Introduction

Categorization of auditory sensory information is vital for making rapid decisions in an acoustic environment. For instance, the ability to correctly map a complex harmonic tone to the category car horn is advantageous when one is crossing a road. Recent decades have brought about a considerable body of research on how categories are formed and maintained (Ashby & Waldron, 1999; McQueen, 1996; Nosofsky, 1988; Rosch, 1973, 1978; Russ, Lee, & Cohen, 2007; Sloutsky, 2003; Spiering & Ashby, 2008; Verbeemen, Vanpaemel, Pattyn, Storms, & Verguts, 2007; Yamauchi, Love, & Markman, 2002; for reviews, see Ashby & Maddox, 2005, 2011). This work has become increasingly focused on auditory categories (Goudbeek, Cutler, & Smits, 2008; Guenther & Bohland, 2002; Guenther, Nieto-Castanon, Ghosh, & Tourville, 2004; Holt & Lotto, 2006; Mirman, Holt, & McClelland, 2004).

There is a consensus that auditory categorization involves the utilization and integration of different acoustic dimensions (e.g., spectrum [pitch, timbre], duration; Goudbeek, Swingley, & Smits, 2009; Holt & Lotto, 2006), but it is less clear how integration of information from integral (nonseparable; Goudbeek et al., 2009) dimensions might differ from integration of information from nonintegral (separable) dimensions. Furthermore, integration across dimensions might be influenced by prior knowledge of specific correlations between dimensions, particularly from speech. The purpose of this study was to assess the degree to which integration of information from two dimensions is influenced by (1) the integrality of acoustic dimensions (here, location of spectral peaks in frequency space), (2) the correlation between these dimensions (positive vs. negative correlation of first [S1] and second [S2] spectral peak), and (3) the presence or absence of immediate corrective feedback.

To that end, we focused on two modeling approaches that are particularly apt to these purposes: decision bound theory and logistic regression. Logistic regression has previously been applied to auditory categorization and can be used to predict category membership decisions on the basis of stimulus properties (Hosmer & Lemeshow, 2000). Values along a stimulus dimension are entered into the regressions as continuous variables, and the β-weight for this regressor reflects the predictive power of the stimulus dimension with respect to categorization responses. Comparisons of β-weights for different stimulus dimensions allow assessment of the degree to which individual stimulus properties are used for categorization. For instance, Goudbeek et al. (2009) assessed strategy use in two conditions where either frequency or duration was the relevant dimension. Responses were predicted from frequency and duration with logistic regressions that yielded β-weights for either dimension. A higher β-weight for frequency, as compared with duration, in the condition where frequency was the relevant dimension showed that participants indeed used this dimension for their response.

On the other hand, decision bound theory (Ashby & Gott, 1988) assumes that category acquisition involves learning to divide perceptual space, corresponding to an internal representation of stimulus space, into response regions according to a linear or nonlinear boundary (Ashby & Waldron, 1999). The position of a novel stimulus in perceptual space is compared with the location of the boundary, and the corresponding response is assigned. Thus, from this perspective, category learning is a signal detection problem where the decision bound separating categories corresponds to the response criterion. Decision bound models for auditory categorization have been extensively studied in the visual domain and, thus, provide a framework from which we can generate specific predictions about strategy use in novel category learning.

Thus far, no study of which we are aware has combined these two approaches (i.e., logistic regression and decision bound models). In this regard, the present study complements and goes beyond previous research. In what follows, we will describe in detail two strategies for novel category learning that emerge from decision bound theory and that we assess in the context of auditory category learning in the present study.

Rule-based and information integration category learning

The distinction between rule-based and information integration category learning comes from a neuropsychological model called competition between verbal and implicit systems (COVIS; Ashby, Alfonso-Reese, Turken, & Waldron, 1998; Ashby & Waldron, 1999). The model assumes that learning involves two systems that compete or interact (Ashby & Crossley, 2010) and differ in their functions and neural underpinnings. Rule-based learning involves categorization based on an explicit rule that is frequently relatively easy to verbalize (e.g., if a tone is high in frequency, respond “category A”; otherwise, respond “category B”). Generally, rule-based decision bounds are orthogonal to the dimension on which the decision should be based. Rule-based learning is assumed to predominantly depend on an explicit hypothesis-testing system (Maddox, Filoteo, Lauritzen, Connally, & Hejl, 2005), subserved by the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the anterior cingulate, and the caudate nucleus (Ashby & Ell, 2001; Rao et al., 1997).

On the other hand, information integration category learning tasks require a predecisional combination of information from more than one dimension. Usually, the optimal rule in information integration tasks is not easily verbalized (e.g. if a tone is higher in frequency than it is long in duration, respond “A”; otherwise, respond “category B”). Decision bounds are not orthogonal to the dimension on which the decision should be based but, rather, are represented as a diagonal—for example, in a two-dimensional stimulus space. Successful learning of an information integration task is proposed to rely on an implicit procedural learning system that depends on feedback processes (Ashby & Waldron, 1999; Maddox, Filoteo, Hejl, & Ing, 2004; Maddox et al., 2005). This system is claimed to be subserved by the body and tail of the caudate (Nomura et al., 2007; Seger & Cincotta, 2005). In several studies, it has been shown that information integration is indeed dependent on (immediate) feedback in categorization or discrimination tasks (Ashby, Queller, & Berretty, 1999; Ashby & Waldron, 1999).

In the present study, we applied decision bound modeling of rule-based and information integration category learning to an auditory categorization task. Decision bound theory provided us with an optimal tool with which to evaluate the success of information integration across two acoustic dimensions in making category membership decisions.

Auditory category learning

A number of studies have attempted to distinguish between rule-based and information integration strategies in auditory categorization performance (e.g., Goudbeek et al., 2008; Goudbeek et al., 2009; Holt & Lotto, 2006, 2008; Maddox, Ing, & Lauritzen, 2006; Mirman et al., 2004; Smits, Sereno, & Jongman, 2006), although relatively few of them directly applied decision bound models to describe learning. For instance, in studies by Goudbeek et al. (2009) and Smits et al., participants learned to categorize inharmonic complex tones that varied along the dimensions of duration and spectral filter location (analogous to the first formant frequency, F1, in speech). Goudbeek et al. (2009) examined performance for category distributions that were best separated by a unidimensional duration-based boundary, a unidimensional frequency (i.e., spectral filter location) boundary, and a diagonal (information integration) boundary that required categorizing stimuli on the basis of a combination of duration and frequency values. The authors observed much poorer performance for the information integration condition, relative to either of the rule-based conditions. On this basis, they hypothesized that rule-based category learning may be the default strategy in audition (cf. Maddox et al., 2006).

