Next Article in Journal
A Multi-Channel Ensemble Method for Error-Related Potential Classification Using 2D EEG Images
Next Article in Special Issue
An Integrated Machine Learning-Based Brain Computer Interface to Classify Diverse Limb Motor Tasks: Explainable Model
Previous Article in Journal
Low-Cost Dual-Frequency GNSS Receivers and Antennas for Surveying in Urban Areas
Previous Article in Special Issue
Classification of Motor Imagery EEG Signals Based on Data Augmentation and Convolutional Neural Networks
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Abnormal Brain Circuits Characterize Borderline Personality and Mediate the Relationship between Childhood Traumas and Symptoms: A mCCA+jICA and Random Forest Approach

1
Clinical and Affective Neuroscience Lab (CL.I.A.N. Lab), Department of Psychology and Cognitive Sciences (DiPSCo), University of Trento, 38068 Rovereto, Italy
2
Centre for Medical Sciences (CISMed), University of Trento, 38122 Trento, Italy
3
Unit of Neuroscience, Department of Medicine and Surgery, University of Parma, 43126 Parma, Italy
4
Department of Brain and Behavioral Sciences, University of Pavia, 27100 Pavia, Italy
5
Cognitive Neuropsychology Centre, ASST “Grande Ospedale Metropolitano” Niguarda, 20162 Milan, Italy
6
Milan Centre for Neuroscience (NeuroMI), 20126 Milan, Italy
7
Department of Psychology, Science Division, New York University of Abu Dhabi, Abu Dhabi P.O. Box 129188, United Arab Emirates
8
Universitas Mercatorum, 00186 Rome, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sensors 2023, 23(5), 2862; https://doi.org/10.3390/s23052862
Submission received: 3 February 2023 / Revised: 1 March 2023 / Accepted: 2 March 2023 / Published: 6 March 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Brain Activity Monitoring and Measurement)

Abstract

:
Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is a severe personality disorder whose neural bases are still unclear. Indeed, previous studies reported inconsistent findings concerning alterations in cortical and subcortical areas. In the present study, we applied for the first time a combination of an unsupervised machine learning approach known as multimodal canonical correlation analysis plus joint independent component analysis (mCCA+jICA), in combination with a supervised machine learning approach known as random forest, to possibly find covarying gray matter and white matter (GM-WM) circuits that separate BPD from controls and that are also predictive of this diagnosis. The first analysis was used to decompose the brain into independent circuits of covarying grey and white matter concentrations. The second method was used to develop a predictive model able to correctly classify new unobserved BPD cases based on one or more circuits derived from the first analysis. To this aim, we analyzed the structural images of patients with BPD and matched healthy controls (HCs). The results showed that two GM-WM covarying circuits, including basal ganglia, amygdala, and portions of the temporal lobes and of the orbitofrontal cortex, correctly classified BPD against HC. Notably, these circuits are affected by specific child traumatic experiences (emotional and physical neglect, and physical abuse) and predict symptoms severity in the interpersonal and impulsivity domains. These results support that BPD is characterized by anomalies in both GM and WM circuits related to early traumatic experiences and specific symptoms.

1. Introduction

According to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5th edition (DSM 5), borderline personality disorder (BPD) is characterized by a pervasive pattern of dysregulation of affect (anger outbursts, depression episodes, and anxiety), cognition (dissociative experiences and self-image disturbances), interpersonal relationships (unstable relationships and fear of abandonment), together with marked impulsivity [1,2]. The disorder has a 3% prevalence in the general population [3,4] and is associated with significant impairment of patients’ psychological functioning [4,5,6].
In the last twenty years, several neuroimaging studies have tried to understand and delineate the neural bases of BPD. So far, neuroanatomical models of BPD indicate significant reduction in grey matter volume and density in the bilateral medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) [7], medial orbital frontal cortex (OFC) [8,9], bilateral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) [10], bilateral amygdale, and right parahippocampal gyrus [11,12]. An increase in grey matter volume and density has also been observed in the bilateral precuneus, right medium/paracingulate gyrus, and posterior cingulate gyrus [7]. By contrast, other studies have shown cortical volume reductions using specific regions of interest (ROIs), including in the anterior cingulate cortex, the orbitofrontal cortex, and the right parietal cortex [13,14,15]. Other studies have also examined brain structure in BPD using voxel-based morphometry (VBM) [16] showing structural alterations in the pars opercularis and triangularis of the right inferior frontal gyrus, in the precentral gyrus, and in the right superior frontal gyrus and bilaterally in the temporal lobe cortex [17,18,19]. However, other studies failed to report evidence of volume reductions in the hippocampus or amygdala, see for example [20]. Besides structural alterations, functional alterations have been found too [19,21,22,23].
Based on these results, two distinct (but not necessarily incompatible) neurobiological models of BPD pathology can be outlined. In the first model, the emotional instability/impulsivity/emotion regulation of BPD has been characterized as the core element of this disorder [1,6,8]. In support of this model, functional imaging studies consistently describe a possible phenotype of emotional instability as hyper-reactivity of the amygdala (limbic system) reacting to highly arousing and negative emotional stimuli and impaired recruitment of cognitive processes regulating emotions mainly through activations of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [1]; see also [19]. The second explanatory model of BPD underlines the abnormal social and interpersonal functions (including theory of mind deficits) that are characteristic of patients with BPD [17]. Both models (emotion and impulse dysregulation) and (social-interpersonal deficits) may capture different but complementary aspects of BPD symptomatology. Previous studies focused on one of these aspects and returned a partial view of the complexity of BPD. The aim of our studies is to consider both deficits and possibly separate the brain circuits responsible for these disturbances. Besides the conceptual limitation of previous studies, methodological limitations were also present. First, the dependence between the various voxels was not considered, as mass univariate approaches were used [24,25]. This may have limited the possibility to uncover latent patterns of interaction between sparse parts of the brain. Second, the majority of the above-cited studies focused only on specific and a priori decided regions of interest (ROIs), thus limiting the explanatory capacity of the results and possibly hiding the relevance of other structures [26]. As such, these approaches may fail to provide a comprehensive view of a complex disorder such as BPD that may rely on distributed abnormal brain regions.
To overcome the above limitations, a recent study [27] used a data-driven supervised machine learning approach to elucidate whole brain grey matter (GM) alterations in a group of BPD compared with healthy controls (HC), and a clinical control group (bipolar patients). This study showed that a circuit, including basal ganglia, amygdala, and portions of the temporal lobes and of the orbitofrontal cortex, correctly classified BPD against HC (84.62%) and against bipolars (80%). For what concerns the WM contribution, Lapomarda and colleagues [25] applied unsupervised machine learning methods and found that BPD were characterized by white matter (WM) alterations when compared with healthy controls and a clinical control group (bipolar patients) [25]. In particular, BPD patients showed increased white matter concentration in frontal–parietal and temporal regions possibly associated with a dysfunctional top-down emotion regulation [25]. However, in this study, GM and WM were analyzed separately. As such, this analysis does not allow to capture cross-information between modalities [28], as it is not sensitive to find linked hidden structures in the data or any integration between different datatypes [28]. Using both GM and WM properties in a data fusion approach, can greatly improve our understanding of the pathophysiology of BPD [29].

