A peripheral onset cue can briefly capture attention and facilitate responses to targets at the same location (Jonides, 1981). Later on, however, an inhibitory cueing effect (ICE) emerges at the cued location and delays target responses (Berlucchi, Di Stefano, Marzi, Morelli, & Tassinari, 1981; Cohen, 1981; Posner & Cohen, 1984). This later ICE was named “inhibition of return” (IOR) by Posner, Rafal, Choate, and Vaughan (1985), to reflect the theoretical proposition that, once attention has left a location, it is inhibited to return. As a theoretical construct implied in Posner et al. (1985), IOR entails both a cause and an effect: “In cause, IOR occurs in the aftermath of oculomotor activation” and “in effect, IOR is a long-lasting response bias that affects overt and covert orienting” (Hilchey, Klein, & Satel, 2014, p. 1604). For clarity, in the present article we will use the term ICE rather than IOR to describe cueing effects that may have been caused by IOR or, alternatively, by other mechanisms functionally similar to it.

Posner and Cohen (1984) originally suggested that the ICE evoked by peripheral cues was the result of sensory stimulation (i.e., an input-based effect). Posner et al. (1985), however, found that this ICE had no effect on the perceptual arrival time of targets (see also Klein, Schmidt, & Müller, 1998; Maylor, 1985) but, nevertheless, biased eye movements away (see also Clohessy, Posner, Rothbart, & Vecera, 1991). These findings suggest that the observed ICE may actually represent a motor bias against previously attended locations (i.e., an output-based effect). Subsequent studies demonstrated that many ICEs are closely linked with the oculomotor system (e.g., Dorris, Klein, Everling, & Munoz, 2002; Posner et al., 1985; Rafal, Calabresi, Brennan, & Sciolto, 1989; Sumner, Nachev, Vora, Husain, & Kennard, 2004) and can be readily evoked in cueing tasks that require eye movement responses (e.g., Chica, Taylor, Lupiáñez, & Klein, 2010; Klein & Hilchey, 2011).

Although efficient orienting of the eyes is crucial for visual selection, the skeletomotor system is what enables an individual to actually interact with objects in the external world. Briand, Larrison, and Sereno (2000) found that ICEs appeared later for manual than for saccadic responses (but see Khatoon, Briand, & Sereno, 2002). With S-cone stimuli that were invisible to the superior colliculus (at least along the retinotectal pathway), Sumner and colleagues (Sumner, 2006; Sumner et al., 2004) observed ICEs with manual but not with saccadic responses. Zhang and Zhang (2011) also found that loading up visual working memory interfered with ICEs measured with manual but not with saccadic responses. In addition, it has been shown that ICEs interact with the Simon effect (Ivanoff, Klein, & Lupiáñez, 2002) and that ICEs double in magnitude when a nonresponding hand is placed on the keyboard (Ivanoff & Klein, 2001). All of these findings suggest that the skeletomotor system may also play an important role in the generation and expression of ICEs. However, the use of peripheral onset cues makes it difficult to determine whether these ICEs were also contributed to by a sensory deficit at the cued location (e.g., Fecteau & Munoz, 2005; Müller & Kleinschmidt, 2007; Satel, Wang, Trappenberg, & Klein, 2011). With central arrow cues and targets, Cowper-Smith, Eskes, and Westwood (2013) observed slower reaching responses toward previously touched locations, providing clear evidence that skeletomotor activation also gives rise to output-based ICEs. Although such an ICE does not meet the theoretical definition of IOR (Hilchey et al., 2014; Posner et al., 1985), it dovetails with the observation of inhibitory tags in manual foraging (Thomas et al., 2006) and affords the function of biasing orienting toward novelty.