However, it is also possible that rule-based learning may have been used more often due to use of duration and frequency dimensions, which have been suggested to be separable, rather than integral (Grau & Kemler Nelson, 1988; Silbert, Townsend, & Lentz, 2009). Prior research on acoustic-phonetic processing has suggested that dimensions that vary in the same domain (e.g., frequency) are more likely to be integral (Kingston, Diehl, Kirk, & Castleman, 2008; Kingston & Macmillan, 1995; Kingston, Macmillan, Dickey, Thorburn, & Bartels, 1997). Consistent with this suggestion, Maddox and colleagues (Maddox, Molis, & Diehl, 2002) showed that a model of categorization performance assuming information integration accounted well for performance when stimuli varied in their second and third resonance frequencies; however, they did not examine learning per se, since the categories were already highly learned.

Furthermore, previous research also stressed the importance of how acoustic dimensions are related to each other. In this respect, negative correlations between spectral filter dimensions are relevant with respect to intrinsic pitch of vowels, reflecting the impression that high vowels (with a low F1) have slightly higher pitch (higher f0) than do low vowels (with a higher F1; Lehiste & Peterson, 1961), and vowel nasalization, showing that the more nasalized a vowel, the lower its F1 frequency (Diehl, Kluender, & Walsh, 1990). Given that these correlations hold cross-linguistically (likely to be based on articulatory constraints; Carre, 2009), listeners should be familiar with them and, correspondingly, benefit in novel categorization situations that employ such correlations.

Our two experiments sought to assess whether (1) information is integrated across two integral acoustic dimensions, (2) information integration depends on the correlation of the acoustic dimensions, and (3) information integration requires immediate feedback. For these reasons, both experiments used stimuli that differed in the location of two spectral peaks (analogous to the first formant frequencies of vowels, F1 and F2) and comprised a learning phase with immediate feedback as well as a maintenance phase without feedback. In Experiment 1, we examined distributions whose decision bounds would similarly allow for rule-based or information integration categorization, thereby assessing the natural inclination for a particular strategy during the categorization of auditory stimuli with integral dimensions (Fig. 1a). In contrast, Experiment 2 used stimulus distributions that required information integration for optimal performance (Fig. 2a).

Fig. 1
figure 1

a Stimulus distributions in the S1–S2 space for Experiment 1. The dotted lines indicate unidimensional decision bounds for either S1 (vertical) or S2 (horizontal). The diagonal solid line represents a decision bound for information integration. The “rising” distribution (left) is characterized by a positive correlation between S1 and S2, while the “falling” distribution (middle) yields a negative S1–S2 correlation. The right panel shows the equidistantly spaced grid for the maintenance phase. b Stimulus distributions for Experiment 2. Information integration decision bounds are represented by the solid diagonal lines. Again, the rightmost panel illustrates the stimulus arrangement for the maintenance phase. Note that stimuli in the maintenance phase were matched to the corresponding distributions in the learning phase, such that there exist small differences between Experiments 1 and 2

Fig. 2
figure 2

Performance measure per block in the learning phase of Experiment 1. Error bars indicate across-subjects standard errors of the means. Note the absence of any effect of falling versus rising stimulus distribution on performance

Our hypotheses are as follows:

  1. 1.

    On the basis of Maddox et al. (2006), we assume that rule-based categorization is the preferred strategy in audition. Therefore, in both experiments, we should see substantial evidence for rule-based behavior.

  2. 2.

    The long-term experience with negative correlations in speech (and corresponding decision bounds) should shape the short-term categorization of nonspeech stimuli. As a result, more ready use of an information integration strategy, and consequently, better category-learning performance should be observed for distributions with negative correlations between spectral peak frequencies. Since Experiment 2 was designed such that information integration would yield optimal performance (Fig. 1b), we expect differences between correlations to particularly manifest themselves in this experiment.

  3. 3.

    Finally, information integration seems to require immediate feedback (Ashby et al., 1999; Ashby & Waldron, 1999). We therefore expect more information integration in the learning than in the maintenance phase of our experiments.

Experiment 1

Experiment 1 extended the work of Goudbeek and colleagues (Goudbeek et al., 2008; Goudbeek et al., 2009) to stimuli varying along integral acoustic dimensions. Two distribution types were examined. In the falling condition, stimulus distributions were characterized by a negative correlation between spectral filter locations, while the rising condition distributions were characterized by a positive correlation. In both conditions in Experiment 1, rule-based and information integration strategy use would have yielded little performance difference so that we could assess the natural inclinations of participants.

Accuracy analyses were supplemented by fitting a number of decision bound models (Ashby, 1992) to individual participant data in order to assess strategy use during category learning. As was outlined above, we also calculated logistic regressions with the dependent variable category A vs. B for both learning and maintenance phases (Hilbe, 2009) in order to quantify the contributions of each spectral dimension to single-trial category membership decisions. On the basis of previous findings (little information integration use; Goudbeek et al., 2009; Maddox et al., 2006) and as a consequence of the stimulus materials in Experiment 1 (no strong bias for either strategy), we predicted a bias toward using rule-based strategies.

We did not expect differences between the rising and falling distributions. This is because observing a difference between rising and falling distributions should have depended on adoption of an information integration strategy, which we did not predict to observe in Experiment 1. For the same reason, we predicted that immediate feedback in Experiment 1 would play no or only a negligible role, since it is claimed to be important for information integration, but not rule-based learning (Ashby et al., 1999; Ashby & Waldron, 1999).

Method

Materials

Stimuli were created with PRAAT (Boersma & Weenink, 2011) in two steps. First, a 90-ms white noise was generated; the duration was chosen on the basis of previous studies (Goudbeek et al., 2008; Goudbeek et al., 2009). Second, the white noise was filtered in two frequency bands that approximated the location of the first and second formant frequencies of naturally produced vowels—that is, F1 and F2, respectively. Target filter frequencies are referred to as spectral filter frequencies, S1 and S2, throughout this article, and are normalized to Bark (Zwicker, 1961). The Bark conversion is commonly applied in acoustic-phonetic research and accounts for the nonlinearity of the frequency resolution by the human auditory system.

