Introduction

Boat and ship traffic worldwide are on the rise (United Nations Conference on Trade and Development—UNCTAD 2018), particularly in coastal areas (Marine Traffic 2013–2019). In addition to commercial ship traffic, the recreational use of yachts, motorboats and personal watercrafts (e.g. jet skis) worldwide has increased strongly in the last decades. This has manifold impacts on the coastal environment (Davenport and Davenport 2006), especially since many recreational users also enter marine reserves (Gonson et al. 2016). The increase in ship and boat traffic is likely to have an impact on wildlife, especially marine mammals (Lusseau 2005), seabirds (Ronconi and St. Clair 2002; Velando and Munilla 2011) and sea ducks (Schwemmer et al. 2011). It has, for example, been shown that species such as loons (Gavia ssp.) and common scoters (Melanitta nigra) tend to avoid areas with high ship traffic (Bellebaum et al. 2006; Kaiser et al. 2006; Schwemmer et al. 2011).

From a theoretical point of view, wildlife might perceive approaching boats and ships in the same way as intruding predators (Frid and Dill 2002) and therefore respond accordingly. By displaying alert behaviour, flocks of birds signal an approaching predator upfront that it has been detected, which may discourage the predator from attacking (Jacobsen and Morten 1992; Caro 2005). By doing so, the birds can remain in the vicinity of their previous foraging site. Optimal foraging theory (cf. Stephens and Krebs 1986) also predicts that if the foraging habitat is of high quality, prey species will initiate their escape response later and show a lower displacement distance (i.e. the distance between original location and location after disturbance) than if pre-disturbance foraging habitat is of low quality (Ydenberg and Dill 1986). Finally, independent of foraging habitat, birds may get habituated to regularly used shipping routes. Hence, flocks of common eider (Somateria mollisima) and long-tailed ducks (Clangula hyemalis) showed shorter flight initiation distances (i.e. the distance between ship and birds at which birds start to fly off or dive away) in shipping lanes than outside of shipping lanes in offshore areas of the North and Baltic Sea during winter (Schwemmer et al. 2011). Thus, habituation might save energy, since flight costs in sea ducks are exceptionally high. For instance, Pelletier et al. (2008) showed that 10 min/day in flight amounts to 4–5% of their daily energy budget and that flight times are minimized to save energy. However, unlike commercial ship traffic, recreational boats and fishing vessels are unpredictable in speed and course and therefore pose a higher disturbance potential for birds (Schwemmer et al. 2011). Furthermore, recreational activities take place mostly in coastal waters, and thus overlap spatially with the habitat of sea ducks, which increases the potential for disturbance. This was demonstrated in a study from Southwestern Greenland where persistent traffic of small, fast moving, open boats had cumulative disturbance effects on wintering common eiders, with significantly decreased time for feeding (up to a 60% decrease) while locomotion activity tripled (Merkel et al. 2009).

For sea ducks, the moulting period, initiated after egg laying for males and during and after the chick-rearing period in females is especially energy demanding. In Norwegian waters, common eider males moult during July and August, whereas for females the moulting period is August and September. Moulting sea ducks lose their flight feathers simultaneously which restrict their diving and flight abilities, and increase their energy expenditure, which for the common eider is 9% higher during moult compared to the post-moult period (Guillemette et al. 2007). Due to their restricted dive capabilities sea ducks therefore moult in shallow areas with high food availability (Fox et al. 2008). A recent study on common eiders indicated carry-over effects of energetic demands and stress (measured as baseline corticosterone in feathers) from the moult period to the subsequent breeding season (Harms et al. 2015). Females with lower feather corticosterone concentrations (and thus less stress during moult) had a higher body mass, arrived earlier in their breeding colonies and showed higher breeding success and survival (Harms et al. 2015). Disturbance effects during moult, especially when these lead to displacement from optimal foraging areas, could therefore have potential carry-over effects on individual fitness and possibly population trajectories. Although the moult period is a particularly sensitive period in the ducks’ annual life cycle, very few assessments of the effects of boat traffic on moulting sea ducks have been performed.