Manual reaching responses were required in Cowper-Smith et al.’s (2013) study. With manual buttonpresses, however, Taylor and Klein (2000) observed no ICE in a similar experimental setup. Since Fischer, Pratt, and Neggers (2003) also failed to observe an ICE when participants made reaching responses to central arrow targets, it seems unlikely that response modes were behind these conflicting findings. One other prominent methodological difference between these two studies was that the cue–target onset asynchrony (CTOA) was only 1,000 ms in Taylor and Klein’s study, whereas that in Cowper-Smith et al.’s was 2,800 ms. With central arrow targets, discrimination of the arrow direction is needed before a response can be issued. It is known that ICEs measured with discrimination responses take longer to emerge on a behavioral level (e.g., Lupiáñez, Milán, Tornay, Madrid, & Tudela, 1997). The primary purpose of the present study was to clarify whether Taylor and Klein’s (2000) failure to observe ICEs with manual button responses was due to their relatively short CTOA. It is critical to clear up this issue, because if ICEs can be evoked by skeletomotor activation, they should be observed when the skeletomotor system is actively engaged by either manual reaches (as in Cowper-Smith et al., 2013) or buttonpresses (as in Taylor & Klein, 2000).

As has been alluded to before, ICEs linked to oculomotor activation have been frequently reported in the literature (e.g., Abrams & Dobkin, 1994; Posner et al., 1985; Satel, Hilchey, Wang, Story, & Klein, 2013; Satel & Wang, 2012; Taylor & Klein, 2000; Wang, Satel, & Klein, 2012). Nevertheless, it remains unclear whether these oculomotor ICEs carry over to the skeletomotor system. With saccadic responses to the cue and manual responses to the target, Taylor and Klein (2000) observed ICEs of the same magnitude, regardless of whether the cue and target were central arrows or peripheral onsets. Fischer et al. (2003), however, observed ICEs with saccadic responses, but not with any type of manual response to central arrow targets, and they concluded that “motor-based IOR is restricted to the oculomotor system” (p. 379). This is quite surprising, given that neural activation in the oculomotor system certainly does carry over to the skeletomotor system (e.g., Werner, 1993). A secondary purpose of the present study was to reexamine this issue. Saccadic responses to central arrow cues have been shown to evoke an oculomotor ICE—that is, an ICE that satisfies the theoretical definition of IOR (Hilchey et al., 2014). If oculomotor ICEs do carry over to the skeletomotor system, a robust ICE would also be revealed with manual button responses to central arrow targets.

To achieve these goals, for the present study we adopted two cueing tasks. Manual button responses were required to central arrow targets in both tasks, whereas saccadic responses were required to central arrow cues in one task (saccadic–manual), and manual localization responses were required in the other (manual–manual). On the basis of previous results, we expected saccadic responses to the cues to evoke an output-based ICE in the oculomotor system (e.g., Chica et al., 2010; Satel & Wang, 2012; Taylor & Klein, 2000; Wang et al., 2012). Manual button responses to the cues might also evoke an ICE, but the results of Taylor and Klein (2000) suggest otherwise. In four experiments, ICEs were consistently observed with manual button responses to the cue—but only when the CTOA was relatively long (2,000 ms or longer). In addition, in these experiments we also examined the time courses of ICEs evoked by skeletomotor and oculomotor activation (Exp. 4) and the boundary conditions under which oculomotor ICEs carry over to the skeletomotor system (Exps. 13).

Experiment 1: visual cue-back signal

To examine ICEs evoked by manual and saccadic responses to arrow cues, we first replicated two of Taylor and Klein’s (2000) 24 cueing tasks, with the addition of a much longer CTOA. Nonpredictive central arrows were used as the cues, and manual localization responses were required to central arrow targets in both tasks. These two tasks were blocked—saccadic responses were made to the cues in the first task (saccadic–manual), whereas manual responses were made to the cues in the second task (manual–manual).

Method

Participants

In all experiments reported here, all participants reported normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity and had no motor, or neurological abnormalities. They were paid 40 Yuan per hour for their participation. Twenty-three volunteers participated in Experiment 1. One of these was excluded from the analysis because she did not finish the tasks. The mean age of the remaining 22 participants (17 females, five males) was 20.82 years.

Apparatus and stimuli

The stimuli were presented on a 17-in. CRT monitor, and the viewing distance was maintained at about 62 cm with a chinrest. Stimulus presentation and response registration were controlled with scripts written in Python. Eye movements were monitored with an EyeLink 1000 (SR Research) eyetracking system. The spatial resolution of the eyetracker was 0.2° or better, and the sampling rate was set to 500 Hz.

