, … if the genes are efficiently to control behavior … they must specify the goals for action.” (Rolls, 2000, pp. 183, 190). Together with the evolution of general approach and avoidance systems that are not tied to any specific motivating stimulus (reinforcer), we would expect evolution of the long-term adaptive control of their overall sensitivity to adequate inputs. Such stable sensitivity would be the neurobiological basis of approach and avoidance personality traits.

Determining the appropriate neurobiological measure for the sensitivity of a highly evolved approach or avoidance system is not simple. These systems have hierarchically organized neural levels with processing ranging from “quick and dirty” to “slow and sophisticated” for both perception (LeDoux, 1994) and action (Graeff, 1994, 2010). Sensitivity to input determines which level of the system is activated and so sensitivity cannot reside in any one of the modules within the system (McNaughton and Corr, 2004). The source of any sensitivity must, therefore, be identified independently—in essence requiring at least a preliminary surface level description of traits.

Existing theories of personality provide a number of competing surface level, lexically-derived, systems with trait measures that relate to approach and avoidance either indirectly via constructs such as Extraversion and Neuroticism (Eysenck, 1957) or directly via constructs such as Harm Avoidance (Cloninger et al., 1993). Each system is stable, with links to mental disorder (Strelau and Zawadzki, 2011; Gomez et al., 2012; Mullins-Sweatt and Lengel, 2012; Trull, 2012) and brain structure (Gardini et al., 2009; DeYoung et al., 2010). But even when starting with approach and avoidance as primary constructs, they are derived “top-down” from pools of lexically-chosen questionnaire items (Carver and White, 1994; Elliot and Thrash, 2010) not from biological anchors. They also depend on factor analysis, which determines the number of dimensions, but not location of trait axes of the personality “space” that items occupy (Lykken, 1971; Corr and McNaughton, 2008). It is little more than an act of faith to believe that the causal structure of personality is isomorphic with its lexical factor structure. So, even if we knew for certain that there were only two dimensions within a particular measured personality space, one questionnaire system could have a single simple trait anxiety dimension (orthogonal to, say, impulsiveness) that was a combination of neuroticism and introversion in another (Gray, 1970)—the two systems differing only on which items from an original pool were used to create scales. Factor analytically derived trait measures can also easily meet the criterion of having “simple structure” (in the sense that a set of items loads highly on only one factor so factors can be clearly identified by unique item loadings) while implying improbable causation (Lykken, 1971). Further, not only is there no reason to suppose that biologically accurate scales should have simple structure but also current scale systems, even though designed to have this, often do not (DeYoung, 2006, 2010).

The plethora of competing trait scales can to some extent be encompassed by just five major trait dimensions that include both normal people and those with psychiatric disorders (Markon et al., 2005; Revelle et al., 2011; Krueger et al., 2012). However, the traits of the competing systems have complex relations to these five large scale dimensions and it is open to question whether there are five fundamental dimensions or whether these are complex facets riding on two or even just one major dimension of personality (Markon et al., 2005; DeYoung et al., 2007; Rushton and Irwing, 2009). These large scale dimensions have “facets” that potentially represent the true underlying sources of personality; and different “approaches differ substantially in the number and nature of the facets they propose, indicating that further conceptual and empirical work is needed to achieve a consensual specification of the Big Five factors at lower levels of abstraction. [Further], given that the Big Five were derived initially from analyses of the personality lexicon, one might wonder whether they merely represent linguistic artifacts” (John et al., 2008, p 141). With no “bottom up” neural anchor to definitely locate the correct rotation of any true biological trait/facet axis, there is no unequivocal way to unify the various systems currently in use.

A related problem, on which we focus below, is that the bulk of personality research has required statistical independence (orthogonality) of the extracted factors. To do otherwise would greatly increase the already large number of alternative trait solutions for any particular item space. However, as we will see, there is good reason to see surface level behavior as being determined interactively even if the biological control of the underlying sensitivities is independent. Likewise, even if the control of factors is neurally independent, when one, e.g., neuroticism, is a risk factor for another, e.g., anxiety (Andrews et al., 1990), then they will become statistically linked in the population as a result.