By experimentally approaching flocks of moulting common eiders, this study aimed to improve the understanding of the impact of boat traffic during the moulting period. In particular we assessed the birds’ behavioural reaction in response to decreasing flock-to-boat distance, distinguishing between undisturbed and alert behaviour, swimming away from the boat, flying (in few cases of eiders that had not initiated moult during the study period), escaping with flapping wings and diving. Furthermore, we assumed that flapping/diving away, particularly in a stressful situation as an escape from a boat (perceived as a potential predator), is similarly energetically costly for common eiders as flying (Pelletier et al. 2008). Thus, flight initiation distances during decreasing flock-to-boat distance at which birds initiated energy-demanding escape responses as diving, escaping with flapping wings or flying were measured. Then, to gain better insights into the potential energetic consequences of boat disturbances, we measured the displacement distances (i.e. the distance the birds moved away from their initial position) and the time it took for a flock to regain pre-disturbance behaviour (i.e. an indication for the time loss associated with the disturbance which would have otherwise been used for feeding, resting or social activities). We further assessed the proportion of accessible benthic foraging habitat (indicated by suitable water depths for foraging in a radius of 150 m around the position of the flock) before and after the disturbance. We hypothesized that the proportion of accessible foraging habitat would be higher before compared to after the disturbance. Finally, to improve our understanding of the potential consequences of the disturbance on the foraging abilities, we investigated whether displacement distance and time-to-return to pre-disturbance behaviour were linked to accessible foraging habitat, but also variables such as flock size, wind speed, tide, and time of day. These variables were included since they may influence activity patterns of the ducks: Flight initiation distances have been shown to be positively correlated with flock size (Schwemmer et al. 2011) and wind speed (Laursen et al. 2005), and both factors may consequently also influence displacement distances and time-to-return to pre-disturbance behaviour. Eider ducks typically show a peak in foraging activities during morning and evening hours and also during low tide (Minot 1980; Systad and Bustnes 2001; Merkel et al. 2009). Both time of day and tide may thus affect particularly their time-to-return to pre-disturbance and foraging activities.

Methods

Study site

The study was conducted between the 6th and 24th of August 2013 in shallow coastal areas in central Norway, near the islands Grip (63° 13.20 N, 7° 36.16 E), Smøla (63° 25.00 N, 7° 48.95 E) and Frøya (63° 45.47 N, 8° 17.34 E; see Fig. 1). The area is characterized by few large main islands surrounded by many smaller islands, islets and small skerries and shallow waters, which creates particularly suitable foraging habitats for common eiders. This was confirmed by an aerial count of moulting eiders completed in July and August 2012, based on which we identified possible locations for our experimental approach. During summer (June–August) these areas are popular for recreational boating and both recreational and commercial fishing, and thus hold potential for moulting common eiders to be disturbed.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Overview map of the study area. The inset map shows the location of the study area within Northern Europe (red box). Triangles in the main map indicate the locations of the eider flocks prior to disturbance. The location of the island of Grip is highlighted with a red circle

The breeding populations of common eiders have been declining along the Norwegian Atlantic and North Sea coasts (Anker-Nilssen et al. 2015), and similar trends are visible in Sweden and Finland (Kilpi et al. 2015). Systematic count data for the breeding season from the study area are missing, thus not allowing information on a population trend in this specific area. However, the number of common eiders wintering around the island of Smøla has decreased annually by 3.9% between 1980 and 2013 (estimate based on annual count data, trend estimate calculated following methodology in Žydelis et al. 2006).

Experimental design and data collection

We used two recreational motor boats to approach and experimentally disturb common eider flocks; a Korsö 1100 with a 500 hp engine, and a Skorgenes 350 with a 400 hp engine, both being 11 m long, and with a height of ca. 3 m from sea level to the wheelhouse, and 5 m from sea level to the top of the antennas.