The stimuli were similar to those of Taylor and Klein (2000). Three gray placeholder boxes subtending 1.8° (visual angle) and were visible at all times. The center-to-center distance between two adjacent boxes was 9°. Both the cue and target stimuli were arrows presented in the central box, measuring 0.8° (width) by 0.2° (height).

Design and procedure

The sequence of events in both tasks is illustrated in Fig. 1. Self-paced drift correction was performed at the beginning of each trial, with successful drift correction signaled by a beep. Then a fixation cross appeared in the central box, and an error message was displayed if eye movements were detected during a 500-ms fixation period. The cue was an arrow presented for 300 ms in the central box, pointing either left or right. In the manual–manual task, participants maintained fixation throughout a trial and pressed the “Z” and “/” keys in response to left- and right-pointing arrow cues, respectively. Failure to respond within 500 ms, pressing the wrong key, or making an eye movement triggered an error message and the termination of the trial. In the saccadic–manual task, participants made saccades to the peripheral box indicated by the central arrow. If participants failed to initiate an eye movement within 500 ms, or if the eyes missed the center of the appropriate peripheral box by more than 2.5°, an error message was displayed and the trial was terminated. Five hundred milliseconds after cue onset, the central box was brightened for 300 ms (visual cue-back signal). Participants maintained fixation in the manual–manual task, whereas they made saccades back to the central box in the saccadic–manual task. Failure to maintain fixation in the manual–manual task, or failure to saccade back to the central box within 500 ms, would trigger an error message and the termination of the trial. Unlike Taylor and Klein (2000), who only tested a CTOA of 1,000 ms, in the present experiment we also tested a much longer CTOA (2,000 ms). These two CTOAs were intermixed within blocks of trials. So, 1,000 or 2,000 ms following cue onset, an arrow target appeared in the central box and participants quickly responded with the “Z” or the “/” key. An error message was displayed if saccades were detected before the target response or if the participant pressed the wrong key.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The display sequence in a sample trial. Participants always made manual localization responses to the target, but also made manual (in the manual–manual task) or saccadic (in the saccadic–manual task) responses to the cue. For illustration purposes, the stimuli are not drawn to scale

In the present experiment, we adopted a 2 (Task: manual–manual vs. saccadic–manual) × 2 (CTOA: 1,000 vs. 2,000 ms) × 2 (Cueing: cued vs. uncued) within-subjects design. Each experimental cell was tested for 32 trials, and thus each participant needed to successfully complete a total of 256 trials. The manual–manual and saccadic–manual tasks were blocked and counterbalanced across participants. Whenever an error message was displayed, the trial was discarded and later presented to the participants in a random order, until all trials had been completed successfully. The participants could take a break after every 80 trials, and a practice block of eight trials was provided at the beginning of each task.

Results

All statistical analyses were performed in R (R Development Core Team, 2015). The effect size measure reported for analyses of variance (ANOVAs) was generalized eta squared (η G 2; small size = .02, medium size = .13, and large size = .26; see Bakeman, 2005).

Target RTs

Only successfully completed trials were considered in the response time (RT) analysis. These RTs were cleansed on a per-participant, per-experimental-cell basis, using the nonrecursive method recommended by Van Selst and Jolicœur (1994). After outlier removal, 97.78 % and 97.22 % of the trials remained in the manual–manual and saccadic–manual tasks, respectively.

The mean target RTs in all conditions are presented in Table 1. A repeated measures ANOVA was performed, revealing significant main effects for task [F(1, 21) = 14.71, MSE = 3,749, p < .001, η G 2 = .11] and CTOA [F(1, 21) = 17.97, MSE = 954, p < .001, η G 2 = .04]. RTs were generally longer in the saccadic–manual task, and were longer in the short-CTOA conditions. The main effect of cueing did not reach significance [F(1, 21) = 2.80, MSE = 1,014, p = .11, η G 2 = .01], but a two-way interaction between cueing and CTOA was observed [F(1, 21) = 20.07, MSE = 96.6, p < .001, η G 2 = .005], suggesting that the magnitudes of ICEs differed across CTOAs. The two-way interaction between CTOA and task approached significance [F(1, 21) = 3.39, MSE = 553.9, p = .08, η G 2 = .001], whereas that between cueing and task did not reach significance [F(1, 21) = 1.55, MSE = 443.4, p = .23, η G 2 = .002]. The three-way interaction between task, cueing, and CTOA was not significant [F(1, 21) = 2.13, MSE = 130.2, p = .16, η G 2 = .001].

