Skip to main content
Log in

Combining attention: a novel way of conceptualizing the links between attention, sensory processing, and behavior

  • Published:
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Many everyday behaviors appear to require both the interpretation of incoming sensory information and the maintenance of a current task goal. This intuitive notion suggests that combining attentional control processes might reflect a fundamentally novel way in which attention supports complex behavior. Using an established paradigm, here we show that joint recruitment in multiple attention control systems leads to corresponding combined increases in behavior and underlying sensory processing of attended targets. Moreover, our data also revealed that the nature of the combined effect depends on a flexible allocation of attentional resources to individual component processes, which change dynamically as a function of task demands. Together, these data provide a new conceptual framework for characterizing the role of attention in behavior and suggest important extensions to the prevailing theories of attention.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Participants also completed a condition in which the shape did not inform about the target location. Task-irrelevant shapes did not influence attention (RT & Accuracy: all Fs < 1), confirming that spatially predictive shape condition provided a measure of endogenous attention in isolation.

  2. Our results did not vary either as a function of target type (‘target’ vs ‘non-target’) or key assignment (‘b’/’h’ vs ‘h’/’b’ corresponding to target/non-target responses; RT & Accuracy: all Fs<2.5, ps > 0.12).

  3. We report analyses of the magnitudes of orienting in the easy task conditions for RT only due to the overall high response accuracy and a lack of meaningful difference scores across cue validity conditions in accuracy measures.

References

  • Anderson, B. A., Laurent, P. A., & Yantis, S. (2011a). Learned value magnifies salience-based attentional capture. PLoS ONE, 6(11), e27926.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, B. A., Laurent, P. A., & Yantis, S. (2011b). Value-driven attentional capture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(25), 10367–10371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Awh, E., Belopolsky, A. V., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(8), 437–443.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, A., Henik, A., & Rafal, R. (2005). Competition between endogenous and exogenous orienting of visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(2), 207–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carrasco, M. (2011). Visual attention: the past 25 years. Vision Research, 51(13), 1484–1525.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta, M., Patel, G., & Shulman, G. L. (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron, 58(3), 306–234.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, G. J., & Gibson, B. S. (2012). Going rogue in the spatial cuing paradigm: high spatial validity is insufficient to elicit voluntary shifts of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(5), 1192–1201.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DiQuattro, N. E., Sawaki, R., & Geng, J. J. (2013). Effective connectivity during feature-based attentional capture: evidence against the attentional reorienting hypothesis of TPJ. Cerebral Cortex. doi:10.1093/cercor/bht1172

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eimer, M. (1997). Uninformative symbolic cues may bias visual-spatial attention: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Biological Psychology, 46(1), 67–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Friesen, C. K., Ristic, J., & Kingstone, A. (2004). Attentional effects of counterpredictive gaze and arrow cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30(2), 319–329.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkins, H. L., Hillyard, S. A., Luck, S. J., Mouloua, M., Downing, C. J., & Woodward, D. P. (1990). Visual attention modulates signal detectability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(4), 802–811.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayward, D. A., & Ristic, J. (2013). The uniqueness of social attention revisited: working memory load interferes with endogenous but not social orienting. Experimental Brain Research, 231(4), 405–414.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hickey, C., Chelazzi, L., & Theeuwes, J. (2010). Reward changes salience in human vision via the anterior cingulate. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(33), 11096–11103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., Pratt, J., Colzato, L., & Godijn, R. (2001). Symbolic control of visual attention. Psychological Science, 12(5), 360–365.