Before approaching a flock, birds were counted and observed from a distance (250–1500 m) for 10 min to determine pre-disturbance behaviour, categorized as foraging, swimming (i.e. transit between areas) and social behaviour/resting such as sleeping or preening.

The following experimental disturbance phase consisted of the boat approaching on a straight course towards the flock with a speed of 11.1 km/h (6 knots). One observer on board continuously recorded the changes of behavioural responses and flock-to-boat distances. Behavioural responses were categorized as: undisturbed behaviour/no response; alert behaviour (raising their head)—attention towards boat and flocking of individuals; swimming away from the boat; escape diving; sprinting away from the boat with flapping wings or flying away (i.e., the few ducks that had not shed their wing feathers yet).

In the post-disturbance phase, the boat stayed as far as possible away from the flock to not influence their behaviour. The distance between the boat and the flock depended on visibility due to weather and/or island topography (visibility range of on average 585 ± 216 m; range 150–1000 m). The behaviour of the ducks was recorded and the timespan from the end of the disturbance phase until the flock regained pre-disturbance behaviour was recorded.

Throughout the entire experiment (i.e. from pre-disturbance to post-disturbance) the position of the boat (latitude and longitude based on GPS), the bearing to the flock and the flock-to boat distance and flock positions were recorded. Flock-to-boat distance and flock positions were determined based on GPS-positions in combination with natural landmarks (islands, shallows) and the observers’ judgement (approximate accuracy 50 m). The water depths at the flocks’ positions were afterwards determined from navigational charts. For pre- and post-disturbance positions of the flocks, we assessed the proportion of accessible foraging habitat, using ArcGIS v. 10.1 (ESRI 2013). Water depth has been shown to be the key indicator for suitable foraging habitat in common eiders (Guillemette et al. 1993; Bustnes and Lønne 1997), although other factors such as size of blue mussels Mytilus edulis, which is the main food source of common eiders, may further shape habitat quality (Varennes et al. 2015). Common eiders typically perform dives to less than 6 m of depth (Guillemette 1998; Larsen and Guillemette 2000; Guillemette et al. 2004) and appear to avoid foraging in waters deeper than 20 m (Bustnes and Lønne 1997). We therefore used 20 m [measured at lowest astronomical tide (Kartverket 2013)] as a threshold to define proportion of accessible foraging habitat within a radius of 150 m around the flocks’ position since common eider flocks typically spread out spatially while foraging. Thus, if the entire benthic area within the 150 m radius around the flocks’ position was ≤ 20 m deep, the accessible foraging habitat was set to 100%, if it was all deeper than 20 m, it was 0%.

Time was included as hour of the day (range from 8 to 18 h). Tidal information was obtained from the Norwegian Mapping Authority (www.kartverket.no) and was categorized into four levels of 2-h duration each: Level 1 represented 1 h before to 1 h after low tide, level 2 1–3 h after low tide, level 3 3–5 h after low tide and level 4 1 h before to 1 h after high tide. Tidal range in the study area and during the study period was on average 1.70 ± 0.28 m (range 1.0–2.52 m). Wind speed data was downloaded from the Norwegian Meteorological Institute (www.met.no) and was coded according to the Beaufort scale (average 2.78 ± 1.13; range 0–5).

We approached a total of 49 flocks, with an average flock size of 51.8 ± 49.7 individuals (range 7–300). The timespan between two separate approaches on flocks in a given area was a minimum of 5 days, and approaches were as such treated as independent events.

Statistics

The behavioural response of common eiders to the approach distance of a boat was modelled using a series of generalized additive mixed models (GAMMs) in the R package mgcv (Wood 2016) with a logit link function. For each behavioural response (as dependent variable; i.e. undisturbed, alert, swimming, diving, escaping with flapping wings and flying), a binomial GAMM with flock-to-boat-distance as explanatory variable and flock ID as random effect was fitted. GAMMs allow the fitting of non-linear responses to predictor variables, which is a major advantage, as animals rarely respond linearly to their environment (Aarts et al. 2008; Barbraud et al. 2011). To allow a non-linear relationship, we fitted flock-to-boat distance as a smooth-term with a cubic spline smoother. The number of knots, which determine the “wiggliness” of the smoothing, was initially set to a maximum of 4 to avoid overfitting, and we used the functions gam.check and compareML (R package itsadug; van Rij et al. 2017) to check whether models with more knots had a better fit, which was not the case. We subsequently extracted the predicted values and standard errors for each of the six GAMMs. Sample sizes for GAMMs were N = 48, since flock-to-boat distance was missing in one case.