Table 1 Mean target response times (RTs), error rates, and RT differences between cued and uncued conditions (ICEs) in the manual–manual and saccadic–manual tasks of Experiments 14

Taylor and Klein’s (2000) empirical findings and their theory predicted no ICE in the manual–manual task and a robust ICE in the saccadic–manual task. Thus, planned comparisons were performed to evaluate the ICEs in all conditions, even though the three-way interaction did not reach significance. A reliable ICE (21 ms) was observed in the manual–manual task when the CTOA was 2,000 ms [t(21) = 4.06, p < .001], but the ICEs in all other conditions did not reach significance [all ts < 1.08, all ps > .29] (see Fig. 2a).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Inhibitory cueing effects (ICEs) observed in all conditions of (a) Experiment 1 (visual cue-back), (b) Experiment 2 (no cue-back), (c) Experiment 3 (auditory cue-back), and (d) Experiment 4 (time course). Error bars denote ±1 SEM. * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001

Target response errors

Trials with erroneous eye movements or responses were aborted and recycled. Most recycled trials were terminated before target presentation; only those recycled due to incorrect target responses were considered in the response error analysis. Target response errors are presented in Table 1. Analyses revealed a marginal main effect for cueing [F(1, 21) = 3.80, MSE = 17.47, p = .06, η G 2 = .04], with higher error rates observed in the cued conditions. A significant two-way interaction emerged between task and CTOA [F(1, 21) = 15.86, MSE = 3.58, p < .001, η G 2 = .04], because the error rate decreased as the CTOA increased in the saccadic–manual task. The two-way interaction between cueing and task was marginally significant [F(1, 21) = 4.09, MSE = 10.78, p = .06, η G 2 = .03]; the error rate was higher in the cued condition, but only in the manual–manual task. All other effects did not reach significance [all Fs < 2.64, all ps > .11].

Discussion

In the manual–manual task, an ICE was observed for the long, but not the short, CTOA, suggesting that their relatively short CTOA (1,000 ms) might be the reason for Taylor and Klein’s (2000) failure to observe an ICE in this task. Although Taylor and Klein (2000) observed reliable ICEs in their saccadic–manual tasks, no ICE was observed in the present experiment, regardless of the CTOA. Note that, in Taylor and Klein’s study, central arrow cues and peripheral onset cues were intermixed within blocks of trials. It is possible that this unique manipulation may have created an attentional set that gave rise to the ICEs observed in their saccadic–manual condition.

To closely replicate Taylor and Klein’s (2000) design, we also used a visual cue-back signal to encourage the participants to orient attention back to the central fixation. In the saccadic–manual task, this cue-back signal stimulated the retinal locus corresponding to the uncued box, and thus may have invoked inhibition there (Wang et al., 2012). Although it is unclear whether this retinal stimulation could delay responses toward the uncued box, we eliminated this potential methodological confound in subsequent experiments.

Experiment 2: no cue-back signal

Experiment 2 was similar to Experiment 1, except that the visual cue-back signal was eliminated.

Method

Participants

Twenty-two volunteers (14 females, eight males; mean age: 22.42 years) participated in Experiment 2.

Apparatus, task procedure, and design

The apparatus, task procedure, and design were identical to those of Experiment 1, except that the visual cue-back signal was eliminated and, in the saccadic–manual task, the participants were instructed to quickly saccade back to the central box, immediately after the eyes had reached the peripheral box indicated by the cue. As in Experiment 1, any incorrect eye movements or responses resulted in trial termination and recycling.

Results

Target RTs

The RTs from successfully completed trials were cleansed using the same protocol as in Experiment 1. After data cleaning, 97.80 % and 96.86 % of the trials remained in the manual–manual and saccadic–manual tasks, respectively.