2. The Present Study

To overcome the limitations described above, in the present study we used an alternative approach recently developed in the machine learning field that seeks to quantitatively examine the relationship amongst different imaging modalities. This machine learning method is multivariate in nature, thus taking into account the relationship among voxels, is a whole-brain approach, and uses cross-information from multimodal neuroimaging data to explore the complex interplay of brain alterations in different modalities. The method is known as multimodal canonical correlation analysis (mCCA) in conjunction with joint independent component analysis (jICA) [30]. The use of mCCA in conjunction with jICA (mCCA+jICA) [28] is able to fuse two modalities (for example GM and WM) to find the correlations between them (a multimodal canonical covariate matrix, mCCA), and to separate the covariance matrix into independent networks of covarying GM-WM (jICA). As such, mCCA allows a multimodal fusion (MMF) that helps to identify the unique and shared variance associated with each imaging modality that underlies cognitive functioning in healthy controls and impaired mental illness [30,31,32]. Loading coefficients of GM-WM circuits are then tested for their differences between normal and abnormal populations. Previous studies have shown the benefit of MMF for understanding complex syndromes such as schizophrenia [30,31,33] and bipolar disorder [33]. These data-driven parcellation methods can be more biologically plausible than atlas-based parcellations that rely on anatomical or histological properties of the brain tissues that may not be directly related to psychological functions [34]. ICA-based methods by contrast decompose the brain into networks of regions that structurally (or functionally) covary. It is reasonable to assume that if some regions covary in their structural or functional properties across subjects, they do so because they belong to a similar network responsible for similar psychological functions. Thus, the first aim of the present study was to apply a multi-modal fusion (MMF) approach to find covarying GM-WM circuits that differ between BPD patients and HC. We predict to find a GM and WM circuit including temporal and frontal regions, as well as the insula, the amygdala, and the hippocampus in line with previous observations [17,18,19,27]. These regions have been found to be compromised in previous studies [17,18,19,25,27].
Once the covarying GM-WM circuits that differ between BPD and HC have been identified, a supervised machine learning approach known as random forest will be used to build a predictive model of BPD (second aim). This will allow us to test the generalization of our results to new unobserved cases. Random forests will be used to this aim. Random forest is an ensemble learning method for classification based on a forest of multiple decision trees [35,36,37]. The holdout method will be used to train the model on 80% of the participants and to test it on the remaining 20% every time a tree is created. As such, this method can return a measure of generalizability. We predict that the same circuits found in the first aim allow generalization to new cases. Moreover, with random forest being a hierarchical approach (all the networks are estimated and ordered from the best to the worst predictive), it may be that other networks will display a high level of predictability of BPD diagnosis. If the random forest will be able to correctly classify BPD against HC, this means that the networks allowing this classification may be used as potential biomarkers of BPD pathology.
Once the brain networks that separate (aim 1) and predict (aim 2) the BPD diagnosis are found, we aim at characterizing the psychological meaning of these networks. Of note, we aim to find possible relations with childhood traumas that are known to be potential etiological factors of BPD, and with specific symptoms BPD suffer from. Among relevant clinical data that may contribute to the identification of the psychological meaning of relevant neural mechanisms involved in BPD, in the present study we considered traumatic experiences in childhood. Meta-analyses of cross-sectional studies indicated that BPD patients are more likely to report childhood trauma history than nonclinical individuals, including experiences of sexual and physical abuse, neglect, maladaptive parenting, and parental conflict [38], and are more likely to report childhood trauma experiences than other psychiatric groups [39]. Conversely, studies comparing maltreated and non-maltreated children on the prevalence of borderline features show that maltreated children were significantly more likely to present borderline features [40,41]. Prospective longitudinal research provided further evidence for the hypothesis that exposure to adverse events in childhood increased the risk of being diagnosed with BPD in adulthood [42,43,44,45]. In a previous attempt to assess the impact of traumatic experiences on the brain of BPD patients, Dadomo and colleagues [46] reported evidence that a circuit including the amygdala, the Heschl area, the caudate, the putamen, and portions of the cerebellum was predictive of sexual abuse. They also reported that another circuit involving temporal and cerebellar regions was predictive of interpersonal problems. However, this study considered only the contribution of GM, ignoring the fact that early traumatic experiences may affect WM too, and that WM may contribute to specific symptoms. Moreover, that study considered every factor in isolation (independent statistical models), without assessing the contribution of all factors in one unique model to assess for their relative contribution. In the present study, by contrast, we aim at building one unique model, to test a specific hypothesis that early life traumatic events such as physical and emotional abuse or neglect as well as sexual abuse (as measured by the Childhood Traumatic Questionnaire (CTQ) [47]), affect the abnormal brain networks found in aims 1 and 2, and how they in turn support specific symptoms in the cognitive, affective, interpersonal, and impulsivity domains, as measured by the Zanarini Rating Scale for Borderline Personality Disorder (ZAN-BPD) [48]. This analysis may help us in identifying the neural bases of the two models of BPD symptoms discussed before (the emotional-impulsivity dysregulation model vs. the interpersonal-social deficits model). The third aim of the present study was to assess the impact of early traumatic experiences on specific brain circuits, and how such altered brain circuits give rise to specific BPD symptoms according to the current models of this disorder. Mediation analysis will be used to assess the mediating role of the brain networks found in aim 1 and 2 in explaining the link between specific child traumas and specific symptoms. We predict that the networks that differ between BPD and HC are affected by specific traumatic experiences such as emotional and physical mistreatments, and that these abnormal neural circuits explain at least some of the psychological problems BPD patients suffer from such as, for example, interpersonal and impulsivity problems. In summary, we predict that these analyses will shed light on an abnormal impulsivity network and a more interpersonal network.

3. Materials and Methods

3.1. Participants

A total of 20 patients with BPD (mean age = 35.75 years, SD = 8.61 years) and 45 healthy participants as controls (HC) without history of psychiatric and neurological disease (mean age 36.69 years, SD = 8.46 years), matched for age and sex, were considered. All the data were extracted from the shared OpenNeuro database [49]. Demographic information about participants is displayed in Table 1. The recruitment was carried out by outpatient and support services from around Edinburgh. The exclusion criteria were the presence of neurological disease or mental illness rather than BPD (Structural Clinical Interview, SCID-II, SCID- IV), and the use of psychoactive substance, pregnancy, MRI contraindications. The BPD diagnosis was verified using the Structured Clinical Interview for Diagnostic Statistical Manual fourth edition (DSM-IV) (SCID-II). See Table 1.