To assess which variables affected displacement distances, i.e. the distance that a flock escaped from its original position, a set of candidate linear models was fitted and evaluated using information theoretic approaches (Akaike Information Criterion for small sample sizes; AICc; Burnham and Anderson 2002). The variables considered were wind speed, pre-disturbance habitat, flock size, time of day and tidal category. Each of these five variables was included separately in a univariate model at first. Time of day and flock size were included as log-transformed data since original data were not normally distributed and violated model assumptions. From a biological point of view, the effects of pre-disturbance habitat and flock size were of particular interest so we also constructed additive models which included either pre-disturbance habitat or flock size in either addition or in interaction with a second variable (being wind speed, tide, flock size or time of day), and finally one set of models which included pre-disturbance habitat, flock size and one additional variable (being wind speed, tide or time of day). This led to a combination of 23 models, including the null model (see Supplementary material). For the five best models, we in addition to AICc values also present AICc weights (wi) and adjusted R2 values as an indicator of variance explained by the model. We present parameter estimates calculated from model averaging (based on AICc) of all models within ΔAICc < 6, following the suggestions in Symonds and Moussalli (2011), using the R package MuMIn (Barton 2018).

Time until birds returned to pre-disturbance behaviour was analysed using Cox proportional hazard models in the R package survival (Therneau 2015). Since the focus of the study was on the foraging behaviour and influence of the pre-disturbance habitat, only flocks that showed social behaviour/resting or foraging behaviour, but not those that were swimming between areas were included (N = 44). Since swimming flocks were changing their habitat, an inclusion into the analysis did not seem meaningful. Two additional flocks were omitted due to missing values, and five flocks did not regain pre-disturbance behaviour but kept swimming (i.e. transit behaviour) and were included in our analyses as right-censored [meaning the model accounted for the fact that the event under study (here: pre-disturbance-behaviour) was not experienced by the last observation]. Models were based on time until flocks returned to pre-disturbance behaviour as the response variable. For the explanatory variables, we adopted the same variables and model structure as previously used for linear models (see above), but additionally added two more variables, namely displacement distance and post-disturbance habitat, resulting in a total of 35 models including the null model (see Supplementary material). As for the linear models, model selection was based on AICc, we present AICc weights and R2-values, and parameter estimates were based on model averaging of all models within ΔAICc < 6.

In addition to the above-detailed main analyses, we assessed flight initiation distances of moulting common eiders, i.e. we identified the mean flock-to-boat distance ± standard deviation at which birds initiated an energy-demanding escape response, namely diving, escaping with flapping wings or flying. In agreement with other studies investigating flight initiation distances in sea ducks (Schwemmer et al. 2011; Fliessbach et al. 2019), we did not consider swimming here, since this is less energetically demanding behaviour (Norberg 1996).

Finally, we compared whether common eider flocks encountered equally accessible foraging habitat pre- and post-disturbance using a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) with a binomial distribution in the R package lme4 (Bates et al. 2011). Foraging habitat was fitted as dependent variable, point in time (pre/post-disturbance) as factor and flock ID as random effect.

All statistical procedures were run in R (version 3.5.1; R Core Team 2018). Model assumptions were validated using the protocols described in Zuur et al. (2009). Significance level was p = 0.05. If not stated otherwise, mean values are presented with standard deviation.