The mean target RTs are presented in Table 1. A repeated measures ANOVA revealed significant main effects for task [F(1, 21) = 52.49, MSE = 2,964, p < .001, η G 2 = .29] and CTOA [F(1, 21) = 82.43, MSE = 518, p < .001, η G 2 = .1]. RTs were generally longer in the saccadic–manual task, and were longer for the short-CTOA conditions. The main effect of cueing was marginally significant [F(1, 21) = 3.15, MSE = 728.2, p = .09, η G 2 = .01]. Significant two-way interactions occurred between cueing and CTOA [F(1, 21) = 8.71, MSE = 211.3, p < .01, η G 2 = .005], due to a larger ICE in the longer-CTOA condition, and between task and CTOA [F(1, 21) = 24.65, MSE = 748, p < .001, η G 2 = .05], because the speeding up of RTs at the long CTOA was greater for the saccadic–manual task. The two-way interaction between cueing and task was not significant [F(1, 21) = 0.24, MSE = 263.24, p = .63, η G 2 = .00], whereas the three-way interaction was [F(1, 21) = 8.16, MSE = 229.3, p < .01, η G 2 = .004]. Planned comparisons revealed an ICE (21 ms) for the manual–manual task when the CTOA was 2,000 ms [t(21) = 4.16, p < .001]. The ICEs in all other conditions did not reach significance [all ts < 0.96, all ps > .35] (see Fig. 2b).

Target response errors

Response error rates are also presented in Table 1. A repeated measures ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of cueing [F(1, 21) = 6.33, MSE = 8.10, p = .02, η G 2 = .02], with more errors occurring in uncued conditions. A significant two-way interaction between task and CTOA was also observed [F(1, 21) = 8.83, MSE = 3, p < .01, η G 2 = .03], because the error rate decreased as the CTOA increased only in the saccadic–manual task. All other effects did not reach significance [all Fs < 1.31, all ps > .26].

Discussion

The results of Experiment 2 were consistent with those of Experiment 1 (see Fig. 2a and b).Footnote 1 In the manual–manual task, an ICE was observed only at the long CTOA; in the saccadic–manual task, no ICE was observed at either CTOA. Our failure to observe an ICE in the saccadic–manual task was in drastic contrast to previous observations of ICEs in similar tasks (e.g., Posner et al., 1985; Taylor & Klein, 2000). However, since previous work has demonstrated the importance of the cue-back signal in revealing ICEs (e.g., Pratt & Fischer, 2002; Prime & Jolicœur, 2009; but see Possamaï, 1991), it is possible that we failed to observe ICEs in the saccadic–manual task because Experiment 2 did not include cue-back signals. This possibility was explored in Experiment 3 through the use of an auditory cue-back signal. With auditory cue-back signals, Satel and Wang (2012) observed robust ICE in a similar experimental setup.

Experiment 3: auditory cue-back signal

Experiment 3 followed the same design as Experiments 1 and 2, except that an auditory cue-back signal was used to reorient attention to the central fixation following responses to the cues.

Method

Participants

A total of 16 participants (15 females, one male; mean age: 21.2 years) took part in Experiment 3.

Apparatus, task procedure, and design

The apparatus, stimuli, and task procedure were the same as in Experiment 1, except that (a) the visual cue-back signal was replaced by an auditory beep, and (b) the short CTOA was extended to 1,200 ms, so as to allocate enough time for participants to respond to the cue-back signal.

Results

Target RTs

After data cleansing, 97.70 % and 97.13 % of the successfully completed trials remained in the manual–manual and saccadic–manual tasks, respectively.