The same original white noise token was used as the basis for all 1,000 stimuli that were generated for each category (A and B) and for each distribution (falling and rising), with different S1 and S2 filter frequencies in each case. Filter frequencies were drawn from the distributions shown in Fig. 1a. In order to arrive at the stretched distributions along the falling and rising diagonals in the S1/S2 space (with a slope of −1 and +1), the linear equation for the diagonal running through the distribution center was calculated. Then individual bivariate normal distributions were generated that had means, μ, at 40 (x,y) locations along the diagonal and equal standard deviations, σ (see Table 1 for details). Twenty-five tokens per distribution were randomly generated, yielding a total of 1,000 stimuli per distribution.

Table 1 Stimulus details in Experiment 1: Distribution ranges (first value, starting point; second value, end point), means (μ) and standard deviations (σ) are given for both spectral filter frequencies (S1, S2) in Bark. Physical δ′ is calculated on the basis of distribution means and standard deviations. For rule-based (RB) optimal boundaries, δ′ can be calculated for both dimensions, while for information integration (II) optimal boundaries, δ′ can be calculated only for the integrated dimension and is based on the Euclidean distance between the distribution means in the two-dimensional space. A and B are arbitrary labels and refer to stimulus sets in the S1/S2 space, where the leftmost set (with low S1) is A and the rightmost (with high S2) is B

Noises were filtered with fast IIR filters, comprising two recursive filter coefficients. Filter bandwidths were 0.2 times the target filter frequency. Stimuli were normalized to an equal average intensity that approximated 60 dB SPL (Boersma & Weenink, 2011). Onsets were multiplied with the first half period of a [1 cos(x)] * 0.5 function, and offsets with the first half period of a [1 + cos(x)] *0.5 function, over a duration of 10 ms in each case, in order to eliminate acoustic artifacts.

In order to arrive at a stimulus-based measure for the likelihood of strategy preference, we determined the normalized distance between the means of category A and category B distributions, here referred to as δ', along both the S1 and S2 dimensions. The information integration δ' (Euclidean distance between distribution means) was of comparable size to the rule-based δ' values (difference between means for each dimension, S1 and S2). We expect that larger category distances would lead to better categorization performance, such that if participants were to utilize optimal strategies, they ought to prefer those for which δ' is largest. In Experiment 1, the similarity of the distances therefore suggested equal strategy preference.

Stimuli for the nonfeedback maintenance phase were arranged in an equidistantly spaced grid with step sizes of 2/3 Bark in either dimension (S1, 5–9 Bark; S2, 9–13 Bark). They thus described a 6 × 6 grid that evenly covered the critical region of the original stimulus space (Fig. 2a).

Participants and procedure

Thirty-three native speakers of German (all right-handed) participated in Experiment 1 (16 males; mean age 25.76, SD 2.42). They were drawn from the participant pool of the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, and received monetary compensation for their participation. None of them reported a history of hearing problems.

Participants were randomly assigned to either the rising (n = 17; 7 males; mean age 25.29, SD 2.08) or the falling (n = 16; 9 males, mean age 26.25, SD 2.72) distribution condition. Participants first completed eight blocks (36 trials each) during the learning phase. On each trial, a single stimulus was randomly selected from category A (1,000 exemplars) or B (1,000 exemplars), with the following restrictions: (1) No stimulus could be selected more than once for a given participant in the learning phase of the experiment; (2) within each block, category A and B stimuli were equally probable (p = .5). After stimulus presentation, participants indicated whether it belonged to category A or category B by pressing one of two keys on a computer keyboard (button assignments for the two categories were counterbalanced across participants).

Following the response, participants received corrective feedback, which was displayed for 1 s in the middle of a CRT screen (Sony Multiscan E430). Correct feedback was given in bold green font (24 points), while incorrect feedback was given in bold red font (24 points). Participants were allowed a short break following each block.

Participants then completed two maintenance blocks (also 36 trials each). On each trial, participants were presented with a stimulus sampled from the equidistantly spaced grid described above. Critically, during the maintenance phase, participants did not receive feedback about their responses. The entire experiment lasted for about 20 min.

Stimuli were presented on a Windows-based PC, using the stimulation software PRESENTATION (Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc., version 13.9), and were transmitted through a Creative Labs Audigy II sound card onto Sennheiser HD 201 headphones.

Results

Accuracy results

Overall, performance differed significantly from chance (d= 1.35, SD = 0.43), t(32) = 32.94, p < .001. Accuracy in the learning phase was assessed by d′, a signal detection measure of perceptual sensitivity that is independent of response bias (Macmillan & Creelman, 2005). Figure 2 shows d′ as a function of block separately for the rising and falling distributions. In order to assess learning, d′ values were entered into a mixed–measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with block as a within-subjects and distribution (rising vs. falling) as a between-subjects variable. For all ANOVAs, we report partial eta squared (η p 2) as a measure of effect size and Greenhouse–Geisser-corrected p-values and degrees of freedom in cases of sphericity violations. There were no significant main effects [block, F(6.1, 188.9) = 1.14, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.04 \), p = .34; distribution, F(1, 31) = 0.002, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.00001 \), p = .97] and also no block × distribution interaction, F(6.1, 188.9) = 0.43, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.04 \), p = .89. Hence, performance did not differ across blocks, and performance across blocks did not differ as a function of distribution condition.

Logistic regression

In order to assess the degree to which category membership judgments (i.e., A vs. B) depended on the acoustic dimensions under investigation (i.e., S1, S2), logistic regressions were calculated in order to predict category A responses from S1 and S2 and their interaction. Note that a significant β-weight indicates the importance of the dimension in determining category membership. Logistic regression models were calculated separately for the learning and the maintenance phases.

Learning phase

The model comprised the regressors S1 and S2 and the factors block and distribution (rising, falling). The following interactions were also included in the model: S1 × S2, S1 × block, S2 × block, S1 × distribution, and S2 × distribution. S1 values per trial significantly predicted category judgments, β = −1.27, z = −4.82, p < .001, but there was no interaction with block, z = −0.02, p = .24, indicating that participants similarly weighted S1 information in making category judgments over the course of the learning phase. The S1 × distribution interaction reached significance, β = −0.35, z = −2.21, p < .05, indicating that category A responses were better predicted by S1 in the falling than in the rising distribution. None of the other factors or interactions were significant, z < 2, n.s.