Results

When the boat approached, birds at first showed signs of alertness, which was typically followed by flocks starting to swim away from the boat, a response that was triggered at a maximum flock-to-boat distance of 700 m (Table 1; Fig. 2). Finally, when the boat approached to a maximum of 400 m, ducks initiated to dive (Table 1, Fig. 2). A small proportion of individuals moved away by flapping or flying (Fig. 2). At a minimum flock-to-boat distance of 150 m, all individuals in all flocks showed disturbance behaviour (Table 1). Based on the observation data, the average flight initiation distances, i.e., the distance at which birds showed an energy-demanding evasive response and thus either diving, flapping or flying, were 177 ± 98 m (range 30–400 m).

Table 1 Behaviour of common eider flocks (N = 48) in response to an approaching boat
Fig. 2
figure 2

Behavioural response of moulting common eider flocks (N = 48) to an experimental approach with a boat. Results are based on binomial generalised additive mixed models (GAMMs) run separately for each behaviour in response to flock-to-boat distance

Displacement distance was on average 771 ± 295 m (range 283–1462 m). Flocks encountered less accessible habitat at their post-disturbance position compared to their pre-disturbance position (GLMM: F = 4.86, df = 1, p = 0.028; Fig. 3). Displacement distance was best explained by wind speed as single environmental predictor (see Table 2), but overall wind speed explained a comparatively small proportion of variance (adjusted R2 = 0.086). Tide and time of day were not supported as explanatory variables in models and their inclusion resulted in higher AICc-values than the null model (Supplementary material). Model averaging over the four most-supported models indicated that displacement distances were shorter under high wind speeds, if pre-disturbance habitat accessibility was high and if larger flocks were disturbed (Table 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Box-plot showing the difference in accessible foraging habitat (defined as proportion of benthic area of less than 20 m depth within a 150 m radius of the geographical position of each eider flock) before and after boat disturbance. N = 49

Table 2 Displacement distance of common eider flocks (N = 49) in response to wind speed, flock size, pre-disturbance habitat and time of day
Table 3 Parameter estimates for displacement distance (top), and time-to-return to pre-disturbance behaviour (bottom) for those variables included in top models as shown in Tables 2 and 4, based on model averaging (see “Methods”)

After disturbance, five flocks which were resting or feeding pre-disturbance, did not return to their pre-disturbance behaviour within the observation period (18–60 min past disturbance) but kept swimming and were thus treated as right-censored in our analyses. The reminder of flocks settled faster, and 75% of flocks had returned to pre-disturbance behaviour 10 min after disturbance (Fig. 4). Time-to-return to pre-disturbance behaviour was best explained by the combination of pre-disturbance habitat and wind speed (Table 4). Based on model averaging, the time-to-return to pre-disturbance behaviour was reduced under higher wind speeds and if pre-disturbance habitat accessibility was high but increased with larger flock sizes and time of day (Table 3).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Visualisation of the best model (see Table 3) to explain time-to-return to pre-disturbance behaviour by common eider flocks. The solid line presents the model estimate, dashed lines present the 95% confidence intervals. N = 42 flocks, N = 37 events (5 right-censored)

Table 4 Time of eider flocks to return to pre-disturbance behaviour in response to wind speed, flock size, pre-disturbance and post-disturbance habitat, time of day and displacement distance

Discussion

Our study is the first to investigate the behavioural responses of moulting eider ducks to approaching small boats, a highly relevant topic given the increase in recreational boat traffic in recent years and the overlap in eider duck distribution and moult period with the boating season and coastal area use. We found that moulting common eiders showed flight initiation distances of on average 177 m and that boat disturbance displaced common eider flocks on average by 771 m. 75% of flocks returned to pre-disturbance behaviour within 10 min after the disturbance had ceased, while few flocks remained disturbed for 45 min or longer.