3.2. Questionnaires

To reach our aims, the scores of the Child Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) [47] that measures five sources of traumas (physical and emotional neglect and abuse, and sexual abuse) and the Zanarini Rating Scale for Borderline Personality Disorder (ZAN-BPD) [48] that measures borderline symptoms in five domains (cognitive, affective, interpersonal, and impulsivity) were taken into consideration. The CTQ is a self-assessment questionnaire developed to evaluate traumatic experiences experienced during childhood that includes five sources of traumas: physical neglect, emotional neglect, physical abuse, emotional abuse, and sexual abuse. The Zan-BPD was designed to capture the severity of symptoms in four main sectors: the Affective sector indicating anger outbursts, feelings of emptiness, and mood instability; the Cognitive sector relative to identity disturbance disassociation and paranoia; the Impulsivity sector relative to impulsivity such as self-mutilative/suicidal efforts; and the Interpersonal sector indicating intense, unstable relationships, and frantic efforts to avoid abandonment.

3.3. Preprocessing

T1-weighted images were pre-processed through SPM12 (Statistical Parametric Mapping, https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/) [50] and the CAT12 toolbox (Computational Anatomy Toolbox for SPM, http://www.neuro.uni-jena.de/cat/) [51]. After having manually re-oriented all the images, placing the anterior commissure as the origin, the segmentation into gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid was computed. The Diffeomorphic Anatomical Registration using Exponential Lie algebra tools for SPM12 (DARTEL) [52] was used for registration. Finally, the normalization to the MNI space with a spatial Gaussian smoothing of 8 was performed.

3.4. Data Fusion Unsupervised Machine Learning

mCCA+jICA was applied to structural data using the FusionICA Toolbox (FIT, http://mialab.mrn.org/software/fit) [53] in the MATLAB 2021b environment (https://it.mathworks.com/products/matlab.html) (MATLAB (R2021b), 2021). The number of components was estimated for both modalities with information theoretic criteria [54]. To assess the consistency of each modality, ICASSO [55,56] was run ten times and the Infomax algorithm was selected. The resulting output consists of a matrix with the number of subjects (rows) and the loading coefficients for each component (columns). Loading coefficients represent how each component is expressed for every subject. Eventual differences between groups was calculated with a t-test on the loading coefficients of the GM-WM circuits. As a final step, we converted the independent components into Talairach coordinates in order to specify the brain areas. Areas with both positive and negative values, if present, were considered and plotted in Surf Ice (https://www.nitrc.org/projects/surfice/) using a different template for gray and white matter.

3.5. Predictive Model

Besides testing for differences between groups by using frequentists approaches (e.g., t-tests) whose results are limited to the sample considered, we also used a supervised machine learning (SML) approach to extract a statistical model to predict new cases. In other words, we aimed to test our results for their generalization. To carry out this test, an SML method known as random forest classification was used. The name random forest is derived from decision trees, another SML for classification, but it uses multiple trees and then averages their performance (bagging method). To classify a label (e.g., BPD patient) from an input vector (made of the loading coefficients of every independent GM-WM network derived by mCCA+jICA), the input vector is inserted in a tree. The trees classify the label, and then votes for that class. The trees with the lower error rates are the strongest classifiers. The forest chooses the classification having most of the votes [35,36,37]. Random forest is an ensemble learning method for classification based on a forest of decision trees [35,36,37]. For classification tasks, the output of the random forest is the class selected by the most trees. Of note, random decision forests outperform the decision trees (and other SML algorithms) in terms of having less overfitting problems than many other classification algorithms [57]. One of the main reasons random forest was used in this study is that it allows for ranking the importance of variables in a classification problem. In other words, the output is a hierarchical model that allows to estimate the most important feature to the least important. As such, this method can help us understand not only the brain circuits that differ between BPD and HC (frequentist approach), but also to assess which circuits predict new unobserved cases. The algorithm is indeed trained to correctly classify a part of the sample (including BPD and HC), and then tested for its predictive power on the unobserved subsample. The statistical results refer to the prediction of new unobserved cases and as such can be used as a measure of generalization and for the creation of a future possible biomarker [58]. See Figure 1 and Figure 2.

4. Results

4.1. Groups Comparison

The information theoretic criteria estimated 10 independent covarying gray matter (IC-GM) and white matter (IC-WM) networks. The positive values of these networks indicate increased gray/white matter concentration, whereas negative values indicate decreased concentration. The meaning of the covariation between a gray matter and a white matter component refers to a similar pattern of gray/white matter concentration. The results indicate that the following components can be ordered by importance, statistically differed between groups: ICGM2 (t = 3.715, p < 0.001) and ICWM2 (t = 4.189, p < 0.001), ICGM6 (t = 2.625, p = 0.011) and ICWM6 (t = 2.501, p = 0.015), ICGM8 (t = −2.384, p = 0.020), ICWM8 (t = −2.513, p = 0.015), and ICGM4 (−2.335, p = 0.023) but not ICWM4 (t = −1.231, p = 0.223). Whereas the others did not (ICGM1 p = 0.881, ICWM1 p = 0.701; ICGM3 p = 0.843, ICWM3 p = 0.382; IGGM5 p = 0.719, ICWM5 p = 0.716; ICGM7 p = 0.273, ICWM7 p = 0.514, ICGM9 p = 0.308, ICWM9 p = 0.975; and ICM10 p = 0.038 ICWM10 p = 0.447). However, after applying a Bonferroni corrected threshold (p = 0.00125), only the ICGM2-ICWM2 survived. Regions included in the ICGM2 were the post central and precentral gyri, the superior middle temporal gyrus, the insula, the superior, middle, and inferior frontal gyrus, the parietal lobule, the uncus (including the amygdala), cerebellar portions, and the hippocampus among others. See Figure 2 and Table 2A–D. GM and WM of IC2 were also highly correlated with each other (r = 0.857), indicating a strong common profile of alterations in a network that differs between BPD and HC. See Figure 3, network plot, for a representation of each GM-WM component and the strength of the correlation. See Supplementary Table S1.

4.2. Predictive Model Results

For the random forest classification, the holdout method of 20% for validation and 20% for testing was selected. In other words, the model was trained on 39 subjects, the model was then validated during learning on 13 subjects, and finally, the model was tested on 13 previously unobserved subjects. The proportion of BPD and HC was kept among the 3 partitions with 30% BPD and 70% HC. The number of trees to reach optimal performance was 78 with 3 features per split. Random forest classification returned a test accuracy of 84.6% for both BPD and HC, a precision (positive predictive value) of 75% for BPD and 88.9% for HC, a recall (true positive rate) of 75% for BPD and 88.9% for HC, a false positive rate of 11.1% for BD and 25% for HC, and a false discovery rate of 25% for BD and 11.1% for HC. The area under the curve was 0.861 for BPD and 0.889 for HC (average = 0.875). The mean decrease in purity confirmed the importance of ICGM2 as a main predictor and main split in trees. Then followed these components: ICGM6, ICGM9, ICGM8, and ICGM7. See Figure 4. Of note, ICGM2 and ICGM6 were very similar in the power of prediction (respectively, a mean decrease in accuracy of 0.055 and 0.052). Building on this result, we also decided to comment further on the ICGM-WM6 component. This component included regions such as temporal–parietal regions, the parahippocampus, the cingulate, the fusiform gyrus, the cuneus, and the insula. The GM and WM of IC6 were also highly correlated with each other (r = 0.777), indicating a strong common profile of alterations in a network that differs between BPD and HC. See Table 3A–D and Figure 5.

