Skip to main content
Log in

Cueing others’ memories

  • Published:
Memory & Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Many situations require us to generate external cues to support later retrieval from memory. For instance, we create file names in order to cue our memory to a file’s contents, and instructors create lecture slides to remember what points to make during classes. We even generate cues for others when we remind friends of shared experiences or send colleagues a computer file that is named in such a way so as to remind them of its contents. Here we explore how and how well learners tailor retrieval cues for different intended recipients. Across three experiments, subjects generated verbal cues for a list of target words for themselves or for others. Learners generated cues for others by increasing the normative cue-to-target associative strength but also by increasing the number of other words their cues point to, relative to cues that they generated for themselves. This strategy was effective: such cues supported higher levels of recall for others than cues generated for oneself. Generating cues for others also required more time than generating cues for oneself. Learners responded to the differential demands of cue generation for others by effortfully excluding personal, episodic knowledge and including knowledge that they estimate to be broadly shared.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Andersson, J., & Ronnberg, J. (1997). Cued memory collaboration: Effects of friendship and type of retrieval cue. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 273–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ariel, R., Dunlosky, J., & Bailey, H. (2009). Agenda-based regulation of study-time allocation: When agendas override item-based monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138, 432–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bellezza, F. S., & Young, D. R. (1989). Chunking of repeated events in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 990–997.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benjamin, A. S., & Bird, R. D. (2006). Metacognitive control of the spacing of study repetitions. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 126–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borgida, E., & Brekke, N. (1981). The base-rate fallacy in attribution and prediction. In J. H. Harvey, W. J. Ickes, & R. F. Kidd (Eds.), New directions in attribution research (pp. 66–95). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H. (1972). Stimulus-sampling theory of encoding variability. In A. W. Melton & E. Martin (Eds.), Coding processes in human memory. Washington, DC: Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Camerer, C., Lowenstein, G., & Weber, M. (1989). The curse of knowledge in economic settings: An experimental analysis. Journal of Political Economy, 97, 1232–1254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Danks, J. H. (1970). Encoding of novel figures for communication and memory. Cognitive Psychology, 1, 179–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Earhard, M. (1967). Subjective organization and list organization as determinants of free-recall and serial-recall memorization. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 501–507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Estes, W. K. (1955). Statistical theory of distributional phenomena in learning. Psychological Review, 62, 369–377.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, S. M., & Underwood, B. J. (1957). Stimulus recall following paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53, 11–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Finley, J. R., & Benjamin, A., S. (2012). Adaptive changes in encoding strategy with experience: Evidence from the test expectancy paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(3), 632–652.

  • Hanna, J. E., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Trueswell, J. C. (2003). The effects of common ground and perspective on domains of referential interpretation. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 43–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horton, W. S., & Keysar, B. (1996). When do speakers take into account common ground? Cognition, 59, 91–117.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, R. R., & Smith, R. E. (1996). Accessing the particular from the general: The power of distinctiveness in the context of organization. Memory & Cognition, 24(2), 217–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isaacs, E. A., & Clark, H. H. (1987). References in conversation between experts and novices. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116, 26–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., Kruglanski, A. W., & Nelson, T. O. (1998). Social metacognition: An expansionist review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 137–154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction. Psychological Review, 80, 237–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keysar, B., Barr, D. J., Balin, J. A., & Brauner, J. S. (2000). Taking perspective in conversation: The role of mutual knowledge in comprehension. Psychological Science, 11, 32–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keysar, B., Barr, D. J., Balin, J. A., & Paek, T. S. (1998). Definite reference and mutual knowledge: Process models of common ground in comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keysar, B., Lin, S., & Barr, D. J. (2003). Limits on theory of mind use in adults. Cognition, 89, 25–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., & Bjork, R. A. (2006). Mending metacognitive illusions: A comparison of mnemonic-based and theory-based procedures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1133–1145.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., Ma’ayan, H., & Nussinson, R. (2006). The intricate relationships between monitoring and control in metacognition: Lessons for the cause-and-effect relation between subjective experience and behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 36–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kornell, N., & Bjork, R. A. (2009). A stability bias in human memory: Overestimating remembering and underestimating learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138, 449–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kornell, N., & Metcalfe, J. (2006). Study efficacy and the region of proximal learning framework. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 32, 609–622.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kornell, N., & Son, L. K. (2009). Learners’ choices and beliefs about self-testing. Memory, 17(5), 493–501.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krauss, R. M., Vivekananthan, P. S., & Weinheimer, S. (1968). “Inner speech” and “External speech”: Characteristics and communication effectiveness of socially and nonsocially encoded messages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 295–300.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. J. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 476–490.