The mean target RTs in all conditions are presented in Table 1. An ANOVA of the RTs revealed significant main effects for task [F(1, 15) = 75.68, MSE = 2,472, p < .001, η G 2 = .42], CTOA [F(1, 15) = 21.82, MSE = 985, p < .001, η G 2 = .08], and cueing [F(1, 15) = 5.16, MSE = 1,353, p < .05, η G 2 = .03]. The main effect of cueing emerged because RTs were generally longer for cued than for uncued targets, suggesting the observation of an overall ICE. The main effects of task and CTOA were due to faster responses in the manual–manual condition and at the long CTOA. A significant two-way interaction was observed between CTOA and task [F(1, 15) = 24.47, MSE = 514, p < .001, η G 2 = .05], because there was a greater reduction in RTs at the longer CTOA in the saccadic–manual task. The two-way interactions between cueing and CTOA [F(1, 15) = 3.25, MSE = 247.2, p = .09, η G 2 = .003] and cueing and task [F(1, 15) = 3.31, MSE = 227.1, p = .09, η G 2 = .003] approached significance. The three-way interaction between task, cueing, and CTOA did not reach significance [F(1, 15) = 2.87, MSE = 372.8, p = .11, η G 2 = .004]. Planned comparisons revealed that, consistent with Experiments 1 and 2, in the manual–manual task an ICE (21 ms) was observed only at the long CTOA (2,000 ms) [t(15) = 2.39, p < .05]. However, with auditory cue-back signals, ICEs emerged in the saccadic–manual task at both short (21 ms) [t(15) = 2.23, p < .05] and long (19 ms) [t(15) = 2.20, p = .05] CTOAs.

Target response errors

Response error rates are also presented in Table 1. A repeated measures ANOVA revealed marginal main effects for task [F(1, 15) = 4.40, MSE = 14.01, p = .05, η G 2 = .03], and cueing [F(1, 15) = 4.34, MSE = 7.39, p = .05, η G 2 = .01]. Error rates were higher in the manual–manual task and in cued conditions. A marginal two-way interaction was observed between task and cueing [F(1, 15) = 3.97, MSE = 21.93, p = .06, η G 2 = .04], because the effect of cueing was more prominent in the manual–manual task. The three-way interaction also reached marginal significance [F(1, 15) = 3.84, MSE = 12.51, p = .07, η G 2 = .02], because the error rate increased with CTOA in the manual–manual task, whereas it decreased with CTOA in the saccadic–manual task. All other effects did not reach significance [all Fs < 2.06, all ps > .17].

Discussion

Experiment 3 produced two important findings. First, with an auditory cue-back signal, reliable ICEs were observed in the saccadic–manual task. This was in contrast with the two previous experiments, in which no ICE was observed with a visual cue back (Exp. 1) or without any cue back (Exp. 2). It thus seems that, although making a saccadic response to a cue is sufficient to evoke an ICE (e.g., Posner et al., 1985; Rafal et al., 1989; Satel & Wang, 2012; Taylor & Klein, 2000), the ICE will only delay manual responses under certain conditions. Second, a reliable ICE was again observed only at the long CTOA in the manual–manual task.

Experiment 4: time course

The findings of Experiments 13 clearly show that (a) ICEs can be evoked and revealed with arrow cues/targets and manual localization responses, although only with sufficiently long time intervals between the cue and target, and (b) ICEs evoked by saccadic responses to arrow cues do carry over to the skeletomotor system to delay manual responses. To further understand these ICEs, it is necessary to characterize their time courses.

Method

Participants

Seventeen volunteers participated in Experiment 4. One of them was excluded from the analysis because she did not finish the tasks. The mean age of the remaining 16 participants (13 females, three males) was 19.38 years.

Apparatus, task procedure, and design

The apparatus, task procedure, and design were the same as in Experiment 3, except that three CTOAs (1,200, 2,000, and 3,000 ms), rather than two, were tested.

Results

Target RTs

After cleansing, 97.67 % and 97.17 % of the successfully completed trials remained in the manual–manual and saccadic–manual tasks, respectively.

The mean target RTs in all conditions are presented in Table 1. A repeated measures ANOVA on the RTs revealed significant main effects for task [F(1, 15) = 52.44, MSE = 1,554, p < .001, η G 2 = .17], CTOA [F(2, 30) = 40.49, MSE = 687, p < .001, η G 2 = .12], and cueing [F(1, 15) = 14.36, MSE = 1,187, p < .001, η G 2 = .04]. The RTs were generally longer in the saccadic–manual task and for cued targets, and decreased as the CTOA increased. A significant two-way interaction was observed between task and CTOA [F(2, 30) = 23.91, MSE = 710, p < .001, η G 2 = .08], due to a greater reduction in RTs as CTOA increased in the saccadic–manual task. The two-way interactions between cueing and CTOA [F(1, 15) = 0.03, MSE = 123.52, p = .97, η G 2 = .00] and cueing and task [F(1, 15) = 0.49, MSE = 285, p = .49, η G 2 = .00] were not significant. The three-way interaction between task, CTOA, and cueing was significant [F(2, 30) = 6.15, MSE = 218.5, p < .01, η G 2 = .006], because the ICEs increased with CTOA in the manual–manual task, whereas they decreased in the saccadic–manual task (see Fig. 2d). Planned comparisons revealed that, in the manual–manual task, ICEs were observed for the 3,000-ms (26 ms) [t(15) = 4.35, p < .01] and 2,000-ms (16 ms) [t(15) = 2.54, p < .05] CTOAs, but not for the 1,200-ms CTOA [t(15) = 1.28, p = .22]. In the saccadic–manual task, ICEs were observed for the 1,200-ms (29 ms) [t(15) = 6.71, p < .001] and 2,000-ms (22 ms) [t(15) = 3.25, p < .01] CTOAs, but not for the 3,000-ms CTOA [t(15) = 1.16, p = .26].

