Meta-analysis and systematic review of the literature characterizing auditory mismatch negativity in individuals with autism

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.01.008Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Meta-analyses assessed the auditory MMN response in ASD across 22 publications.

  • Only studies that counterbalanced stimuli showed a trend toward weaker MMN in ASD.

  • Weaker MMN response in ASD was most evident in young children and in nonspeech-based paradigms.

  • MMN response in adolescents and those with below-average verbal IQ is understudied.

  • Targeted study of within-group variability may reveal MMN response anomalies better than between-group analyses.

Abstract

A number of past studies have used mismatch negativity (MMN) to identify auditory processing deficits in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Our meta-analysis compared MMN responses for individuals with ASD and typically developing controls (TD). We analyzed 67 experiments across 22 publications that employed passive, auditory-based MMN paradigms with ASD and TD participants. Most studies lacked design characteristics that would lead to an accurate description of the MMN. Variability between experiments measuring MMN amplitude was smaller when limited to studies that counterbalanced stimuli. Reduced MMN amplitude was found among young children with ASD compared to controls and in experiments that used nonspeech sounds. Still, few studies included adolescents or those with below-average verbal IQ. Most studies suffered from small sample sizes, and aggregating these data did not reveal significant group differences. This analysis points to a need for research focused specifically on understudied ASD samples using carefully designed MMN experiments. Study of individual differences in MMN may provide further insights into distinct subgroups within the heterogeneous ASD population.

Introduction

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is characterized by impairments in social communication and interaction as well as by the presence of repetitive and restricted behaviors or interests, including atypical responses to sensory stimuli like sounds (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Language impairments, while not core symptoms in ASD, often co-occur (Tager-Flusberg et al., 2005). Atypical responses to auditory stimuli and difficulty in learning spoken language are linked to disruptions of auditory filtering, acoustic feature discrimination, sound source identification, and auditory working memory (Anderson & Kraus, 2010; Foss-Feig et al., 2012; Näätänen et al., 2012; O’Connor, 2012). Given that these processes are vital components of auditory processing, several researchers have hypothesized that in ASD, there is a common disruption in neural networks that govern basic auditory processing (Bomba and Pang, 2004; Marco et al., 2011). To pinpoint the underlying bases of atypical auditory processing in brain-based disorders, researchers often turn to measures like electroencephalography (EEG) and magnetoencephalography (MEG). These neuroelectric imaging approaches have the temporal resolution necessary to track neural activity associated with specific auditory events, thereby providing a window into auditory processing not afforded by other noninvasive neural measures1. Here, a meta-analysis was undertaken to determine the extent to which neural response that reflect acoustic feature discrimination and auditory working memory in early auditory processing differs in ASD relative to typical development (TD).

We focused on one common approach that can capture such features of early auditory processing: the mismatch negativity (MMN) paradigm (Näätänen et al., 2012; Näätänen et al., 2007). The MMN measures an individual’s ability to detect changes in auditory patterns by presenting a regularly occurring, “standard” pattern that is interrupted at random with rare, “deviant” stimuli (Näätänen et al., 1978). Deviant stimuli usually differ perceptually from standards on a single acoustic feature, such as intensity, pitch, or phoneme. Typically, the unexpected, rare sounds elicit neural responses not present when that same sound is expected. The size of those neural responses indexes the degree to which a listener has built up a memory trace of an ongoing auditory pattern and detected a deviation from that trace (Näätänen et al., 2007). It has been argued that this neural response is driven by NMDA receptor activity in the bilateral auditory and frontal cortices (Näätänen et al., 2012). MMN components can be well detected on the scalp’s frontal-central midline using EEG and can be quantified as a negative component that occurs 100 to 250 ms following a deviant stimulus onset (Haesen et al., 2011). In source space, the mismatch field arises from frontal and supratemporal generators during a similar time window (Giard et al., 1990; Novak et al., 1990).

The MMN component itself is calculated from the difference between the response evoked by the same event when it is a standard and when it is a deviant. By directly comparing responses to identical stimuli when they are expected versus when they are deviants, the MMN in a baseline-corrected measure, revealing neural activity driven by hearing an unexpected event. Response latency of the MMN is determined based on the timing of the negative peak in the difference waveform. Response amplitude can be computed by taking the average response in a window centered on this negative peak. However, the analysis window used to determine MMN amplitude and latency varies across studies (e.g., it can be based on each individual subject’s waveform, based on the average waveform of each subject group, or based on the average from all participants). Both MMN amplitude and latency metrics signify rapid discrimination that is driven by both bottom-up automatic and top-down attentive processes at early stages of cortical processing (Näätänen et al., 2012; Roberts et al., 2011).