  • Mäntylä, T. (1986). Optimizing cue effectiveness: Recall of 500 and 600 incidentally learned words. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 66–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mäntylä, T., & Nilsson, L. G. (1983). Are my cues better than your cues? Uniqueness and reconstruction as prerequisites for optimal recall of verbal materials. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 24, 303–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mäntylä, T., & Nilsson, L. G. (1988). Cue distinctiveness and forgetting: Effectiveness of self-generated retrieval cues in delayed recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 502–509.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J. (2002). Is study time allocated selectively to a region of proximal learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 349–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nairne, J. S. (2002). The myth of the encoding-retrieval match. Memory, 10, 389–395.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O., Dunlosky, J., Graf, A., & Narens, L. (1994). Utilization of metacognitive judgments in the allocation of study during multitrial learning. Psychological Science, 5(4), 207–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Schreiber, T. A. (1998). The University of South Florida word association, rhyme, and word fragment norms. http://www.usf.edu/FreeAssociation/

  • Nickerson, R. S., Baddeley, A., & Freeman, B. (1987). Are people’s estimates of what other people know influenced by what they themselves know? Acta Psychologica, 64, 245–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Son, L. K. (2004). Spacing one’s study: Evidence for a metacognitive control strategy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 601–604.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Son, L. K., & Metcalfe, J. (2000). Metacognitive and control studies in study-time allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(1), 204–221.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thiede, K. W., & Dunlosky, J. (1999). Toward a general model of self-regulated study: An analysis of selection of items for study and self-paced study time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 1024–1037.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toppino, T. C., Cohen, M. S., Davis, M. L., & Moors, A. C. (2009). Metacognitive control over the distribution of practice: When is spacing preferred? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 1352–1358.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tullis, J. G., Benjamin, A. S., & Feichter, J. (in prep.). The effectiveness of learners’ testing choices.

  • Tullis, J. G., & Benjamin, A. S. (2011). On the effectiveness of self-paced learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 64, 109–118.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tullis, J. G., & Benjamin, A. S. (2012). Consequences of restudy choices in younger and older learners. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 743–749.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tullis, J. G., & Benjamin, A. S. (under review). Cue generation: How learners flexibly support future retrieval.

  • Tullis, J. G., Benjamin, A. S., & Liu, X. (in press). Self-pacing study of faces of different races: Metacognitive control over study does not eliminate the cross-race recognition effect. Memory & Cognition.

  • Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80, 352–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, O. C., & Watkins, M. J. (1975). Buildup of proactive inhibition as a cue-overload effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 1, 442–452.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author Note

This research was funded in part by grant R01 AG026263 from the National Institutes of Health. The first author was supported by a graduate fellowship from the National Science Foundation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jonathan G. Tullis.

Appendix A

Appendix A

We combined the cued recall performance data across Experiments 1 and 2 to analyze the pattern of data across all subjects, and the results are shown in Fig. 6. A repeated measures ANOVA on cued recall revealed a significant interaction between cue originator and intended recipient (F(1, 78) = 14.82, p < 0.001, η2 partial = 0.16), and a main effect of cue originator (F(1, 78) = 304.05, p < 0.001, η2 partial = 0.80). Follow-up t-tests show that when the cue originator is a different learner, cues intended for others led to better memory (M = 0.47) than cues intended for self (M = 0.40; t(79) = 3.28, p = 0.002, η2 partial = 0.37). When the cue originator is oneself, cues intended for others led to numerically worse recall (M = 0.75) than cues intended for self (M = 0.77), but this difference did not reach significance (t(79) = 1.55, p = 0.12, d = 0.18).

We further analyzed cued recall performance combined across Experiments 1 and 2 based upon whether the learner provided different cues for self and for other. Cue originators differentiated between cues for self and for others on approximately 50 % of the trials. For the subset of trials on which the cue originator provided different cues for self and other, the cue originator interacted with intended recipient (F(1,54) = 17.61, p < 0.001, η2 partial = 0.25), as shown in the left graph in Fig. 6. T-tests showed that cues for others were better at supporting memory than cues for self when given to others (t(60) = 4.44, p < 0.001, d = 0.57) and cues for self were better at supporting memory than cues for others when given to self (t(78) = 2.01, p = 0.05, d = 0.24). As shown in the right graph of Fig. 6, the proportion of targets that learners recalled did not differ based upon intended recipient when the cue originator did not provide different cues for self and for others (F(1, 62) = 1.08; p = 0.30, η2 partial = 0.02), as recall could only differ through the random assignment of items to conditions. When learners distinguished between cues for self and cues for others, learners’ own cues intended for themselves were more beneficial than learners’ cues intended for others. When learners relied upon idiosyncratic information in generating their cue, it supported their own future memory performance better than shared, common cues.

Fig. 6
figure 6

Cued recall performance as a function of cue originator and intended recipient, conditionalized upon whether the cue originator provided different cues for self and other (left graph) or did NOT provide different cues for self and other (right graph). Error bars (and corresponding number labels) show the 95 % confidence intervals of the difference between intended recipients for each pair of bars. Both graphs show data combined across Experiments 1 and 2

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tullis, J.G., Benjamin, A.S. Cueing others’ memories. Mem Cogn 43, 634–646 (2015). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0478-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0478-y

Keywords

Navigation