As is clear from Fig. 2d, the ICE generally decreased as the CTOA increased in the saccadic–manual task, much like the ICEs observed in the classic cueing paradigm (e.g., Fecteau & Munoz, 2005; Samuel & Kat, 2003). Confirming this trend, an ANOVA on the ICEs in the saccadic–manual task revealed a significant main effect of CTOA [F(2, 30) = 4.15, MSE = 354.4, p < .05, η G 2 = .03]. Pairwise comparisons revealed a significant difference in ICEs between the 1,200-ms and 3,000-ms CTOAs [t(15) = 2.29, p < .05], and a marginally significant difference between the 2,000-ms and 3,000-ms CTOAs [t(15) = 2.09, p = .05]; the difference between the 1,200-ms and 2,000-ms CTOAs did not reach significance [t(15) = 1.25, p = .22]. The manual–manual task, however, produced an ICE that started later and had no sign of decreasing at the longest CTOA tested in the present experiment (3,000 ms). An ANOVA of the ICEs in this task also revealed a significant effect of CTOA [F(2, 30) = 3.72, MSE = 329.7, p < .05, η G 2 = .04]. Pairwise comparisons revealed only a significant difference in ICE between the 1,200- and 3,000-ms CTOAs [t(15) = 2.71, p < .05]; the differences between the 1,200- and 2,000-ms CTOAs [t(15) = 1.28, p = .22] and the 2,000- and 3,000-ms CTOAs [t(15) = 1.44, p = .17] did not reach significance.

Target response errors

Response error rates are presented in Table 1. An ANOVA revealed only a significant two-way interaction between task and CTOA [F(2, 30) = 3.49, MSE = 8.70, p < .05, η G 2 = .02]. Again, error rates appeared to increase with CTOA in the manual–manual task, but to decrease with CTOA in the saccadic–manual task. All other effects did not reach significance [Fs < 2.07, ps > .14].

Discussion

In Experiment 4, we examined the time courses of ICEs evoked by saccadic and manual responses. As is shown in Fig. 2d, the ICE in the saccadic–manual task generally decreased as the CTOA increased, whereas that in the manual–manual task had a trend to increase with CTOA. It is unclear why this pattern of results emerged, but it is likely that the ICE in the manual–manual tasks originates from neurodynamic mechanisms that are distinct from the oculomotor IOR effects evoked by saccadic cue responses.

General discussion

With two cueing tasks, in the present study we examined output-based ICEs evoked by saccadic and manual localization responses to arrow cues. ICEs were consistently evoked and revealed with manual responses to arrow cues and targets in all experiments—but only when the CTOA was 2,000 ms or longer. Following saccadic responses to arrow cues, ICEs were also observed, but only when an auditory cue-back signal was supplied to reorient the participant’s gaze to the central fixation (Exps. 34). Furthermore, the present experiments show that the magnitudes of the ICEs evoked by saccadic responses generally decreased as the CTOA increased, mimicking the pattern of ICEs observed with peripheral onset cues (Samuel & Kat, 2003). The ICEs evoked by manual responses to the cue, however, appeared later in time and did not decrease even after 3 s had elapsed since cue onset (Exp. 4).