4.3. Mediation Analysis

To test the hypothesis that early traumatic experiences as measured by the CTQ (independent variables, IVs) may support specific symptoms as measured by the Zanarini BPD symptoms questionnaire (dependent variables, DVs) via the contribution of specific neural circuits that differ between BPD and HC (mediating variables, MVs), we ran a mediation analysis (MA). The MA included the five CTQ subscales (the IVs), the four neural circuits (ICGM2, ICWM2, ICGM6, and ICWM6) (the MAs), and the four symptoms sectors of Zanarini (the DVs). See Figure 6. For what concerns the indirect effect, or the impact of the IVs on the DVs mediated by the MVs of interest for the present study, the results showed that emotional neglect (b = −0.300, 0.018) and physical abuse (b = 0.519, p = 0.016) predicted the IC2 (WM) network and that this in turn predicted symptoms in the impulsivity domain. Physical neglect (b = 0.630, p = 0.048) and abuse (b = 0.579, p = 0.036) predicted IC6 (GM) and this in turn predicted interpersonal symptoms. In summary, at least one of the two modalities of each brain circuit are related to specific traumatic sources and support specific symptoms. See Supplementary Table S2 and Supplementary Figure S1.

5. Discussion

In the present study, we contributed to the clarification of the neural basis of BPD by taking advantage of a novel combination of two methodologies (see [59] for a similar method). First, we used an unsupervised data fusion machine learning approach known as mCC+jICA to determine latent abnormal covariance patterns of the gray and white matter that separate BPD patients from healthy controls. T-test clarified that one of these networks significantly differs between BPD and controls. Then, we tested the possibility to predict the diagnoses of BPD cases (random forest, a supervised machine learning approach) from these networks. Two networks were found to predict BPD new cases. Last but not least, we tested the role of these circuits as possible mediators between etiological factors of BPD (traumatic experiences in childhood) and different BPD symptoms. We found that the two networks that predict the diagnosis of BPD mediate traumas and impulsivity and interpersonal symptoms. We thus provided support for both of the current models of BPD: the emotional-impulsivity dysregulation model and the interpersonal-social deficits model. In what follows, we describe such patterns in terms of the “impulsivity network” and the “interpersonal network”.

5.1. Impulsivity Network

The first network as expected was specifically predictive of BPD symptoms in the impulsivity domain and supports the model of BPD as a disorder of emotional and impulsivity problems. This network included alterations of white matter in several prefrontal regions, including the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and extended alterations of gray matter in the insula, postcentral, and precentral gyri. Consistently with our results, all these areas have been previously mentioned as implicated in a variety of impulsivity-related functions [6,60,61], and their structural alterations have been previously reported as associated with individual differences in impulsivity [25,62,63,64,65]. As widely described in “top-down cognitive control” models of impulsivity [66,67], PFC areas may act as a brake on impulsive tendencies by exerting inhibitory control, while subcortical structures propel the occurrence of impulsive behaviors. Among other components of executive control, the role of IFG in impulsivity has been largely investigated due to its role in contrasting impulsivity through the suppression of inappropriate behavioral responses [49]. The insula, instead, appears to be involved in a specific component of impulsivity which concerns the preference of smaller immediate rewards instead of waiting for larger delayed rewards (delay discounting). Due to the relevance of impulsivity-related symptoms in BPD, the alterations in such brain structures were expected from the present study and are in line with previous studies on BPD patients [13,14,15]. Extending such previous studies, the present study suggests that it may be possible to derive an objective biomarker for BPD, generated from impulsivity-related brain structure abnormalities. Of note, these results extend the previous results [25] on structural networks differences between BPD and HC. A previous study [25] provided evidence of a network related to affective disturbances in BPD (correlation with the emotional sector of the Zanarini). Our results add another network to the previous one, a network more related to impulsivity. This may indicate that the model of emotional-impulsivity disturbances in BPD may rely not on one unique network, but rather on two different brain networks, one more related to affective disturbances and another more related to impulsivity.

5.2. Interpersonal Network

The second network that was predictive of having a diagnosis of BPD was strictly associated with interpersonal symptoms. This network included different brain regions of increased grey matter concentration in the temporal–parietal junction (TPJ) and other smaller areas located in anterior and posterior midline structures and in the insula. These brain structures correspond to a well-described theory of mind (ToM) network associated with the ability to think about mental states in oneself and especially in others [68]. As such, this network is a good candidate to explain the interpersonal and mentalizing problems these patients suffer from. As part of this network, the TPJ has a core role in inferring the mental states of others [69], as it combines cognitive, social, and affective information during interpersonal situations [69]. Even if most popular neurobiological models of psychopathology are mainly focused on deficits in executive/inhibitory functions, recent contributions have emphasized the additional relevance of semantic functions implicated in the mental representation of self and others [70]. Such semantic alteration is also implicit in clinical models of BPD, which attribute interpersonal difficulties of these patients to impoverished representations of self and others reflected in the shift between positive and negative views of others (e.g., “black-and-white thinking”) [71]. The altered interpersonal-social network (that includes ToM abilities) observed in the present study can be thus considered a significant marker of BPD. Of note, ICGM6 largely overlaps with a network found in a previous study [21]. In that study, the network was found to predict interpersonal problems (measured via the Zanarini questionnaire). Although the method used in our study is very different from the one used in that study (a supervised machine learning method known as multi-kernel regression), the results converge to a similar temporo-frontal network related to interpersonal dysfunctions in BPD patients.

5.3. The Impact of Specific Childhood Traumas on the Brain and Symptoms

We also found that the brain circuits correctly classifying BPD were predicted by specific traumatic experiences during childhood (CTQ questionnaire). Specifically, emotional neglect and physical abuse predicted the “impulsivity network”, and physical neglect and abuse predicted the “interpersonal network”. The negative influence of relational traumatic experiences in childhood has been largely theorized in clinical theories about the pathogenesis of BPD [21,25,27]. Previous studies have described the effect of some of these traumatic experiences on the brain of BPD patients [21,71,72]. However, the present study expands our knowledge on this topic by providing a clearer link between specific traumas and separate brain circuits. It is reasonable to think that relationships characterized by physical neglect and abuse can influence the interpersonal and social areas of the brain included in the “interpersonal network” we found. In the same vein, relationships characterized by emotional neglect (but also physical abuse by a violent caregiver) may lead to abnormalities in brain areas responsible for the control of impulsivity. These areas may correspond to the ones that we found in the “impulsivity network”. A previous study reported an association between BPD and sexual abuse [21]. In our study, we could not find this result, probably due to a different method used. It should be noted that the network found in that study [21] includes areas different from the ones that we found in the two networks of our study. Future studies may aim to better explore this issue.