Maintenance phase

The model included the same predictor variables listed above for the learning phase model, with the exception of block, which was not included here. There was no significant S1 effect, even though there was a trend for more category A responses at lower S1 values, β = −1.82, z = −1.38, p < .16; the S1 effect reached significance if the S1 × S2 interaction was removed from the model, β = −2.34, z = −13.08, p < .001. In the full model, there was a further significant effect of distribution, β = 3.97, z = 2.06, p < .05, reflecting that category A responses were better predicted in the falling than in the rising condition, and this effect was qualified by the significant S1 × distribution interaction, β = −0.53, z = −1.92, p = .05.

Learning versus maintenance phases

Cue utilization during the learning versus maintenance phases was compared directly by entering the absolute values of β-weights for S1 and S2 into a mixed–measures ANOVA with the within-subjects factors phase (learning, maintenance) and filter (S1, S2) and the between-subjects factor distribution (rising, falling). Individual β-weights stemmed from the learning and maintenance models reported above, except that we did not include block in the learning phase model (parallel to the maintenance phase model). Note that for these analyses, only the magnitude (but not the sign) of the β-weights provided interesting information regarding the importance of each dimension to category judgments. There were significant main effects of filter, F(1, 93) = 217.99, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.701 \), p < .001, and phase, F(1, 93) = 43.52, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.319 \), p < .01. Higher βs were observed for S1 (2.55, SD = 1.20) than for S2 (0.62, SD = 0.53), and βs were higher in the maintenance (2.02, SD = 1.57) than in the learning (1.15, SD = 0.90) phase. Furthermore, there was a trend for a distribution × filter interaction, F(1, 93) = 2.03, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.021 \), p = .14, reflecting that within the falling distribution, the difference between βs for S1 and S2 (2.70, SD = 1.18, vs. 0.55, SD = 0.55) was greater than within the rising distribution (2.43, SD = 1.24, vs. 0.67, SD = 0.52). There was also a filter × phase interaction, F(1, 93) = 16.50, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.151 \), p < .01, indicating that βs for S1 and S2 differed more in the maintenance (3.25, SD = 1.20, vs. 0.78, SD = 0.61) than in the learning (1.86, SD = 0.70, vs. 0.45, SD = 0.39) phase.

In order to visualize the degree to which participants relied on the individual dimensions, S1 and S2, we plotted βs for S2 (ordinate) against βs for S1 (abscissa) in the learning and in the maintenance phases. In these scatterplots, participants are coded according to whether they significantly used S1 and S2 (S1 + S2; blue diamonds), S1 only (S1; red squares), S2 only (S2; green triangles), or none of the dimensions (purple circles) for categorization. Significant usage was determined by βs that significantly differed from zero on the basis of the single-subject logistic regression models (α = .05). In these plots, participants who used both dimensions tended to fall on a diagonal. Participants with a preference for S2 are clustered near the ordinate, and participants with a preference for S1 are clustered near the abscissa (Fig. 3a). It can be seen from the figure that most participants relied on S1 in both the learning and the maintenance phases. The percentages of significant βs did not significantly differ between the falling and rising distributions (all χ 2s < 2.5, n.s.), even though we observed a trend for participants to be more likely to rely on both S1 and S2 in the falling, as compared with the rising, stimulus distribution.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Illustration of strategy use in Experiment 1. a. Left: Scatterplots of β weights (per subject) for S1 and S2 in the learning phase (left) and in the maintenance phase (right). Participants are coded as follows: blue diamonds, both S1βs and S2 βs significantly differ from zero; red squares, only S1βs significantly differ from zero; green triangles, only S2 βs significantly differ from zero; purple circles, none of the βs significantly differ from zero. The diagonals illustrate the location of βs that would correspond equally to an S1- and S2-based response. Right: Percentage of significant βs in the learning and maintenance phases, for the falling and rising distributions. Percentages are based on whether βs were significant for S1, S2, or both S1 and S2. b Left: Proportions of participants whose responses were best accounted for by the individual decision bound models. Block numbers marked with an asterisk indicate a significant difference in the proportion of participants using an S1 versus an S2 rule. Right: Percentage of rule-based (S1 and S2) and information integration models whose Bayes factors exceeded 3

In sum, the logistic regressions indicated that most participants relied on the first filter frequency, S1, during categorization and more strongly in the maintenance phase (without feedback) than in the learning phase (with feedback). On the other hand, some participants used information from both S1 and S2, while only very few exclusively used S2 for categorization.

Furthermore, categorization also somewhat differed between the falling and the rising distributions in that the reliance on S1 was greater in the falling than in the rising stimulus distribution and in that more participants tended to use both S1 and S2 in the falling than in the rising distribution.

Modeling results

Three families of decision bound models (e.g., Ashby & Gott, 1988; Maddox & Ashby, 1993) were fit to the data for each individual participant on a block-by-block basis to determine the decision strategy that best accounted for performance (Fig. 3b; see the Appendix for details): unidimensional rule-based, information integration, and random-response models. The two rule-based models assumed that listeners made use of unidimensional rules based on either S1 or S2. Two information integration models assumed an optimal decision bound or allowed decision bound slope and intercept to be free parameters but are summarized as one model for the remainder of this article. Finally, the random-response model presumes that participants guessed randomly on every trial. In order to assess whether the decision bound models provided substantial evidence, we transformed the respective Bayesian information criterion (BIC) scores to Bayes factors (Kass & Raftery, 1995; Raftery, 1986; see the Appendix) and subsequently used Jeffrey’s suggested scale of evidence. According to this scale, Bayes factors greater than 3 indicate substantial evidence for model use.

Almost all model fits (per participant and block) for the rule-based S1 and S2 models provided substantial evidence, while only 10 % of the model fits for information integration exceeded this threshold (Fig. 3b, right). The percentages did not differ between distributions (i.e., falling vs. rising; all χ 2s > 2, n.s.). Figure 3b (left) gives the proportion of listeners in the rising and falling distributions whose data were best fit by each of the tested models across blocks. All winning models had Bayes factors of >3.