The MMN response can be elicited both during active tasks, where the subject makes an overt response upon detecting the deviant stimulus, and in settings when the subject listens passively, with no overt response required. As such, the MMN is one of the few established neural measures of auditory processing that does not require a high degree of instruction, overt attention, or active participation from the research participant (Bishop, 2007; Näätänen et al., 2012). This makes the MMN attractive to researchers studying individuals with ASD, whose verbal and cognitive abilities range across a wide spectrum; for paradigms measured in an active setting that require subjects to follow instructions, pay attention to stimuli, or perform a behavioral task, variations in subjects’ abilities undoubtedly affect the measured response. To make meaningful cross-group comparisons from experiments that include subjects with and without verbal and cognitive deficits, it is important to use a paradigm for which performance is not significantly influenced by attention or other higher-level cognitive processes.

Many passive MMN experiments have been conducted on the ASD population, but there is no consensus across studies as to whether or not people with ASD exhibit a different MMN response to auditory deviants. Some publications have reported heightened and/or earlier MMN responses to acoustic deviants in ASD, suggesting greater auditory sensitivity to changes in acoustic stimuli (Gomot et al., 2011; Lepistö et al., 2007). Other publications have reported suppressed and/or delayed MMN responses to acoustic deviants in ASD, indicating a weaker sensitivity (Andersson et al., 2013; Yu et al., 2015). Still others have reported mixed results, such that some deviant stimuli elicit group differences while others do not (Lepistö et al., 2005; Lepistö et al., 2008). While several past reviews have described these conflicting findings (Foss-Feig et al., 2012; Haesen et al., 2011; Kujala et al., 2013; Mcfadden and Rojas, 2013; Näätänen and Kujala, 2011; O’Connor, 2012; Orekhova and Stroganova, 2014), none have critically evaluated which factors may account for similarities and discrepancies across studies.

This lack of consensus prompted us to conduct a meta-analysis exploring whether there are methodological or stimulus differences that explain apparent inconsistencies across studies. We compared MMN response amplitude and MMN response latency between individuals with ASD and age-matched TD controls. We compiled the results from all experiments that met our inclusion criteria into a comprehensive statistical framework, treating each experiment or statistic as a single data point in our analysis. Given the complexities of collecting EEG and MEG data from individuals with ASD, sample sizes in individual studies tended to be fairly small and lacked strong power on their own. Our meta-analysis synthesized results across studies, thereby increasing the statistical power when testing for group differences.

We began by analyzing all published experiments that measured group differences between ASD and TD participants using either MMN amplitude or latency in a passive, auditory-based MMN paradigm. We then narrowed our analysis to include only those experiments that controlled for general variation in event-related potential or event-related field (ERP/ERF) responses to different stimulus tokens. Specifically, we only included studies in which the MMN was calculated by comparing responses to identical stimuli presented in two different contexts – one in which they were unexpected deviants and the other in which they were expected standards. Without counterbalancing stimuli in this way, any difference in signal morphology between the response to deviants and standards might be due to differences in the unrelated neural responses to the specific stimuli presented, such as a loud sound producing a larger ERP/ERF than a soft sound (Duncan et al., 2009; Kujala, 2007). We followed up with analyses examining how stimulus characteristics (speech versus nonspeech sounds) impacted group-difference effect size and whether participant characteristics (age and verbal reasoning) influenced the findings.

Section snippets

Literature search and screening criteria

Our meta-analysis and systematic review followed PRISMA guidelines (Moher et al., 2009). We began with a comprehensive literature search to identify publications reporting experiments that measured auditory MMN components in individuals with ASD, using the key terms “MMN,” “MMF”, “mismatch negativity,” “mismatch field,” “oddball,” “autism,” and “ASD” on PubMed, ScienceDirect, and Google Scholar. We used the following inclusion criteria:

  • (1)

    The publication had to include an experiment that used a

Full meta-analysis

Our systematic literature review identified a total of 38 publications published between 1980 and 2017 that had at least one experiment that measured EEG or MEG using a passive auditory mismatch negativity paradigm (Table 1a, Table 1b). Twenty-two of these publications included experiments that met all our criteria, yielding a total of 67 separate experiments; each of these experiments explored group differences between ASD and TD listeners based on MMN amplitude and/or latency (Table 1a). From

Summary

Although a fair number of past publications have investigated the MMN in ASD, individual studies have come to different conclusions. We undertook our meta-analysis to try to resolve these differences. Instead, our analysis revealed several important limitations of these studies. First, the majority of published studies included fewer than 20 participants per group. Such small sample sizes lead to problems when aggregating the data on group differences in MMN latency, since our analysis suggests

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health [P50 DC013027], the National Science Foundation, Arlington, VA [SMA-0835976], and the Autism Speaks Foundation, Princeton, NJ [10085].

References (112)

  • R. DerSimonian et al.

    Meta-analysis in clinical trials

    Control Clin. Trials

    (1986)
  • C.C. Duncan et al.

    Event-related potentials in clinical research: guidelines for eliciting, recording, and quantifying mismatch negativity, P300, and N400

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (2009)
  • Y.T. Fan et al.

    Atypical mismatch negativity in response to emotional voices in people with autism spectrum conditions

    PLoS ONE

    (2014)
  • R. Ferri et al.

    The mismatch negativity and the P3a components of the auditory event-related potentials in autistic low-functioning subjects

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (2003)
  • J.H. Foss-Feig et al.