6. Conclusions

Our study found two networks of covarying grey and white matter that significantly differ between BPD and HC by using an innovative combination of supervised and unsupervised machine learning approaches. These networks can correctly classify new unobserved cases. Of note, these networks are strictly related to specific child traumas and support symptoms that typically are displayed by borderline patients. Besides the merits, our study does not come without limitations. First, we must acknowledge that the sample size of patients with BPD was quite small. This limitation is common in the scientific literature on BPD as there are a limited number of studies and a few available open datasets. Future studies may aim to use larger samples to replicate these findings. Second, we focused our analyses only on structural brain features. Future studies may aim to explore the contribution of both structural and functional properties. With such a combination, better classification accuracies can be reached and common anatomo-functional alterations can be demonstrated. Besides the limitations, this combined data fusion of an unsupervised and supervised machine learning approach has not been applied previously to understand the BPD brain. We believe that these and other [73] machine learning approaches can be useful in understanding the neural bases of personality disorders and may pave the way for the creation of possible biomarkers.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/s23052862/s1, Table S1: Other independent components. Table S2: Mediation analysis. Figure S1: Path plot.

Author Contributions

A.G., I.M., G.S. and H.D. conceptualized the study. G.L. and S.S. preprocessed the data and revised the manuscript. A.G. analyzed the data and wrote the Materials and Methods and Results sections. H.D. wrote the manuscript. I.M. and G.S. wrote and revised the manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the ethics committee at the medical faculty of the University of Leipzig (154/13-ff).