  • Hopfinger, J. B., Buonocore, M. H., & Mangun, G. R. (2000). The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nature Neuroscience, 3(3), 284–291.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hopfinger, J. B., & Mangun, G. R. (1998). Reflexive attention modulates processing of visual stimuli in human extrastriate cortex. Psychological Science, 9, 441–446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hopfinger, J. B., & West, V. M. (2006). Interactions between endogenous and exogenous attention on cortical visual processing. NeuroImage, 31(2), 774–789.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye’s movement. In J. B. Long & A. D. Baddeley (Eds.), Vol. Attention & Performance (pp. 187–203). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kincade, J. M., Abrams, R. A., Astafiev, S. V., Shulman, G. L., & Corbetta, M. (2005). An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study of voluntary and stimulus-driven orienting of attention. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(18), 4593–4604.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, R. M. (2009). Canadian Laboratories/Laboratoires Canadiens on the Control of Attention. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale, 63(3), 240–252.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luck, S. J., Hillyard, S. A., Mouloua, M., Woldorff, M. G., Clark, V. P., & Hawkins, H. L. (1994). Effects of spatial cuing on luminance detectability: psychophysical and electrophysiological evidence for early selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(4), 887–904.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mangun, G. R., & Hillyard, S. A. (1991). Modulations of sensory-evoked brain potentials indicate changes in perceptual processing during visual-spatial priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17(4), 1057–1074.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martinez, A., Anllo-Vento, L., Sereno, M. I., Frank, L. R., Buxton, R. B., Dubowitz, D. J., Hillyard, S. A. (1999). Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention. Nature Neuroscience, 2(4), 364–369.

  • Muller, H. J., & Rabbitt, P. M. (1989). Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resistance to interruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15(2), 315–330.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olk, B., Cameron, B., & Kingstone, A. (2008). Enhanced orienting effects: evidence for an interaction principle. Visual Cognition, 16(7), 979–1000.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1), 3–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and Performance X: Control of Language Processes (Vol. Attention & Performance) (pp. 531–566). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinzmetal, W., McCool, C., & Park, S. (2005). Attention: reaction time and accuracy reveal different mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(1), 73–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ristic, J., Friesen, C. K., & Kingstone, A. (2002). Are eyes special? It depends on how you look at it. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(3), 507–513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ristic, J., & Giesbrecht, B. (2011). Electrophysiological evidence for spatiotemporal flexibility in the ventrolateral attention network. PLoS ONE, 6(9), e24436.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ristic, J., & Kingstone, A. (2006). Attention to arrows: pointing to a new direction. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(11), 1921–1930.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ristic, J., & Kingstone, A. (2009). Rethinking attentional development: reflexive and volitional orienting in children and adults. Developmental Science, 12(2), 289–296.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ristic, J., & Kingstone, A. (2012). Automated symbolic orienting: a unique form of human spatial attention. Visual Cognition, 20(3), 244–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ristic, J., Landry, M., & Kingstone, A. (2012). Automated symbolic orienting: the missing link. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 560.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shulman, G. L., Astafiev, S. V., Franke, D., Pope, D. L., Snyder, A. Z., McAvoy, M. P., & Corbetta, M. (2009). Interaction of stimulus-driven reorienting and expectation in ventral and dorsal frontoparietal and basal ganglia-cortical networks. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(14), 4392–4407.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, S. (1969). Memory-scanning: mental processes revealed by reaction-time experiments. American Scientist, 57(4), 421–457.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tipper, C. M., Handy, T. C., Giesbrecht, B., & Kingstone, A. (2008). Brain responses to biological relevance. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(5), 879–891.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tipples, J. (2002). Eye gaze is not unique: automatic orienting in response to uninformative arrows. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(2), 314–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tipples, J. (2008). Orienting to counterpredictive gaze and arrow cues. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(1), 77–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vossel, S., Weidner, R., Driver, J., Friston, K. J., & Fink, G. R. (2012). Deconstructing the architecture of dorsal and ventral attention systems with dynamic causal modeling. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(31), 10637–10648.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1990). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(1), 121–134.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council (NSERC), Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council (SSHRC), the Stairs Foundation, and William Dawson fund to J.R., and fellowships from NSERC, Fonds de Recherche Nature et Technologies Québec (FQRNT), and the Tomlinson foundation to M.L. We would also like to thank Brad Gibson and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jelena Ristic.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ristic, J., Landry, M. Combining attention: a novel way of conceptualizing the links between attention, sensory processing, and behavior. Atten Percept Psychophys 77, 36–49 (2015). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-014-0737-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-014-0737-9

Keywords

Navigation