    Processing of non-speech auditory stimuli in individuals with autism spectrum disorders: the impact of stimulus characteristics

    Int. Rev. Res. Dev. Disabil.

    (2012)
  • B. Haesen et al.

    A review of behavioural and electrophysiological studies on auditory processing and speech perception in autism spectrum disorders

    Res. Autism Spectr. Disorders

    (2011)
  • E. Jansson-Verkasalo et al.

    Deficient auditory processing in children with Asperger syndrome, as indexed by event-related potentials

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (2003)
  • K. Kasai et al.

    Delayed automatic detection of change in speech sounds in adults with autism: a magnetoencephalographic study

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (2005)
  • C. Kemner et al.

    Auditory event-related brain potentials in autistic children and three different control groups

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (1995)
  • T. Kujala et al.

    Atypical pattern of discriminating sound features in adults with Asperger syndrome as reflected by the mismatch negativity

    Biol. Psychol.

    (2007)
  • T. Kujala et al.

    Speech-feature discrimination in children with Asperger syndrome as determined with the multi-feature mismatch negativity paradigm

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (2010)
  • T. Kujala et al.

    The neural basis of aberrant speech and audition in autism spectrum disorders

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2013)
  • T. Kujala et al.

    Neurophysiological evidence for cortical discrimination impairment of prosody in Asperger syndrome

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (2005)
  • T. Lepistö et al.

    Auditory stream segregation in children with Asperger syndrome

    Biol. Psychol.

    (2009)
  • T. Lepistö et al.

    The perception of invariant speech features in children with autism

    Biol. Psychol.

    (2008)
  • T. Lepistö et al.

    Auditory cortical change detection in adults with Asperger syndrome

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (2007)
  • T. Lepistö et al.

    Auditory perception and attention as reflected by the brain event-related potentials in children with Asperger syndrome

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (2006)
  • T. Lepistö et al.

    The discrimination of and orienting to speech and nonspeech sounds in children with autism

    Brain Res.

    (2005)
  • A. Ludlow et al.

    Auditory processing and sensory behaviours in children with autism spectrum disorders as revealed by mismatch negativity

    Brain Cognit.

    (2014)
  • R. Näätänen et al.

    Early selective-attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted

    Acta Psychol.

    (1978)
  • R. Näätänen et al.

    The mismatch negativity (MMN) – a unique window to disturbed central auditory processing in ageing and different clinical conditions

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (2012)
  • R. Näätänen et al.

    The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: a review

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (2007)
  • R. Näätänen et al.

    The mismatch negativity and its magnetic equivalent: an index of language impairment or more general cognitive decline in autism?

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2011)
  • B. Novick et al.

    An electrophysiologic indication of auditory processing defects in autism

    Psychiatry Res.

    (1980)
  • K. O’Connor

    Auditory processing in autism spectrum disorder: a review

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2012)
  • R.D. Oades et al.

    Event-related potentials in autistic and healthy children on an auditory choice reaction time task

    Int. J. Psychophysiol.

    (1988)
  • S. Pakarinen et al.

    Measurement of extensive auditory discrimination profiles using the mismatch negativity (MMN) of the auditory event-related potential (ERP)

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (2007)
  • T.P.L. Roberts et al.

    Auditory magnetic mismatch field latency: a biomarker for language impairment in autism

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2011)
  • S. Seri et al.

    Autism in tuberous sclerosis: evoked potential evidence for a deficit in auditory sensory processing

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1999)
  • V.L. Shafer et al.

    The development of English vowel perception in monolingual and bilingual infants: neurophysiological correlates

    J. Phon.

    (2011)
  • F. Tecchio et al.

    Auditory sensory processing in autism: a magnetoencephalographic study

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2003)
  • L. Trainor et al.

    Changes in auditory cortex and the development of mismatch negativity between 2 and 6 months of age

    Int. J. Psychophysiol.

    (2003)
  • M.M. Abdeltawwab et al.

    Automatic pre-attentive auditory responses: MMN to tone burst frequency changes in autistic school-age children

    J. Int. Adv. Otol.

    (2015)
  • American Psychiatric Association

    Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5®)

    (2013)
  • A. Ben-Sasson et al.

    A meta-analysis of sensory modulation symptoms in individuals with autism spectrum disorders

    J. Autism Dev. Disord.

    (2009)
  • D.V.M. Bishop

    Using mismatch negativity to study central auditory processing in developmental language and literacy impairments: where are we, and where should we be going?

    Psychol. Bull.

    (2007)
  • D.V.M. Bishop

    Test for Reception of Grammar—Electronic

    (2005)
  • D.V.M. Bishop et al.

    Measurement of mismatch negativity in individuals: a study using single-trial analysis

    Psychophysiology

    (2010)
  • A. Bonnel et al.

    Enhanced pitch sensitivity in individuals with autism: a signal detection analysis

    J. Cognit. Neurosci.

    (2003)
  • R. Ceponiene et al.

    Speech-sound-selective auditory impairment in children with autism: they can perceive but do not attend

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (2003)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text