Consistent with the results of the logistic regression analysis, participants in both the rising and falling distribution conditions made almost exclusive use of unidimensional rules, and participants were more likely to use a rule based on S1 than one based on S2. Chi-squared tests indicated that, overall, more participants relied on a unidimensional S1 rule, as compared with a unidimensional S2 rule, in six of the eight blocks. In order to account for multiple comparisons, we corrected our statistics with the false-discovery-rate (FDR) method (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995; FDR-corrected α-level = .05). Taking the distribution conditions separately, participants in the falling distribution condition exhibited this pattern more strongly, using a unidimensional S1 rule more often than a unidimensional S2 rule on four of the eight blocks (ps < .05), whereas this difference was not significant in any block for the rising distribution condition.

Convergence of logistic regression and decision bound models

To our knowledge, no study has assessed the degree to which the two approaches, logistic regressions and decision bound models, converge. For this reason, we explored the relationship between block-averaged β-weights and goodness-of-fit measures (i.e., BICs) separately for the unidimensional S1 and S2 decision bounds in two ANOVAs with block-averaged BIC scores as dependent variables. We were effectively asking to what degree BIC-scores supporting a rule-based S1 or S2 strategy could be predicted from β-weights of S1 or S2 logistic regressions. Note that information integration models were not included in these analyses, since the proportion of participants using information integration in Experiment 1 was too small for meaningful comparisons.

Both models included the between-subjects factor distribution (rising, falling), the regressor β-weight, as well as the β-weight × distribution interaction. The S1 model (with S1 BIC score as dependent variable) showed a significant effect of S1 β-weight, F(1, 29) = 28.16, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.492 \), p < .001, reflecting a negative correlation between β-weights and BIC scores (i.e., higher β-weights for lower BIC scores). However, the correlation was not modulated by distribution, as evidenced by no other significant main effects or interactions (all Fs < 3, all ps > .15). In the S2 model (with S2 BIC scores as dependent variable), >the negative correlation between β-weights was not significant, F(1, 29) = 2.33, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.074 \), p < .15, overall, but depended on distribution [β-weight × distribution: F(1, 29) = 3.07, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.113 \), p = .05]. The β-weight/BIC score correlation was significant for the falling, F(1, 15) = 4.88, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.245 \), p < .05, but not for the rising, F(1, 14) = 0.39, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.027 \), p = .54, distribution.

Overall, BIC scores supporting either S1 or S2 rule-based strategies negatively correlated with the corresponding absolute β-weights from the S1 and S2 logistic regression effects. Thus, β-weights and decision bound model BIC scores converged.

Prediction of performance by decision bound models

Finally, we tried to predict performance from strategy use; that is, we tested whether the likelihood of using a rule-based S1 or S2 categorization strategy was associated with better performance, as indexed by two separate mixed-measures ANOVAs with d' as the dependent variable, the proportions of rulel-based S1 and S2 strategy use and distribution (falling, rising) as independent variables. Since proportions of rule-based S1 and S2 strategy use are necessarily highly correlated, the two factors were investigated in separate ANOVAs.

None of the ANOVAs showed significant main effects or interactions (all Fs > 1, n.s.). Thus, performance in Experiment 1 did not depend on either rule-based S1 or S2 strategy use.

Discussion

Participants in Experiment 1 showed a strong preference for using a unidimensional rule-based decision bound for auditory categorization and primarily relied on the first filter frequency (S1). Thus, our prediction was borne out: Participants preferred a rule-based approach and did not exhibit differences in performance, as indexed by d', as a function of block for either the rising or the falling distribution condition. Performance was overall high beginning from block 1, indicating that listeners in both conditions discovered a strategy yielding good categorization performance immediately. However, additional learning was not apparent over blocks. This is likely because our category distributions overlapped, causing some stimuli to be ambiguous and performance to plateau below ceiling levels.

Our observation that participants generally tended to adopt a unidimensional strategy when categorizing auditory stimuli is in line with the study of Goudbeek et al. (2009). However, here the stimulus dimensions were based on the same acoustic dimension (i.e., frequency), suggesting that rule-based categorization strategies are preferred even if dimensions are integral.

Without an S1–S2 correlation and without feedback (i.e., in the maintenance phase), the magnitude of β-values for S1 was larger than in the learning phase; this S1 preference was more pronounced in the falling than in the rising distribution. Hence, in the maintenance phase, participants seemed to rely on S1 to a greater extent for the falling than for the rising distribution. The assumed special role of the falling distribution condition was further analyzed in Experiment 2, where optimal performance depended on the usage of an information integration strategy and where the decision bound was a falling (with a negative slope) or rising (with a positive slope) diagonal.

Crucially, Experiment 2 was motivated by the observation that negatively correlated acoustic dimensions seemed to be preferred in speech (and more general, in audition), for which reason we assume that if dimensions are required to be integrated, this is done more readily for those that show a negative, as compared with a positive, correlation.

Experiment 2

Experiment 2 made use of stimulus distributions that required a predecisional integration of information from two spectral dimensions—that is, for which rule-based categorization was a suboptimal strategy. We were interested in whether a substantial proportion would use an information integration strategy, and the degree to which this choice depended on the nature of the correlation between spectral filter locations, S1 and S2 (i.e., falling vs. rising). Due to the assumed familiarity with negative acoustic correlations, we expected that information integration would be more readily used in the falling, as compared with the rising, stimulus distribution. We also assumed that if rule-based categorization is indeed predominant in audition (Goudbeek et al., 2009; Maddox et al., 2006), some participants in Experiment 2 would continue using this strategy. Finally, the use of information integration strategies in Experiment 2 should also depend on the availability of immediate feedback (Maddox, Ashby, & Bohil, 2003), for which reason we did not expect indications of information integration in the maintenance phase (designed as in Experiment 1).