The solution for approach/avoidance traits is to anchor their factor spaces to measures derived from existing neural state theory. Figure 1 is derived from one particular detailed neuropsychological theory (Gray and Smith, 1969; Gray, 1982; Gray and McNaughton, 2000; McNaughton and Corr, 2004; Corr and McNaughton, 2012) but its system level description captures issues that must be taken into account by any approach/avoidance account of personality. Adequate stimuli (reinforcers) must first be valued and, importantly, negative stimuli (e.g., losses) have a higher exchange rate that positive ones (e.g., gains); that is, people usually show loss aversion (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Overall relation of approach (BAS), avoidance (FFFS = fight, freeze, flee), and conflict (BIS = behavioral inhibition) systems—an updated model. The inputs to the system are classified in terms of the delivery (+) or omission (−) of primary positive reinforcers (PosR) or primary negative reinforcers (NegR) or conditional stimuli (CS) or innate stimuli (IS) that predict such primary events. The BIS is activated when it detects approach-avoidance conflict—suppressing prepotent responses and eliciting risk assessment and displacement behaviors. The systems interact in a variety of ways to generate behavior, see text. The shaded areas are all points at which traits appear to operate. Figure and legend modified from Gray and McNaughton (2000) and Corr and McNaughton (2012).

Any specific positive or negative reinforcer can produce approach or avoidance depending on its contingency (presentation or omission) with responding. For any given reinforcer, the motivational sensitivity of approach activation is different from avoidance activation; and these are separate from the distinct valuation sensitivities of gain to loss (Hall et al., 2011). The strength of response output for any given level of approach activation also depends on distance from the goal (not shown in Figure 1) and does so to a lesser extent than does avoidance (Miller, 1944).

Even with independent trait sensitivities, state approach output depends on the level of avoidance activation, and vice versa: their activations sum to generate arousal, while subtracting to determine choice—giving rise to phenomena such as behavioral contrast and peak shift (Gray and Smith, 1969). As a result, when approach and avoidance are strongly and equally activated, arousal is high but the probability of both approach and avoidance is low; in addition, the approach-avoidance conflict is detected by a third system (with its own trait sensitivity) that is unlike either pure approach or pure avoidance (withdrawal) in being affected by anxiolytic drugs (Gray, 1977). Both approach and avoidance are then inhibited and replaced by behaviors such as risk assessment (Gray and McNaughton, 2000) and displacement (Hinde, 1998), while arousal and negative bias (risk aversion) are increased. With this plethora of interactions, it will be difficult to extract true approach and avoidance traits from the surface structure of behavior—especially if orthogonal factors such as gain and approach have been conflated in a single construct such as reward (Corr and McNaughton, 2012).

However, neural measures should be able to target the internal representations of the specific elements depicted in Figure 1; challenge their response with appropriate combinations of stimuli; and so dissect out the specific contribution of a particular trait sensitivity. These neural measures can then be used to anchor traits within the conventional factor spaces and determine non-orthogonalities. Paradoxically, we are closest to achieving this with the most embedded neural construct: sensitivity to conflict. The argument for the use of primarily neural rather than questionnaire measures of approach and avoidance sensitivities has been made in detail previously—coupled with arguments for combining bottom up neural analysis with top down behavioral analysis (Smillie, 2008a,b; DeYoung, 2010). Here, we would emphasize, in addition, that the choice of neural measures should be strongly theoretically based and behaviorally and or pharmacologically validated in relation to the theory. Otherwise a plethora of questionnaires becomes a plethora of putative neural measures.

The conflict system is defined by the action of anxiolytic drugs (Gray, 1977) acting on receptors for endogenous compounds (Guidotti et al., 1978; Polc, 1995) that could mediate the system's trait sensitivity. Anxiolytic action is specifically linked to hippocampal rhythmicity in rodents (Woodnorth and McNaughton, 2002; McNaughton et al., 2006, 2007) and this has led to development of a human scalp EEG homolog (McNaughton et al., 2013) that provides a biomarker for conflict sensitivity in humans. This biomarker appears to be linked to the shared variance in neuroticism and trait anxiety much more than either of their unique variances (Neo et al., 2011).

In summary, we believe that approach and avoidance systems have evolved in such a way that global control of sensitivities to gain, loss, approach, avoidance and conflict can underlie human personality traits (Corr and McNaughton, 2012). While each of these long-term sensitivities is likely to be controlled independently, under normal ecological circumstances short-term behavioral output will be the result of complex interactions between them (Figure 1). However, the combination of appropriate neural measures with designs that dissect these interactions should provide the means to anchor trait measures in the data spaces that personality research has already shown have long term stability and important behavioral, and particularly psychiatric, consequences. Critically, the factor analysis of lexically-derived variables at the surface level of description cannot be assumed to reflect the deeper construct processes that are giving rise to surface descriptions; and no adjustment of the basic factor analysis method can avoid the problem created when there is no neural anchor to ensure inclusion of correct items and unique rotational solution after initial factoring.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Andrews, G., Stewart, G., Morris-Yates, A., Holt, P., and Henderson, S. (1990). Evidence for a general neurotic syndrome. Br. J. Psychiatry 157, 6–12. doi: 10.1192/bjp.157.1.6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Carver, C. S., and White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: the BIS/BAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67, 319–333. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319

CrossRef Full Text

Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M., and Przybecky, T. R. (1993). A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 50, 975–990. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820240059008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Corr, P. J. (2013). Approach and avoidance behavior: multiple systems and their interactions. Emot. Rev. 5, 286–291. doi: 10.1177/1754073913477507

CrossRef Full Text

Corr, P. J., and McNaughton, N. (2008). “Reinforcement sensitivity theory and personality,” in The Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory of Personality, ed P. J. Corr (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 155–187.