In response to an approaching boat, common eiders showed increasing behavioural responses from undisturbed to alert, swimming away and finally diving away, with few birds escaping with flapping wings or flying. Average flight initiation distances of 177 m are less than what has been found during winter by Schwemmer et al. (2011; average: 208 m) and combined throughout the entire year by Fliessbach et al. (2019; average 277 ± 218 m). Possibly, since moulting common eiders are flightless and energetically more constrained to stay near accessible foraging habitats, they reduce their flight initiation and displacement distances in comparison to the rest of the year. On the other hand, the fact that moulting eiders are flightless and constrained in their escape responses could equally be an argument for the opposite pattern, namely longer flight initiation distances in moulting eiders as a strategy to avoid getting trapped. The studies by Schwemmer et al. (2011) and Fliessbach et al. (2019) were conducted with larger ships and at a higher approach speed (18 km/h and 18.5 km/h, respectively, compared to 11.1 km/h in our study), which may have also affected flight initiation distances. Finally, it is possible that—given our study area is popular for recreational boating in summer—eider ducks were already somewhat habituated to boat traffic compared to the offshore areas and the season where/under which the studies by Fliessbach et al. (2019) and Schwemmer et al. (2011) took place, and this again affected flight initiation distances.

Displacement distances in general have been largely neglected in animal escape studies (Cooper Jr. and Blumstein 2015). A recent study in passerines found a positive correlation between flight initiation distances and displacement distances for heavier species, but not for lighter ones (Tätte et al. 2018). It remains open whether this pattern holds true also for other groups of birds. Overall, Tätte et al. (2018) concluded that displacement distances represent an independent and informative additional measure besides the commonly measured flight initiation distances. Since data on displacement distances for common eiders or related diving ducks are lacking (Livezey et al. 2016), we could not make any comparisons between our data and the literature. Similarly, time-to-return to foraging has not been investigated in common eiders or any sea ducks in a comparable fashion as in our study. Schwemmer et al. (2011) investigated return of flocks to their original position (after 60 or 90 min) and thus exceeded the time period after disturbance we observed flocks. Merkel et al. (2009) studied the feeding activity of common eiders in response to passing boats but did not publish the actual duration during which birds were disturbed.

If pre-disturbance habitat was highly accessible (i.e. shallow), eider duck flocks displaced shorter distances after disturbance but took longer to return to pre-disturbance behaviour. Furthermore, displacement distances were longer under low wind speeds and for smaller flocks, while time-to-return to pre-disturbance behaviour increased with increasing wind speed but decreased with flock size. Finally, eider flocks took longer to return to pre-disturbance behaviour when disturbed later during the day.

Our result that eider flocks which were located in shallow habitats pre-disturbance had shorter displacement distances but took longer to return to pre-disturbance behaviour may best be explained by optimal foraging theory (Ydenberg and Dill 1986). After being displaced from a good foraging site, ducks should ideally return to their previous patch instead of starting to forage at a likely worse site. They may therefore reduce their displacement distances, but remain alert and active (swimming) longer after disturbance. Our data match the results of a study by Laursen et al. (2016) who investigated displacement strategies and regrouping of common eiders in response to hunting in high-quality feeding areas. Common eiders mitigated risks associated with hunting by re-grouping (seeking shelter in larger flocks during autumn or diluting into smaller, less conspicuous flocks during winter) (Laursen et al. 2016). In contrast, displacement to less productive but undisturbed areas further offshore was identified as a suboptimal decision, since common eiders in offshore areas were in lower body conditions than those in coastal, disturbed areas (Laursen et al. 2016).

Wind speed can affect wave height and thus the energy required to swim away from an approaching boat, but also visibility towards the boat—and in combination with wind direction the engine noise perceived by the ducks. All of these points might explain why displacement distances were lower under stronger wind speeds in our study. Similarly, it has been shown that flight initiation distances in waterbirds in general are lower under higher wind speeds (Laursen et al. 2005). Finally, after the disturbance had ceased, birds may remain more alert under higher wind speeds since wind-generated waves make it more difficult to see the predators (or boat). Finally, increased wave action due to wind might hinder resting or foraging behaviour.