Method

Materials

The stimuli were similar to those in Experiment 1, with the exception that (1) category A and B distributions were parallel in the S1–S2 space, rather than lying on the same diagonal as in Experiment 1, and (2) the spread was increased in both dimensions, S1 and S2, in order to render categorization more difficult. S1/S2 ranges and standard deviations are illustrated in Table 2. In contrast to Experiment 1, the normalized distance, δ′, was considerably higher for the information integration bound than for either of the rule-based bounds; thus, best performance would be attainable by an information integration strategy. Parallel to Experiment 1, stimuli for the maintenance phase consisted of a 6 × 6 grid that covered the critical region of the stimulus space;

Table 2 Stimulus details in Experiment 2. Distribution ranges, means (μ) and standard deviations (σ) are given for both spectral bands (S1, S2). δ' is calculated on the basis of distribution means and standard deviations. For rule-based (RB) optimal boundaries, δ' can be calculated for both dimensions, while for information integration (II) optimal boundaries, δ' can be calculated only for the integrated dimension and is based on the Euclidean distance between the distribution means in the two-dimensional space

Participants and procedure

Thirty-six native speakers of German (all right-handed) participated in Experiment 2 (19 males; mean age 25.14, SD 4.02); participants were drawn from the same pool as in Experiment 1, although none of the participants had been recruited for Experiment 1. As before, participants were assigned to either the rising (11 males; mean age 24.67, SD 3.09) or falling stimulus (8 males; mean age 25.61, SD 4.82) distribution.

Participants received monetary compensation for their participation. No participant reported hearing problems. The procedure was identical to that in Experiment 1.

Results

Accuracy results

Accuracy as measured by d' differed significantly from chance (d' = 1.33, SD = 0.56), t(35) = 19.78, p < .001. As in Experiment 1, learning was assessed in a mixed-measures ANOVA on d' with block and distribution as independent variables. The main effect of block was significant, F(5.5, 188) = 2.09, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.060 \), p < .05, indicating that performance increased over time. There was also a significant main effect of distribution, F(1, 34) = 35.03, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.507 \), p < .001, with participants in the falling distribution condition (d' = 1.61) outperforming participants in the rising distribution condition (d' = 1.04). Learning rate, however, did not depend on distribution, as indicated by a nonsignificant block × distribution interaction, F(5.5, 188) = 0.50, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.014 \), p = .84. The accuracy results are illustrated in Fig. 4.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Performance measure per block in the learning phase of Experiment 2. Whiskers indicate across-participants standard errors of the means

Logistic regression

Logistic regression analyses were conducted as in Experiment 1, which predicted the likelihood of a category A response from S1 and S2 separately for the learning and maintenance phases.

Learning phase

The model comprised the regressors S1 and S2, the factors block and distribution (rising, falling), and the interactions S1 × S2, S1 × block, S2 × block, S1 × distribution, and S2 × distribution. Notably, there was a main effect of S1, β = −1.38, z = −5.60, p < .001; that is, S1 values per trial significantly predicted category judgments. The significant block main effect, β = 0.33, z = −3.23, p < .01, and the S1 × block interaction, β = −0.04, z = −4.32, p < .001, together indicated that category A responses could be increasingly better predicted over the course of the experiment, especially on the basis of S1. Furthermore, category A responses were generally predicted better in the falling than in the rising distribution, β = 9.41, z = 20.41, p < .001. The distribution factor furthermore interacted with both S1,β = −0.50, z = −9.00, p < .01, and S2, β = −0.59, z = −18.23, p < .001, reflecting larger S1 and S2 effects for the falling than for the rising distribution. Finally, there was a significant interaction of S1 and S2, β = 0.60, z = 2.75, p < .01.

Maintenance phase

In the model without a block factor, both S1, β = −2.29, z = −7.37, p < .001, and S2, β = −0.54, z = −3.69, p < .01, were significant and interacted with distribution (S1 × distribution, β = −0.96, z = −8.88, p < .001; S2 × distribution, β = −0.44, z = −6.32, p < .001. S1 and S2 were significant predictors for category A responses, and more so for the falling than for the rising distribution. In general, category A responses were predicted better in the falling than in the rising distribution, β = 10.07, z = 9.26, p < .001. Finally, as in the learning phase, the S1 × S2 interaction was significant, β = 0.72, z = 4.79, p < .001.

Learning versus maintenance phase

The absolute values of the single-subject β-weights (from the same models as those reported in Experiment 1) were used as the dependent variable in an ANOVA with the factors phase (learning, maintenance), distribution (rising, falling), and filter (S1, S2). The ANOVA revealed main effects of filter, F(1, 102) = 184.21, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.644 \), p < .001, phase, F(1, 102) = 28.66, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.219 \), p < .001, and distribution, F(1, 34) = 29.23, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.462 \), p < .001. Weights were higher for S1 than for S2 (1.77, SD = 1.06, vs. 0.47, SD = 0.37), and higher in the maintenance phase than in the learning phase (1.38, SD = 1.26, vs. 0.86, SD = 0.64). β-weights were larger in the falling distribution than in the rising distribution (1.44, SD = 1.21 vs. 0.80, SD = 0.67. Furthermore, the filter × distribution, F(1, 102) = 13.85, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.119 \), p < .001, phase × distribution, F(1, 102) = 7.90, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.072 \), p < .01, and filter × phase, F(1, 102) = 13.86, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.120 \), p < .001, interactions reached significance, reflecting larger β differences between S1 and S2 in the falling (2.27, S = 1.19, vs. 0.61, SD = 0.38) than in the rising (1.28, SD = 0.60, vs. 0.33, SD = 0.30) distribution and in the maintenance (2.21, SD = 1.26, vs. 0.55, SD = 0.43) than in the learning (1.34, SD = 0.55, vs. 0.39, SD = 0.28) phase. Importantly, β differences between the maintenance and learning phases were larger for the falling (1.85, SD = 1.50, vs. 1.04, SD = 0.65) than for the rising (0.93, SD = 0.73, vs. 0.68, SD = 0.59) distribution. The three-way filter × distribution × phase interaction was significant as well, F(1, 102) = 10.02, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.090 \), p < .01, motivating separate analyses for the learning and the maintenance phases.