Corr, P. J., and McNaughton, N. (2012). Neuroscience and approach/avoidance personality traits: a two stage (valuation-motivation) approach. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 2339–2354. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.09.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

DeYoung, C. G. (2006). Higher-order factors of the Big Five in a multi-informant sample. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 91, 1138–1151. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.6.1138

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

DeYoung, C. G. (2010). Personality neuroscience and the biology of traits. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 4, 1165–1180. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00327.x

CrossRef Full Text

DeYoung, C. G., Hirsh, J. B., Shane, M. S., Papademetris, X., Rajeevan, N., and Gray, J. R. (2010). Testing predictions from personality neuroscience. Psychol. Sci. 21, 820–828. doi: 10.1177/0956797610370159

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., and Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between facets and domains: ten aspects of the big five. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 93, 880–896. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.880

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Elliot, A. J., and Thrash, T. M. (2010). Approach and avoidance temperament as basic dimensions of personality. J. Pers. 78, 865–906. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00636.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eysenck, H. J. (1957). The Dynamics of Anxiety and Hysteria. New York, NY: Preger.

Gardini, S., Cloninger, C. R., and Venneri, A. (2009). Individual differences in personality traits reflect structural variance in specific brain regions. Brain Res. Bull. 79, 265–270. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2009.03.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gomez, R., Woodworth, R., Waugh, M., and Corr, P. J. (2012). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms in an adult sample: associations with Cloninger's temperament and character dimensions. Pers. Individ. Dif. 52, 290–294. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.10.015

CrossRef Full Text

Graeff, F. G. (1994). Neuroanatomy and neurotransmitter regulation of defensive behaviors and related emotions in mammals. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. 27, 811–829.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Graeff, F. G. (2010). “Human fear and anxiety,” in Encyclopedia of Behavioral Neuroscience, eds G. F. Koob, M. Le Moal, and R. F. Thompson (Oxford: Academic Press), 70–75.

Gray, J. A. (1970). The psychophysiological basis of introversion- extraversion. Behav. Res. Ther. 8, 249–266. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(70)90069-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gray, J. A. (1975). Elements of a Two-Process Theory of Learning. London: Academic Press.

Gray, J. A. (1977). “Drug effects on fear and frustration: Possible limbic site of action of minor tranquilizers,” in Handbook of Psychopharmacology, Vol. 8: Drugs, Neurotransmitters and Behaviour, eds L. L. Iversen, S. D. Iversen, and S. H. Snyder (New York, NY: Plenum Press), 433–529.

Gray, J. A. (1982). The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry in to the Functions of the Septo-Hippocampal System. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Gray, J. A., and McNaughton, N. (2000). The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry into the Functions of the Septo-Hippocampal System, 2nd Edn. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Gray, J. A., and Smith, P. T. (1969). “An arousal-decision model for partial reinforcement and discrimination learning,” in Animal Discrimination Learning, eds R. Gilbert, and N. S. Sutherland (London: Academic Press), 243–272.

Guidotti, A., Toffano, G., and Costa, E. (1978). An endogenous protein modulates the affinity of GABA and benzodiazepine receptors in rat brain. Nature 275, 553–555. doi: 10.1038/275553a0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hall, P. J., Chong, W., McNaughton, N., and Corr, P. J. (2011). A economic perspective on the reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality. Pers. Individ. Dif. 51, 242–247. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.06.023

CrossRef Full Text

Hebb, D. O. (1949). The Organization of Behavior: A Neuropsychological Theory. New York, NY: Wiley-Interscience.

Hinde, R. A. (1998). Animal Behaviour. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., and Soto, C. J. (2008). “Paradigm shift to the integrative Big Five trait taxonomy: history: measurement, and conceptual issues,” in Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research, eds O. P. John, R. W. Robins, and L. A. Pervin (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 114–158.

Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 47, 263–291. doi: 10.2307/1914185

CrossRef Full Text

Krebs, J. R., Stephens, D. W., and Sutherland, W. J. (1983). “Perspectives in optimal foraging,” in Perspectives in Ornithology, eds G. A. Clark and A. H. Brush (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 165–221.