Larger flocks may displace a shorter distance upon disturbance since larger flocks offer relatively higher protection to the individual bird (Cresswell and Quinn 2011) than smaller flocks. Thus, birds of a larger flock experience “safe” conditions at a shorter displacement distance, likely because flock alertness will be shared among more individuals (Beauchamp 2010). This may also explain why larger flocks returned to pre-disturbance behaviour sooner after the experimental disturbance. Our results contrast, however, with the findings of larger flocks of sea ducks (including common eiders) showing longer flight initiation distances (Schwemmer et al. 2011), a pattern also commonly found among other bird species (Laursen et al. 2005). Further, also contradicting our results, Merkel et al. (2009) found that—focusing on foraging activity only—eider ducks in smaller groups approached normal feeding activity faster than birds in larger groups. Possibly, while flock alertness is shared among more individuals in larger flocks (Beauchamp 2010), variation in behavioural responses and shyness will also be greater in larger flocks. Schwemmer et al. (2011) described that the flight response of the most sensitive individuals within a flock triggered the escape of the entire flock. This could explain the results in the studies by Schwemmer et al. (2011), Laursen et al. (2005) and Merkel et al. (2009), but does not match with our results of larger flocks showing shorter displacement distances and returning earlier to foraging.

Finally, common eiders returned to pre-disturbance behaviour sooner if they were disturbed earlier during the day. If undisturbed, common eiders tend to forage particularly during the morning and evening hours, with decreased foraging activity during midday and at night (Minot 1980; Systad and Bustnes 2001; Merkel et al. 2009). Most of our experiments were conducted between 8 am and 2 pm. Thus, our results possibly reflect the birds’ increased foraging activity driven by the need for nutrient uptake during morning hours and a reduced need for nutrient uptake later during the day.

We did not find an effect of post-disturbance habitat on time-to-return to pre-disturbance behaviour, and similarly there was no effect of tide on either displacement distance or time-to-return to pre-disturbance behaviour. In particular the first result may be explained by the fact that we did not focus directly on foraging activities of the ducks, but also took resting into account as pre- and post-disturbance behaviour. However, due to their low mobility, we expect moulting eider ducks to stay close to their foraging areas also when they are resting, so the ducks’ behaviour pre-disturbance may not be the only explanation. Merkel et al. (2009) found a significant impact of tide on foraging activity of common eiders, yet this study was conducted in areas with a tidal range of 4.5 m, which is about 2.5 times the tidal range that we observed. Thus, we may not have found an influence of tide on displacement distance or time-to-return to pre-disturbance behaviour due to the comparatively low tidal range in our study area.

We did not take wind direction or other factors into account that may affect the ducks’ perception of the boats’ engine noise. Besides wind direction—in combination with wind speed—this would also involve the transmission of noise under water. Due to the physical properties of water, sound travels much faster and further under water than through air (even under tailwinds) (Everest and Pohlmann 2009). Thus, eider ducks might notice the presence of a boat by acoustical cues under water (especially when diving) rather than through the air. As such, the transmission of the boats’ noise might in fact be relevant when investigating distances at which eider ducks changed from pre-disturbance behaviour to an alert state and subsequently initiating a flight escape. However, we did not investigate the influence of wind and other variables on the initial behavioural response towards the approaching boat, but on displacement distances and time-to-return to pre-disturbance behaviour. At the time of flight initiation, ducks should have been visually aware of the boat, since birds during alert behaviour also showed attention towards the boat. Furthermore, since natural predators of diving ducks (especially birds of prey such as white-tailed sea eagles (Haliaeetus albicilla), American mink (Neovison vison) and in rarer cases sea mammals; Anderson et al. 2011; Jaatinen et al. 2011) attack without previous acoustical warning, we believe the visual cue of an approaching boat was more important to determine displacement distances than the noise of the boat. For return to pre-disturbance behaviour, the underwater noise of a nearby boat may be more relevant—but at that time, the engines of our boats were switched off and thus produced no noise. Overall, we therefore consider the effect of wind direction or the boats’ noise in general to be negligible in the framework of our study.