Informed Consent Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved for the reuse of open-access data, see details in the manuscript. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Data Availability Statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found at the UCLA Consortium for Neuropsychiatric Phenomics OpenNeuro database, accession number ds000030.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Koenigsberg, H.W.; Harvey, P.D.; Mitropoulou, V.; Schmeidler, J.; New, A.S.; Goodman, M.; Silverman, J.M.; Serby, M.; Schopick, F.; Siever, L.J. Characterizing affective instability in borderline personality disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 2002, 159, 784–788. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Zanarini, M.C. Psychotherapy of borderline personality disorder. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2009, 120, 373–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Lenzenweger, M.F.; Lane, M.C.; Loranger, A.W.; Kessler, R.C. DSM-IV Personality disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Biol. Psychiatry 2007, 62, 553–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Trull, T.J.; Jahng, S.; Tomko, R.L.; Wood, P.K.; Sher, K.J. Revised NESARC personality disorder diagnoses: Gender, prevalence, and comorbidity with substance dependence disorders. J. Pers. Disord. 2010, 24, 412–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Tomko, R.; Trull, T.J.; Wood, P.K.; Sher, K.J. Characteristics of borderline personality disorder in a community sample: Comorbidity, treatment utilization, and general functioning. J. Pers. Disord. 2014, 28, 734–750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. De Panfilis, C.; Schito, G.; Generali, I.; Gozzi, L.A.; Ossola, P.; Marchesi, C.; Grecucci, A. Emotions at the border: Increased punishment behavior during fair interpersonal exchanges in borderline personality disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2019, 128, 162–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Yu, H.; Meng, Y.-J.; Li, X.-J.; Zhang, C.; Liang, S.; Li, M.-L.; Li, Z.; Guo, W.; Wang, Q.; Deng, W.; et al. Common and distinct patterns of grey matter alterations in borderline personality disorder and bipolar disorder: Voxel-based meta-analysis. Br. J. Psychiatry 2019, 215, 395–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Wolf, R.C.; Thomann, P.A.; Sambataro, F.; Vasic, N.; Schmid, M.; Wolf, N.D. Orbitofrontal cortex and impulsivity in borderline personality disorder: An MRI study of baseline brain perfusion. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2012, 262, 677–685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Ducasse, D.; Courtet, P.; Olié, E. Physical and social pains in borderline disorder and neuroanatomical correlates: A systematic review. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2014, 16, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Wrege, J.S.; Ruocco, A.C.; Euler, S.; Preller, K.H.; Busmann, M.; Meya, L.; Schmidt, A.; Lang, U.E.; Borgwardt, S.; Walter, M. Negative affect moderates the effect of social rejection on frontal and anterior cingulate cortex activation in borderline personality disorder. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2019, 19, 1273–1285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Lou, J.; Sun, Y.; Cui, Z.; Gong, L. Common and distinct patterns of gray matter alterations in borderline personality disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder: A dual meta-analysis. Neurosci. Lett. 2020, 741, 135376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Pan, N.; Wang, S.; Qin, K.; Li, L.; Chen, Y.; Zhang, X.; Lai, H.; Suo, X.; Long, Y.; Yu, Y.; et al. Common and distinct neural patterns of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and borderline personality disorder: A multimodal functional and structural meta-analysis. In Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Irle, E.; Lange, C.; Sachsse, U. Reduced size and abnormal asymmetry of parietal cortex in women with borderline personality disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 2005, 57, 173–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Lyoo, I.K.; Han, M.H.; Cho, D.Y. A brain MRI study in subjects with borderline personality disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 1998, 50, 235–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Hazlett, E.A.; New, A.S.; Newmark, R.; Haznedar, M.M.; Lo, J.N.; Speiser, L.J.; Chen, A.D.; Mitropoulou, V.; Minzenberg, M.; Siever, L.J.; et al. Reduced anterior and posterior cingulate gray matter in borderline personality disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 2005, 58, 614–623. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Ashburner, J.; Friston, K. Voxel-based morphometry—The methods. Neuroimage 2000, 11, 805–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Soloff, P.H.; Pruitt, P.; Sharma, M.; Radwan, J.; White, R.; Diwadkar, V.A. Structural brain abnormalities and suicidal behavior in borderline personality disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2012, 46, 516–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Niedtfeld, I.; Schulze, L.; Krause-Utz, A.; Demirakca, T.; Bohus, M.; Schmahl, C. Voxel-based morphometry in women with borderline personality disorder with and without comorbid posttraumatic stress disorder. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e65824. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Schulze, L.; Schmahl, C.; Niedtfeld, I. Neural correlates of disturbed emotion processing in borderline personality disorder: A multimodal meta-analysis. Biol. Psychiatry 2015, 79, 97–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Aguilar-Ortiz, S.; Salgado-Pineda, P.; Marco-Pallarés, J.; Pascual, J.C.; Vega, D.; Soler, J.; Brunel, C.; Martin-Blanco, A.; Soto, A.; Ribas, J.; et al. Abnormalities in gray matter volume in patients with borderline personality disorder and their relation to lifetime depression: A VBM study. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0191946. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Dadomo, H.; Grecucci, A.; Giardini, I.; Ugolini, E.; Carmelita, A.; Panzeri, M. Schema therapy for emotional dysregulation: Theoretical implication and clinical applications. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1987. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Frederickson, J.J.; Messina, I.; Grecucci, A. Dysregulated anxiety and dysregulating defenses: Toward an emotion regulation informed dynamic psychotherapy. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 2054. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Ruocco, A.C.; Amirthavasagam, S.; Zakzanis, K.K. Amygdala and hippocampal volume reductions as candidate endophenotypes for borderline personality disorder: A meta-analysis of magnetic resonance imaging studies. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 2012, 201, 245–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Sorella, S.; Lapomarda, G.; Messina, I.; Frederickson, J.J.; Siugzdaite, R.; Job, R.; Grecucci, A. Testing the expanded continuum hypothesis of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Neural and psychological evidence for shared and distinct mechanisms. NeuroImage Clin. 2019, 23, 101854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Lapomarda, G.; Grecucci, A.; Messina, I.; Pappaianni, E.; Dadomo, H. Common and different gray and white matter alterations in bipolar and borderline personality disorder: A source-based morphometry study. Brain Res. 2021, 1762, 147401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Pappaianni, E.; De Pisapia, N.; Siugzdaite, R.; Crescentini, C.; Calcagnì, A.; Job, R.; Grecucci, A. Less is more: Psychological and morphometric differences between low vs high reappraisers. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2020, 20, 128–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Grecucci, A.; Lapomarda, G.; Messina, I.; Monachesi, B.; Sorella, S.; Siugzdaite, R. Structural features related to affective instability correctly classify patients with borderline personality disorder. A supervised machine learning approach. Front. Psychiatry 2022, 13, 804440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Sui, J.; Pearlson, G.; Caprihan, A.; Adali, T.; Kiehl, K.A.; Liu, J.; Yamamoto, J.; Calhoun, V.D. Discriminating schizophrenia and bipolar disorder by fusing fMRI and DTI in a multimodal CCA+ joint ICA model. Neuroimage 2011, 57, 839–855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Mechelli, A.; Viera, S. Machine Learning: Methods and Applications to Brain Disorders; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  30. Calhoun, V.; Adali, T.; Giuliani, N.; Pekar, J.; Kiehl, K.; Pearlson, G. Method for multimodal analysis of independent source differences in schizophrenia: Combining gray matter structural and auditory oddball functional data. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2005, 27, 47–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Correa, N.M.; Li, Y.-O.; Adali, T.; Calhoun, V.D. Canonical Correlation analysis for feature-based fusion of biomedical imaging modalities and its application to detection of associative networks in schizophrenia. IEEE J. Sel. Top. Signal Process. 2008, 2, 998–1007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Plis, S.M.; Weisend, M.P.; Damaraju, E.; Eichele, T.; Mayer, A.; Clark, V.P.; Lane, T.; Calhoun, V.D. Effective connectivity analysis of fMRI and MEG data collected under identical paradigms. Comput. Biol. Med. 2011, 41, 1156–1165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Sui, J.; He, H.; Yu, Q.; Chen, J.; Rogers, J.; Pearlson, G.D.; Mayer, A.; Bustillo, J.; Canive, J.; Calhoun, V.D. Combination of resting state fMRI, DTI, and sMRI data to discriminate schizophrenia by N-way MCCA + jICA. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Grecucci, A.; Sorella, S.; Consolini, J. Decoding individual differences in expressing and suppressing anger from structural brain networks: A supervised machine learning approach. Behav. Brain Res. 2023, 439, 114245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Ho, T.K. The random subspace method for constructing decision forests. IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell. 1998, 20, 832–844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Ho, B.C.; Andreasen, N.C. Do structural brain deficits worsen following onset of first-episode schizophrenia? In Schizophrenia Research; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2002; Volume 53, p. 98. [Google Scholar]
  37. Breiman, L. Random forests. Mach. Learn. 2001, 45, 5–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Fossati, A.; Madeddu, F.; Maffei, C. Borderline personality disorder and childhood sexual abuse: A meta-analytic study. J. Pers. Disord. 1999, 13, 268–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Porter, C.; Palmier-Claus, J.; Branitsky, A.; Mansell, W.; Warwick, H.; Varese, F. Childhood adversity and borderline personality disorder: A meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2019, 141, 6–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Cicchetti, D.; Rogosch, F.A.; Hecht, K.F.; Crick, N.R.; Hetzel, S. Moderation of maltreatment effects on childhood borderline personality symptoms by gender and oxytocin receptor and FK506 binding protein 5 genes. Dev. Psychopathol. 2014, 26, 831–849. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Hecht, K.F.; Cicchetti, D.; Rogosch, F.A.; Crick, N.R. Borderline personality features in childhood: The role of subtype, developmental timing, and chronicity of child maltreatment. Dev. Psychopathol. 2014, 26, 805–815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Belsky, D.W.; Caspi, A.; Arseneault, L.; Bleidorn, W.; Fonagy, P.; Goodman, M.; Houts, R.; Moffitt, T. Etiological features of borderline personality related characteristics in a birth cohort of 12-year-old children. Dev. Psychopathol. 2012, 24, 251–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Gratz, K.L.; Latzman, R.D.; Tull, M.T.; Reynolds, E.K.; Lejuez, C. Exploring the association between emotional abuse and childhood borderline personality features: The moderating role of personality traits. Behav. Ther. 2011, 42, 493–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Winsper, C.; Zanarini, M.; Wolke, D. Prospective study of family adversity and maladaptive parenting in childhood and borderline personality disorder symptoms in a non-clinical population at 11 years. Psychol. Med. 2012, 42, 2405–2420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Widom, C.S.; Czaja, S.J.; Paris, J. A prospective investigation of borderline personality disorder in abused and neglected children followed up into adulthood. J. Pers. Disord. 2009, 23, 433–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Dadomo, H.; Salvato, G.; Lapomarda, G.; Ciftci, Z.; Messina, I.; Grecucci, A. Structural features predict sexual trauma and interpersonal problems in borderline personality disorder but not in controls: A multi-voxel pattern analysis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2022, 16, 773593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Bernstein, D.P.; Fink, L.; Handelsman, L.; Foote, J. Childhood trauma questionnaire. Assessment of Family Violence: A Handbook for Researchers and Practitioners; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  48. Zanarini, M.C. Zanarini Rating Scale for borderline personality disorder (ZAN-BPD): A continuous measure of DSM-IV borderline psychopathology. J. Pers. Disord. 2003, 17, 233–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Aron, A.R.; Robbins, T.W.; Poldrack, R.A. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2004, 8, 170–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Penny, W.D.; Mattout, J.; Trujillo-Barreto, N. Bayesian model selection and averaging. Statistical Parametric Mapping: The Analysis of Functional Brain Images; Elsevier: London, UK, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  51. Gaser, C.; Dahnke, R.; Thompson, P.M.; Kurth, F.; Luders, E. CAT—A computational anatomy toolbox for the analysis of structural MRI data. bioRxiv 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Ashburner, J. A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage 2007, 38, 95–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Acar, E.; Levin-Schwartz, Y.; Calhoun, V.D.; Adali, T. Tensor-based fusion of EEG and FMRI to understand neurological changes in schizophrenia. In Proceedings of the 2017 IEEE International Symposium on Circuits and Systems (ISCAS), Baltimore, MD, USA, 28–31 May 2017; pp. 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Wax, M.; Kailath, T. Detection of signals by information theoretic criteria. IEEE Trans. Acoust. Speech Signal Process. 1985, 33, 387–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Himberg, J.; Hyvärinen, A.; Esposito, F. Validating the independent components of neuroimaging time series via clustering and visualization. Neuroimage 2004, 22, 1214–1222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Himberg, J.; Hyvarinen, A. Icasso: Software for investigating the reliability of ICA estimates by clustering and visualization. In Proceedings of the 2003 IEEE XIII Workshop on Neural Networks for Signal Processing (IEEE Cat. No.03TH8718), Toulouse, France, 17–19 September 2003; pp. 259–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Hastie, T.; Tibshirani, R.; Friedman, J. The Elements of Statistical Learning; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  58. Kozak, M.J.; Cuthbert, B.N. The NIMH research domain criteria initiative: Background, issues, and pragmatics. Psychophysiology 2016, 53, 286–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Baggio, T.; Grecucci, A.; Meconi, F.; Messina, I. Anxious brains: A combined data fusion machine learning approach to predict trait anxiety from morphometric features. Sensors 2023, 23, 610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Mitchell, M.R.; Potenza, M.N. Recent insights into the neurobiology of impulsivity. Curr. Addict. Rep. 2014, 1, 309–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  61. Bari, A.; Robbins, T.W. Inhibition and impulsivity: Behavioral and neural basis of response control. Prog. Neurobiol. 2013, 108, 44–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Lim, J.-E.; Kim, S.; Seo, S.; Kang, W.; Kim, A.; Kang, Y.; Choi, K.W.; Tae, W.-S.; Ham, B.-J.; Han, K.-M. Association of prefrontal cortex thinning with high impulsivity in healthy adults. Psychiatry Investig. 2021, 18, 570–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Lee, T.M.; Chan, C.C.; Han, S.-H.; Leung, A.W.; Fox, P.T.; Gao, J.-H. An event-related fMRI study on risk taking by healthy individuals of high or low impulsiveness. Neurosci. Lett. 2008, 438, 138–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Pan, N.; Wang, S.; Zhao, Y.; Lai, H.; Qin, K.; Li, J.; Biswal, B.B.; Sweeney, J.A.; Gong, Q. Brain gray matter structures associated with trait impulsivity: A systematic review and voxel-based meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2021, 42, 2214–2235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Hirjak, D.; Thomann, A.K.; Kubera, K.M.; Wolf, R.C.; Jeung, H.; Maier-Hein, K.H.; Thomann, P.A. Cortical folding patterns are associated with impulsivity in healthy young adults. Brain Imaging Behav. 2016, 11, 1592–1603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Fineberg, N.A.; Chamberlain, S.R.; Goudriaan, A.E.; Stein, D.J.; Vanderschuren, L.J.M.J.; Gillan, C.M.; Shekar, S.; Gorwood, P.A.P.M.; Voon, V.; Morein-Zamir, S.; et al. New developments in human neurocognition: Clinical, genetic, and brain imaging correlates of impulsivity and compulsivity. CNS Spectr. 