In the present experiments, the cues and targets were both arrows at fixation. One might suggest that the RT cost observed for targets pointing in the same direction as the cue might well be a shape-based, nonspatial repetition disadvantage (Fox & de Fockert, 2001; Riggio, Patteri, & Umiltà, 2003). We believe that this is unlikely for several reasons. First, the visual and auditory cue-back signals could have functioned as the “neutral attractors” that are critical for nonspatial repetition disadvantages (Fox & de Fockert, 2001; Law, Pratt, & Abrams, 1995; Taylor & Klein, 1998). However, ICEs were not always observed when these neutral attractors were present (Exps. 1, 3, and 4). Second, the ICEs observed in the present experiments cannot be attributed solely to a repetition disadvantage, because arrows can reflexively orient attention (e.g., Hommel, Pratt, Colzato, & Godijn, 2001; Stevens, West, Al-Aidroos, Weger, & Pratt, 2008). Third, and most importantly, in Fox and de Fockert (2001) and other studies of nonspatial ICEs (e.g., Hu & Samuel, 2011; Kwak & Egeth, 1992; Law et al., 1995; Taylor & Klein, 1998) the required detection or discrimination responses entailed no spatial information, whereas in the present work, the arrow cues and targets both required spatial localization responses. Thus, the ICEs reported here cannot be regarded as nonspatial.

The most important finding of the present study was that a robust ICE was observed when simple manual localization responses were made to central arrow cues. This ICE may arise from habituation of overt orienting responses (Dukewich, 2009), or it may be “merely an epiphenomenon arising from neural adaptation within motor control networks” (Cowper-Smith et al., 2013). Regardless of the underlying mechanism(s), the available evidence seems to suggest that this ICE is linked to skeletomotor activation. This ICE does not depend on the type of manual response required—it can be evoked and revealed with either simple buttonpresses (present experiments) or reaching responses (Cowper-Smith et al., 2013). Also, and more importantly, this ICE takes a longer time to emerge on a behavioral level than does “traditional IOR,” and its magnitude does not seem to decrease even after 3 s have elapsed since cue onset. This unique time course is in dramatic contrast to those of other ICEs that have been reported in the literature (Klein, 2000, 2004; Samuel & Kat, 2003; Wang et al., 2012). It is worth noting that the ICE evoked by oculomotor activation, as revealed by the saccadic–manual task, is largely dispersed 3 s after cue onset. These observations dovetail with the finding that, in visual search tasks, IOR (or inhibitory) tags at manually searched locations (Thomas et al., 2006) last longer than those at previously fixated locations (Dodd, Van der Stigchel, & Hollingworth, 2009), implying that the oculomotor and skeletomotor systems are responsible for relatively fast and slow overt orienting, respectively.

Since the discovery of IOR, various ICEs have been shown to be closely tied to the oculomotor system (e.g., Dorris et al., 2002; Posner et al., 1985; Sapir, Soroker, Berger, & Henik, 1999; Satel et al., 2011; Wang, Satel, Trappenberg, & Klein, 2011). The term “oculomotor IOR” has been used in several recent studies to stress the importance of oculomotor activation in the generation of ICEs (or IOR; e.g., Hilchey et al., 2014; Klein & Hilchey, 2011; Wang et al., 2012). In the present experiment, we explored whether oculomotor ICEs, when evoked by saccadic responses to central arrow cues, carry over to the skeletomotor system to delay manual responses. No ICE was observed when a visual (Exp. 1) or no (Exp. 2) cue-back signal was used in the saccadic–manual task. When an auditory cue-back signal was used in Experiments 3 and 4, however, ICEs were observed in the saccadic–manual task at both short and long CTOAs. This pattern of results suggests that auditory cue-back signals are critical for oculomotor ICEs to transfer to the skeletomotor system.Footnote 2 However, given the methodological differences across experiments, this finding should not be overstated.

Conclusions

In the present study, we examined the output-based inhibitory cueing effects evoked by saccadic and manual localization responses to arrow cues. We showed that ICEs evoked by manual and saccadic responses to central arrow cues can both be revealed with manual target responses, and that the ICE evoked by manual cue responses has a unique time course. We conclude that ICEs linked to skeletomotor activation do exist and that oculomotor ICEs can carry over to the skeletomotor system to delay manual responses.