Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-dnltx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T01:15:54.226Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Welfare by the ear: comparing relative durations and frequencies of ear postures by using an automated tracking system in sheep

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2023

S Vögeli
Affiliation:
Centre for Proper Housing of Ruminants and Pigs, Federal Food Safety and Veterinary Office FSVO, Agroscope, Institute for Livestock Science,Tänikon, CH-8356 Ettenhausen, Switzerland Animal Behaviour, Institute of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental Studies, University of Zurich, Winterthurerstrasse 190, CH-8057 Zurich, Switzerland
B Wechsler
Affiliation:
Centre for Proper Housing of Ruminants and Pigs, Federal Food Safety and Veterinary Office FSVO, Agroscope, Institute for Livestock Science,Tänikon, CH-8356 Ettenhausen, Switzerland
L Gygax*
Affiliation:
Centre for Proper Housing of Ruminants and Pigs, Federal Food Safety and Veterinary Office FSVO, Agroscope, Institute for Livestock Science,Tänikon, CH-8356 Ettenhausen, Switzerland
*
* Contact for correspondence and requests for reprints: lorenz.gygax@agroscope.admin.ch

Abstract

Given the increased interest in animal emotional reactions for assessing welfare, indicators for such reactions are sought. Ear postures and movements have been found to be promising indicators of emotional states in sheep and other animals. The manual recording of ear postures, however, is very time consuming and possibly prone to a degree of inaccuracy due to the subtle and fast nature of ear movements that have to be identified. Therefore, a number of previous studies have analysed the frequency of certain ear postures relative to all ear posture changes rather than measuring the relative duration spent with different ear postures. Here, we present an automated, continuous tracking system that keeps track of small and lightweight marker balls attached to the head and ears of sheep. We measured ear postures and movements when the animals were confronted with three physical stimuli thought to differ in valence (from negative to intermediate to positive). We then compared new ear-posture definitions reflecting the real time spent with certain ear postures during stimulation with previous definitions used for video-based analyses that assessed ear-posture changes in relation to the total number of observed ear postures. In the analysis, we correlated new and previous measures both between and within experimental stimuli using residuals from mixed-effects models. We found that the new and previous definitions of ear postures and movements correlated highly. Given these high correlations and the discussed theoretical and practical advantages of the automated tracking, the new recording system can be recommended highly for assessing reactions in animals that may indicate emotional states.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2014 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Boissy, A, Aubert, A, Désiré, L, Greiveldinger, L, Delval, E and Veissier, I 2011 Cognitive sciences to relate ear postures to emotions in sheep. Animal Welfare 20: 4756Google Scholar
Boissy, A, Manteuffel, G, Jensen, MB, Moe, RO, Spruijt, B, Keeling, LJ, Winckler, C, Forkman, B, Dimitrov, I, Langbein, J, Bakken, M, Veissier, I and Aubert, A 2007 Assessment of positive emotions in animals to improve their welfare. Physiology & Behavior 92: 375397. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.phys-beh.2007.02.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Courtney, JD 1997 Automatic video indexing via object motion analysis. Pattern Recognition 30: 607625. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0031-3203(96)00107-0Google Scholar
Crispim Junior, CF, Pederiva, CN, Bose, RC, Garcia, VA, Lino-de-Oliveira, C and Marino-Neto, J 2012 ETHOWATCHER: validation of a tool for behavioral and video-tracking analysis in laboratory animals. Computers in Biology and Medicine 42: 257264. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.compbio-med.2011.12.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gong, J and Caldas, CH 2011 An object recognition, tracking, and contextual reasoning-based video interpretation method for rapid pro-ductivity analysis of construction operations. Automation in Construction 20: 12111226. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.autcon.2011.05.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greiveldinger, L, Veissier, I and Boissy, A 2009 Behavioural and physiological responses of lambs to controllable vs. uncon-trollable aversive events. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34: 805814. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, PR and Bateson, PPG 1993 Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139168342CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mendl, M, Burman, OHP, Parker, RMA and Paul, ES 2009 Cognitive bias as an indicator of animal emotion and welfare: Emerging evidence and underlying mechanisms. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 118: 161181. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appla-nim.2009.02.023CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfau, T, Robilliard, JJ, Weller, R, Jespers, K, Eliashar, E and Wilson, AM 2007 Assessment of mild hindlimb lameness during over ground locomotion using linear discriminant analysis of iner-tial sensor data. Equine Veterinary Journal 39: 407413. http://dx.doi.org/10.2746/042516407X185719CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team 2013 R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria. http://www.R-project.org/Google Scholar
Reefmann, N, Bütikofer Kaszàs, F, Wechsler, B and Gygax, L 2009a Ear and tail postures as indicators of emotional valence in sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 118: 199207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2009.02.013Google Scholar
Reefmann, N, Wechsler, B and Gygax, L 2009b Behavioural and physiological assessment of positive and negative emotion in sheep. Animal Behaviour 78: 651659. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.06.015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reimert, I, Bolhuis, JE, Kemp, B and Rodenburg, TB 2013 Indicators of positive and negative emotions and emotional contagion in pigs. Physiology & Behavior109: 42-50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.11.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmied, C, Waiblinger, S, Scharl, T, Leisch, F and Boivin, X 2008 Stroking of different body regions by a human: Effects on behaviour and heart rate of dairy cows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 109: 2538. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2007.01.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tami, G and Gallagher, A 2009 Description of the behaviour of domestic dog (Canis familiaris) by experienced and inexperienced people. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 120: 159169. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2009.06.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verbeek, E, Ferguson, D, Quinquet de Monjour, P and Lee, C 2012 Opioid control of behaviour in sheep: Effects of morphine and naloxone on food intake, activity and the affective state. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 142: 1829. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2012.09.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vögeli, S, Lutz, J, Wolf, M, Wechsler, B and Gygax, L 2014 Valence of physical stimuli, not housing conditions, affects behav-iour and frontal cortical brain activity in sheep. Behavioural Brain Research 267: 144155. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2014.03.036CrossRefGoogle Scholar