Another potential weakness of our study approach was that the boat-to-flock distances at which observations took place before the experimental disturbance approach and post disturbance (250–1500 m and 150–1000 m, respectively) overlapped with the maximum flock-to-boat distance at which birds first responded (700 m; see Fig. 2). While a larger distance between the boat and the flocks would have been desirable during the observation times, this was practically not possible due to the local conditions with many skerries and small islands, which partly hindered sight onto flocks. We are nevertheless convinced that our results are robust. First, during observations prior to and post disturbance, the boat was stationary, which should be perceived differently by the ducks than a boat moving at considerable speed towards them. Second, we used the behaviour of the eider ducks to judge if our initial approach to the distance at which we observed them (250–1500 m) had already caused a disturbance. Of the 49 flocks, only 5 were swimming prior to our experimental disturbance approach—the reminder was showing clearly undisturbed behaviour (sleeping, preening or foraging). The five flocks that were swimming did so in a perpendicular direction to the boat, seemingly transiting between foraging areas and not swimming away from the boat. Third, the five flocks that did not return to pre-disturbance behaviour within the observation times (i.e. right-censored in the Cox proportional Hazards Analyses), were 600–1000 m (average 840 ± 150 m) away from our boats during post-disturbance observations, and therefore—except in one case—more than the 700 m limit. We therefore conclude that overall it is very likely that the flock-to-boat distance during pre- and post-disturbance observations did not affect the results of this study.

The proportion of accessible habitat declined from pre- to post-disturbance, i.e. eider ducks were located over deeper water post-disturbance, which likely increased foraging costs or required the flock to move to another site before resuming to forage. This could affect the birds’ body condition. Indeed, Laursen et al. (2016) previously showed that common eiders shot in less suitable habitats were in lower body condition than those in coastal areas with a high abundance of their preferred food, blue mussels. Further, while the majority (75%) of flocks returned to pre-disturbance behaviour within 10 min after the disturbance had ceased, five flocks did not settle into pre-disturbance behaviour within the observation times which exceeded 45 min for three of the concerned flocks. This indicates a substantial disturbance after approach with a relatively small boat at comparatively low speed. Many recreational boats drive faster than we did in our study (e.g. Bellefleur et al. 2009; Merkel et al. 2009), which may cause much larger disturbance and long-lasting effects, especially in areas with high boat traffic. Merkel et al. (2009) found that feeding activity in common eiders decreased significantly if boats approached to within 1 km, and there was a cumulative effect if this occurred more than 1 time per hour. As a result of high disturbance, common eiders compensated by feeding also during midday, at night and at high tide (Merkel et al. 2009). Among the seabird and sea duck species occurring in the German North and Baltic seas, common eiders were recently scored as intermediate in regards to behavioural sensitivity to ship traffic, yet highly sensitive when considering the effects of ship traffic on the population level, and they consequently scored as relatively vulnerable to disturbance overall (Fliessbach et al. 2019). We emphasize that for moulting common eiders, which already have an increased energy expenditure (Guillemette et al. 2007), the additional costs for displacement due to ship or boat disturbance, possibly in a highly energy demanding way as flapping or flying (Pelletier et al. 2008) add up. Furthermore, the observed carry-over effects of energetic demands and stress from the moult period to the subsequent breeding season (Harms et al. 2015) highlight the possible consequences of disturbance and energy expenditure during moult on individual fitness and possibly population trajectories.

Given our results, the ongoing population decline of eider ducks in Finland, Sweden and Norway (Ekroos et al. 2012; Anker-Nilssen et al. 2015; Kilpi et al. 2015), and the expected further increase in coastal recreational and commercial boat and ship traffic, we emphasize the need to reduce disturbance for sea ducks, particularly during moult. Our study provides important quantitative knowledge about the effects of approach of boats on moulting common eiders and can be used for policy-makers to define buffer zones around known moulting sites, e.g. when regulating recreational boat traffic. Such buffer zones should ideally include areas used by common eiders for foraging and resting as well as corridors between both. Further, our results may be used as a knowledge base to create more awareness among boat drivers, particularly those in coastal waters and for recreational purposes.