These analyses showed a significant filter × distribution interaction in the maintenance phase, F(1, 34) = 15.07, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.307 \), p < .01, but not in the learning phase, F(1, 34) = 0.31, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.009 \), p = .58. In order to visualize the degree to which participants relied on the individual dimensions, S1 and S2, we plotted βs for S1 (abscissa) against βs for S2 (ordinate) in the learning and in the maintenance phases, separately for the rising and the falling distributions (Fig. 5a). The plots illustrate that βs were larger in the maintenance than in the learning phase and also larger in the falling than in the rising distribution.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Illustration of strategy use in Experiment 2. a Scatterplots of β weights (per participant) for S1 and S2, for the rising and falling distributions in the learning phase (left) and in the maintenance phase (right). Participants are coded as follows: blue diamonds, both S1 βs and S2 βs significantly differ from zero; red squares, only S1 βs significantly differ from zero; green triangles, only S2 βs significantly differ from zero; purple circles, none of the βs significantly differ from zero. The diagonals illustrate the location of βs that would correspond equally to an S1- and S2-based response. The percentage of significant βs (p < .05) for the dimensions S1 and S2 and the combination of S1 and S2 are illustrated below the scatterplots. Significant proportion differences are indicated with an asterisk. b Left: Proportions of decision bound models used to categorize stimuli from the rising (left) and falling (right) distributions. Block numbers marked with an asterisk indicate a significant difference in the proportion of participants using an S1 versus an information integration rule. Right: Percentage of rule-based (S1 and S2) and information integration models whose Bayes factors exceeded 3

Notably, more participants used both dimensions, S1 and S2, in the falling than in the rising distribution, as reflected by significant differences in proportions of significant βs from the single-subject logistic regressions, χ 2 = 8.86, p(FDR) < .05 (Fig. 5a, right). This distinction was more pronounced in the learning than in the maintenance phase, where the proportions did not differ, χ 2 < 2, n.s.. In the same vein, participants used S2 more often in the falling than in the rising distribution of the learning phase, χ 2 = 7.26, p(FDR) < .05, while again, proportions did not differ in the maintenance phase, χ 2 < 2, n.s.

Modeling results

As in Experiment 1, three families of decision bound models (e.g., Ashby & Gott, 1988; Maddox & Ashby, 1993) were fit to the learning-phase data—that is, unidimensional rule-based (S1, S2), information integration, and random-response models (Fig. 5b, left). Again, we calculated Bayes factors for each model fit. In contrast to Experiment 1, the proportion of models that received substantial evidence differed significantly between the falling and the rising distributions (Fig. 5b, right) all χ 2s > 3, p(FDR) < .05. All winning models had Bayes factors of >3.

The proportions of participants fit best by each model are shown in Fig. 5b (left). Overall, participants most often adopted a unidimensional rule-based strategy based on S1, as in Experiment 1. However, strategy use crucially differed between the rising and falling distribution conditions. For the rising distribution, the unidimensional S1 rule was used in the majority of cases (six of eight blocks; ps < .05, FDR-corrected). In the falling condition, participants used an information integration strategy as often as the rule-based (S1) strategy in all eight blocks. Thus, participants trained on the falling distributions were more likely to adopt the optimal strategy that involved integrating S1 and S2 information before making a category membership decision.

Convergence of logistic regression and decision bound models

As before, the convergence of β-weights and BIC scores was assessed in two ANOVAs. The first ANOVA comprised the dependent measure rule-based S1 BIC score and the independent variables S1-β-weight and distribution. Importantly, there was a main effect of β-weight, F(1, 32) = 130.53, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.803 \), p < .001, reflecting a negative correlation between S1-βs and RB S1 BIC scores, and a main effect of distribution, F(1, 32) = 20.82, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.394 \), p < .001, showing lower BIC scores (better fits) for the falling than for the rising distribution.

The β-weight × distribution interaction, F(1, 32) = 6.73, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.803 \), p < .05, indicated a stronger β–BIC score correlation in the falling than in the rising condition.

The second ANOVA comprised the dependent measure RB S2 BIC score and the independent variables S2-β and distribution and showed an effect of β-weight, F(1, 32) = 133.67, \( \eta_{\mathrm{p}}^2=.806 \), p < .001, as well as an effect of distribution, F(1, 32) = 11.23, η 2 = .260, p < .01, but no β-weight × distribution interaction, F(1, 32) = 1.14, η 2 = .034, p = .29. Again, as in Experiment 1, β-weights and BIC scores converged.

Prediction of performance by decision bound models

In order to directly assess the performance benefit of using an information integration strategy, we carried out a correlation analysis between the proportions of rule-based S1 and information integration strategy use and d'. Notably, there was a positive correlation of proportion of information integration use and d', r = .48, t = 3.20, p < .01, suggesting that using an information integration strategy was indeed beneficial for performance. By contrast, the correlation of proportion of rule-based S1 use and d' was negative, r = −0.23, t = −1.39, n.s.; that is, participants using a rule-based S1 strategy tended to perform worse.

Discussion

The important result of Experiment 2 is that, as compared to Experiment 1, more participants used an information integration strategy, and more so in the falling than in the rising distribution condition. Generally, participants who were more likely to use information integration performed better than those who were more likely to focus on an S1 rule-based strategy.

Intriguingly, despite being disadvantageous, participants still used the S1 dimension to a high degree, as evidenced by both logistic regressions and decision bound models. That is, although Experiment 2 examined classification performance for auditory stimuli that were optimally separated by an information integration (diagonal) decision bound, participants still frequently used a rule-based strategy based on S1 (cf. Goudbeek et al., 2009).

Crucially, the use of the optimal information integration strategy depended on whether stimulus distributions were rising or falling (see Fig. 1b). The falling condition was associated more strongly with use of an information integration decision strategy, and the resulting performance was shown to be better for individuals adopting an information integration strategy. This result was predicted, since we hypothesized that familiarity with negative acoustic (here, spectral) correlations between dimensions would promote their integration.

Finally, a comparison of β-weights for the learning and maintenance phases suggested that the correlation of the stimulus dimensions in the learning phase, as well as immediate feedback, promoted the use of both S1 and S2 dimensions for categorization. This is consistent with previous findings in vision research (cf. Maddox et al., 2003). Our analyses suggested that information integration is characterized by an equal usage of S1 and S2 and that it was present in the learning phase, but not in the maintenance phase. There, without an S1–S2 correlation and without feedback, we observed a significant shift to an almost exclusive rule-based use of S1.

General discussion

Two experiments examined auditory category formation for stimuli varying along two spectral dimensions (i.e., S1 and S2), which exhibited either positive or negative correlations. Decision bound modeling and logistic regressions yielded three main results: (1) better information integration for negative spectral correlations, (2) tendency toward rule-based S1 categorization overall, and (3) dependency of immediate corrective feedback for information integration.

Promotion of information integration by negative correlations

The most important result of the two experiments is that the use of information integration (confined to Experiment 2) depended on the nature of the correlation between dimensions: Negative correlations promoted information integration, while positive correlations inhibited it. Overall, information integration in Experiment 2 predicted better performance.