Krueger, R. F., Derringer, J., Markon, K. E., Watson, D., and Skodol, A. E. (2012). Initial construction of a maladaptive personality trait model and inventory for DSM-5. Psychol. Med. 42, 1879–1890. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711002674

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

LeDoux, J. E. (1994). Emotion, memory and the brain. Sci. Am. 270, 50–59. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0694-50

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lykken, D. T. (1971). Multiple factor analysis and personality research. J. Res. Pers. 5, 161–170.

Markon, K. E., Krueger, R. F., and Watson, D. (2005). Delineating the structure of normal and abnormal personality: an integrative hierarchical approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 88, 139–157. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.1.139

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McNaughton, N., and Corr, P. J. (2004). A two-dimensional neuropsychology of defense: fear/anxiety and defensive distance. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 28, 285–305. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McNaughton, N., Kocsis, B., and Hajós, M. (2007). Elicited hippocampal theta rhythm: a screen for anxiolytic and pro-cognitive drugs through changes in hippocampal function? Behav. Pharmacol. 18, 329–346. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e3282ee82e3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McNaughton, N., Ruan, M., and Woodnorth, M. A. (2006). Restoring theta-like rhythmicity in rats restores initial learning in the Morris water maze. Hippocampus 16, 1102–1110. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20235

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McNaughton, N., Swart, C., Neo, P. S. H., Bates, V., and Glue, P. (2013). Anti-anxiety drugs reduce conflict-specific “theta” - a possible human anxiety-specific biomarker. J. Affect. Disord. 148, 104–111. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.11.057

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Miller, N. E. (1944). “Experimental studies of conflict,” in Personality and the Behavioural Disorders, ed J. M. Hunt (New York, NY: Ronald Press), 431–465.

Mullins-Sweatt, S. N., and Lengel, G. J. (2012). Clinical utility of the five-factor model of personality disorder. J. Pers. 80, 1615–1639. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2012.00774.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Neo, P., Thurlow, J. K., and McNaughton, N. (2011). Stopping, goal-conflict, trait anxiety and frontal rhythmic power in the stop-signal task. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 11, 485–493. doi: 10.3758/s13415-011-0046-x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Polc, P. (1995). Involvement of endogenous benzodiazepine receptor ligands in brain disorders: therapeutic potential for benzodiazepine antagonists. Med. Hypotheses 44, 439–446. doi: 10.1016/0306-9877(95)90504-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Revelle, W., Wilt, J., and Condon, D. (2011). “Individual differences and differential psychology: a brief history and prospect,” in Handbook of Individual Differences, eds T. Chamorro- Premuzic, A. Furnham, and S. Von Stumm (Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell), 3–38.

Rolls, E. T. (2000). Précis of the brain and emotion. Behav. Brain Sci. 23, 177–191. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00002429

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rushton, J. P., and Irwing, P. (2009). A general factor of personality in 16 sets of the big five, the Guilford-Zimmerman temperament survey, the California psychology inventory, and the temperament and character inventory. Pers. Individ. Dif. 47, 558–564. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.05.009

CrossRef Full Text

Smillie, L. D. (2008a). The conceptualisation, measurement and scope of reinforcement sensitivity in the context of a neuroscience of personality. Eur. J. Pers. 22, 411–425. doi: 10.1002/per.687

CrossRef Full Text

Smillie, L. D. (2008b). What is reinforcement sensitivity? Neuroscience paradigms for approach-avoidance process theories of personality. Eur. J. Pers. 22, 359–384. doi: 10.1002/per.674

CrossRef Full Text

Strelau, J., and Zawadzki, B. (2011). Fearfulness and anxiety in research on temperament: temperamental traits are related to anxiety disorders. Pers. Individ. Dif. 50, 907–915. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.07.008

CrossRef Full Text

Trull, T. J. (2012). The Five-Factor model of personality disorder and DSM-5. J. Pers. 80, 1697–1720. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2012.00771.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Woodnorth, M. A., and McNaughton, N. (2002). Similar effects of medial supramammillary or systemic injection of chlordiazepoxide on both theta frequency and fixed-interval responding. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2, 76–83. doi: 10.3758/CABN.2.1.76

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: approach, avoidance, conflict, personality, biomarkers, anchoring

Citation: McNaughton N and Corr PJ (2014) Approach, avoidance, and their conflict: the problem of anchoring. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:124. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00124

Received: 03 April 2014; Accepted: 16 June 2014;
Published online: 03 July 2014.

Edited by:

Daniela Laricchiuta, IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation, Italy

Reviewed by:

Luke D. Smillie, The University of Melbourne, Australia

Copyright © 2014 McNaughton and Corr. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: nmcn@psy.otago.ac.nz

Download