Regarding the special status of negative acoustic correlations, our study extends the phonetic work by Kingston and colleagues (Kingston et al., 2008; Kingston & Macmillan, 1995). Kingston and colleagues characterized the interaction of fundamental frequency (f0, pitch) and first resonance frequency (F1) in the human oral cavity, as well as the interaction of F1 and nasalization (i.e., the resonance frequencies in the human nasal cavity) in speech vowel and nonspeech vowel-like sounds, and observed that both the f0–F1 and F1–nasalization relations approximate a negative correlation. With respect to nasalization, vowels with a higher degree of nasalization tend to have lower F1 frequencies (Diehl et al., 1990). Kingston and colleagues demonstrated that both of the discussed negative correlations (f0–F1 and F1–nasalization), in comparison with their positive counterparts, led to better categorization performance for speech and nonspeech stimuli (Kingston et al., 2008; Kingston & Macmillan, 1995). On the basis of these findings, we suggest that there is a general inclination toward encountering negative acoustic correlations in speech (resulting from articulatory bases as discussed in Carre, 2009), possibly shaping the learning of nonspeech stimuli with similar negative correlations.

Preference of rule-based strategies

In both experiments, participants predominantly based their categorization on the first spectral filter, S1. This inclination, which is in line with previous research (Goudbeek et al., 2009; Maddox et al., 2006), seems remarkable in the light of Experiment 2, where rule-based categorization was clearly suboptimal and where participants using this strategy performed worse than those employing information integration. In general, this inclination may have a neural explanation: Previous neuroimaging studies on visual categorization have shown that cortico–striatal connections are vital for information integration (Ashby & Ell, 2001; Ashby & Ennis, 2006; Ashby & Spiering, 2004; Helie, Roeder, & Ashby, 2010; Nomura et al., 2007; Seger, 2008). Moreover, cortico–striatal connections between the auditory cortex and the caudate have been argued to be more diffuse than cortico–striatal connections between the visual cortex and the caudate (Maddox et al., 2006). Thus, information integration may be relatively less likely in auditory categorization than in vision, due to anatomical constraints.

A second possible explanation for the reliance on rule-based strategies is developmental in nature. We have argued that information integration depends on acoustic correlations familiar from speech; thus, speech itself should presumably be acquired predominantly by information integration. A potential bias toward information integration learning in early life is related to the observation that brain structures such as the prefrontal and medial cortices that support rule-based learning (Gabrieli, Brewer, Desmond, & Glover, 1997; Schacter & Wagner, 1999) develop relatively late (Diamond, 2002). As a result, rule-based learning in early life does not compete with information integration learning as strongly as in adolescence or adulthood (Huang-Pollock, Maddox, & Karalunas, 2011).

The preference for using specifically the S1 dimension during categorization may reflect that pitch changes (presumably, the perceptual dimension conveyed by S1 variation; Remez & Rubin, 1993) are easier to verbalize than timbre changes (as potentially reflected by S2; cf. Smits et al., 2006). That is, pitch changes can be easily verbally described as being “high” or “low,” while timbral differences are more difficult to label verbally. The easier-to-verbalize dimension (i.e., S1) was then not surprisingly more often used for rule-based categorization, which, by definition, is based on easy-to-verbalize rules (Ashby & Maddox, 2005; Ashby & Maddox, 2011).

The influence of immediate feedback

The third main finding from the present experiments concerns the comparison of the learning phase with the maintenance phase. First, the learning phase contained distributional information from which the correlations between dimensions could be extracted, while in the maintenance phase, no such information was available. Second, there was immediate feedback in the learning phase, but not in the maintenance phase. Previous research indicates that both of these aspects may contribute to the likelihood that information integration strategies are used for categorization. Research from vision provides evidence that immediate feedback is crucial for adopting an information integration strategy (cf. Ashby et al., 1999; Ashby & Waldron, 1999). Additionally, β-weights in the maintenance phase of both experiments were larger, and more so for S1 than for S2. The falling condition in Experiment 2 furthermore showed that even though both dimensions were used during learning, concomitant with information integration, participants reverted to using S1 in the maintenance phase.

Potential limitations of decision bound models

Decision bound models are not unequivocally accepted; they implicitly assume dissociations of multiple memory systems subserving rule-based and information integration learning. In particular, they assume that rule-based learning requires working memory, while information integration does not (Filoteo, Lauritzen, & Maddox, 2010). Challenging this claim, (Lewandowsky, Yang, Newell, & Kalish, 2012; Newell & Dunn, 2008; Newell, Dunn, & Kalish, 2010), showed that rule-based as well as information integration learning tax working memory.

Furthermore, decision bound models are not the only means by which auditory categorization can be modeled. On the one hand, prototype models (Rosch, 1973) assume that a novel stimulus is assigned to the category whose average or most representative member (i.e., the prototype) it is most similar to. Different formulations of prototype theory suggest that categorization decisions are also based in part on the spread of category members around the prototype (i.e., category variance; Nearey & Assmann, 1986). Exemplar models (Nosofsky, 1986), on the other hand, assume (in their most extreme formulation) that categorization involves comparing novel acoustic items with all previously encountered members of relevant existing categories and then making a category membership decision on the basis of the maximum of the summed similarities to the members of the relevant categories. Prototype and exemplar models have been extensively compared elsewhere (Nosofsky & Stanton, 2005; Tunney & Fernie, 2012) and have also found to be somewhat inferior to decision bound models (Smits et al., 2006).

The present data show that decision bound models provide a valuable tool for assessing the contribution of different acoustic dimensions to auditory categorization. Furthermore, since Experiment 2, in particular, provides converging results from logistic regressions and decision bound models regarding the use of specific stimulus dimensions for categorization, we are confident that decision bound models accurately account for participants’ response behavior.

Conclusions

In sum, the present experiments provided evidence that listeners tend to use a rule-based—that is, an explicit—hypothesis-testing approach when they are categorizing novel auditory stimuli. Our results further suggest that long-term experience with sound distributions characterized by a negative spectro–spectral (S1–S2) correlation shapes the categorization of novel auditory stimuli. This is in line with experiments on speech sound categorization where long-term experience with correlations among auditory dimensions could not be easily overridden by short-term exposure to contrasting dimension correlations (Idemaru & Holt, 2011). Even though this long-term experience promoted information integration strategies and better performance in auditory categorization, our experiments challenge the view that the most successful strategy is necessarily the one participants use most frequently.