2014, 19, 69–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Dalley, J.W.; Everitt, B.J.; Robbins, T.W. Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron 2011, 69, 680–694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Carrington, S.J.; Bailey, A.J. Are there theory of mind regions in the brain? A review of the neuroimaging literature. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009, 30, 2313–2335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Schurz, M.; Radua, J.; Tholen, M.G.; Maliske, L.; Margulies, D.S.; Mars, R.B.; Sallet, J.; Kanske, P. Toward a hierarchical model of social cognition: A neuroimaging meta-analysis and integrative review of empathy and theory of mind. Psychol. Bull. 2021, 147, 293–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Messina, I.; Sambin, M.; Beschoner, P.; Viviani, R. Changing views of emotion regulation and neurobiological models of the mechanism of action of psychotherapy. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2016, 16, 571–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  71. Kernberg, O. Borderline personality organization. J. Am. Psychoanal. Assoc. 1967, 15, 641–685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Roy, A. Childhood trauma and impulsivity. Possible relevance to suicidal behavior. Arch. Suicide Res. 2005, 9, 147–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Sharkawy, A.-N. Principle of neural network and its main types: Review. Adv. Appl. Comput. Math. 2020, 7, 8–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Schematic workflow of the analyses. After fusing the two modalities (GM and WM), the brain was decomposed into independent networks of covarying GM-WM (mCCA+jICA). Then Bonferroni corrected t-test was used to assess the networks that differed between groups.
Figure 1. Schematic workflow of the analyses. After fusing the two modalities (GM and WM), the brain was decomposed into independent networks of covarying GM-WM (mCCA+jICA). Then Bonferroni corrected t-test was used to assess the networks that differed between groups.
Sensors 23 02862 g001
Figure 2. Predictive model generation. The loading coefficients of the GM-WM networks derived from mCCA+jICA were entered in a random forest classification model to predict BPD diagnosis. Several trees were generated to classify the labels BPD and HC. Each tree voted for that class. The forest then chose the classification having most of the votes.
Figure 2. Predictive model generation. The loading coefficients of the GM-WM networks derived from mCCA+jICA were entered in a random forest classification model to predict BPD diagnosis. Several trees were generated to classify the labels BPD and HC. Each tree voted for that class. The forest then chose the classification having most of the votes.
Sensors 23 02862 g002
Figure 3. A covarying GM-WM network that differs from BPD and HC. Top: violin plots of the loading coefficients for GM and WM of the IC2. Central: network plot showing in green the strength of correlations between components. Bottom: brain plot of positive (increased GM-WM concentration) and negative (decreased GM-WM concentration) of IC2.
Figure 3. A covarying GM-WM network that differs from BPD and HC. Top: violin plots of the loading coefficients for GM and WM of the IC2. Central: network plot showing in green the strength of correlations between components. Bottom: brain plot of positive (increased GM-WM concentration) and negative (decreased GM-WM concentration) of IC2.
Sensors 23 02862 g003
Figure 4. Prediction of new cases. Random forest classification performance metrics.
Figure 4. Prediction of new cases. Random forest classification performance metrics.
Sensors 23 02862 g004
Figure 5. Brain plots from random forest analysis. Top: violin plots of the loading coefficients for GM and WM of the IC6. Bottom: brain plot of positive (increased GM - WM concentration) and negative (decreased GM-WM concentration) of IC6.
Figure 5. Brain plots from random forest analysis. Top: violin plots of the loading coefficients for GM and WM of the IC6. Bottom: brain plot of positive (increased GM - WM concentration) and negative (decreased GM-WM concentration) of IC6.
Sensors 23 02862 g005
Figure 6. Mediation analysis results. Emotional neglect and physical abuse predicted the IC2 (WM) network and that this in turn predicted symptoms in the impulsivity domain. Physical neglect and abuse predicted IC6 (GM) and this in turn predicted interpersonal symptoms. The colors indicate the same indirect effect linking a given child trauma (IV) to a specific symptom (DV) mediated by a specific IC (MV).
Figure 6. Mediation analysis results. Emotional neglect and physical abuse predicted the IC2 (WM) network and that this in turn predicted symptoms in the impulsivity domain. Physical neglect and abuse predicted IC6 (GM) and this in turn predicted interpersonal symptoms. The colors indicate the same indirect effect linking a given child trauma (IV) to a specific symptom (DV) mediated by a specific IC (MV).
Sensors 23 02862 g006
Table 1. Demographic data of the sample.
Table 1. Demographic data of the sample.
BPDHCp-Values
Participants2045
Age35.75 (±8.61)36.69 (±8.46)p = 0.401
GenderF = 17, M = 3F = 34, M = 11p = 0.647
Education≥8 years of formal education
Exclusion criteriaNeurological disease, psychoactive substance, pregnancy, MRI contraindications, previous head injuryNeurological disease, psychoactive substance, mental illness (SCID-II, SCID-IV), pregnancy, MRI contraindications, previous head injury
Table 2. Brain areas of IC2.
Table 2. Brain areas of IC2.
AreaBrodmann AreaVolume (cc)Random Effects: Max Value (x, y, z)
(A) IC2 Increased Grey Matter Concentration
Postcentral gyrus2, 3, 401.0/1.57.4 (−45, −26, 36)/10.0 (43, −24, 39)
Precentral gyrus130.1/0.54.2 (−45, −21, 37)/8.7 (46, −21, 37)
Angular gyrus*0.0/0.80 (0, 0, 0)/8.7 (40, −58, 33)
Sub-gyral371.3/2.06.4 (−22, 7, 47)/7.9 (40, −24, 36)
Middle temporal gyrus19, 211.9/0.17.9 (−39, −63, 22)/4.0 (58, −45, −2)
Insula13, 453.8/3.87.4 (−37, −4, 11)/6.3 (39, −11, 14)
Middle frontal gyrus6, 8, 110.4/1.85.4 (−24, 4, 44)/7.1 (25, 17, 41)
Precuneus7, 31, 390.5/2.45.9 (−16, −63, 21)/6.6 (13, −61, 38)
Cerebellar tonsil*1.2/1.26.4 (−27, −44, −42)/6.2 (33, −46, −39)
Superior parietal lobule70.3/0.45.9 (−25, −52, 43)/6.3 (30, −55, 43)
Superior frontal gyrus60.4/0.26.0 (−22, 11, 48)/4.1 (22, 15, 43)
Pyramis*0.4/0.64.4 (−9, −80, −23)/5.7 (3, −80, −25)
Inferior parietal lobule400.2/0.74.4 (−28, −49, 43)/5.7 (40, −59, 38)
Fusiform gyrus18, 36, 370.5/1.03.9 (−48, −42, −21)/5.7 (45, −43, −12)
Uncus (inc amygdala)20, 28, 360.4/0.05.6 (−30, −9, −29)/0 (0, 0, 0)
Extra-nuclear*0.9/0.15.6 (−34, 6, 5)/4.0 (37, −11, 7)
Medial frontal gyrus*0.2/0.05.6 (−24, 36, 27)/0 (0, 0, 0)
Culmen*0.9/0.25.4 (−1, −48, −1)/4.9 (3, −48, −1)
Claustrum*0.4/0.44.8 (−34, −10, 9)/4.4 (36, −4, 6)
Declive*0.2/0.53.9 (−4, −81, −21)/4.7 (6, −83, −20)
Superior temporal gyrus39, 41, 420.4/0.04.6 (−48, −24, 7)/0 (0, 0, 0)
Inferior frontal gyrus470.6/0.14.0 (−37, 25, 0)/4.6 (40, 6, 33)
(B) IC2 Decreased Grey Matter Concentration
Posterior cingulate30, 312.3/1.813.4 (−22, −58, 8)/11.6 (22, −64, 10)
Cuneus17, 18, 19, 23, 304.0/5.111.7 (−16, −69, 10)/13.0 (21, −68, 10)
Extra-nuclear*0.6/0.811.7 (−21, −53, 8)/10.7 (25, −55, 8)
Thalamus*3.8/2.09.3 (−10, −17, 9)/5.9 (9, −13, 8)
Lingual gyrus18, 193.7/1.49.1 (−18, −52, 5)/6.8 (22, −54, 5)
Lateral ventricle*0.3/0.56.0 (−28, −58, 8)/8.3 (28, −58, 8)
Middle temporal gyrus390.8/0.38.1 (−50, −55, 7)/5.2 (34, −72, 19)
Sub-gyral*0.3/1.04.7 (−27, −89, 2)/6.8 (28, −54, 5)
Cerebellar tonsil*0.0/0.60(0, 0, 0)/6.4 (12, −56, −41)
Anterior cingulate320.4/0.64.0 (−9, 26, 25)/5.8 (10, 41, 8)
Precuneus70.4/0.15.8 (−19, −62, 43)/4.2 (4, −73, 24)
Inferior parietal lobule400.3/0.55.7 (−40, −36, 57)/4.4 (53, −26, 28)
Middle frontal gyrus90.6/0.55.6 (−37, 15, 38)/5.2 (39, 18, 31)
Postcentral gyrus1, 2, 3, 401.5/0.15.6 (−48, −19, 51)/4.1 (64, −28, 21)
Middle occipital gyrus18, 190.4/0.15.4 (−28, −86, 4)/3.8 (34, −75, 16)
Inferior semi-lunar lobule*0.0/0.60 (0, 0, 0)/4.8 (9, −59, −41)
(C) IC2 Increased White Matter Concentration
Middle temporal gyrus37, 390.2/1.24.8 (−56, −56, 10)/9.9 (59, −51, −5)
Inferior temporal gyrus370.0/0.60 (0, 0, 0)/8.4 (62, −54, −5)
Middle frontal gyrus6, 8, 9, 10, 11, 462.2/3.65.5 (−45, 30, 39)/7.6 (49, 34, 33)
Inferior frontal gyrus10, 46, 471.4/0.07.1 (−48, 33, −13)/0 (0, 0, 0)
Superior frontal gyrus6, 8, 9, 101.2/1.86.5 (−19, 50, 38)/7.0 (22, 0, 65)
Superior temporal gyrus13, 22, 391.0/1.24.8 (−52, −16, 9)/6.9 (61, −49, 15)
Claustrum*0.3/0.45.7 (−34, −6, 6)/6.8 (36, −5, 7)
Insula131.7/1.16.5 (−36, −2, 8)/5.8 (36, −5, 11)
Postcentral gyrus1, 2, 3, 51.2/0.36.4 (−56, −27, 39)/5.2 (67, −14, 33)
Inferior parietal lobule402.2/1.56.0 (−46, −44, 48)/6.3 (52, −50, 43)
Extra-nuclear*0.6/0.84.6 (−34, −3, 3)/6.2 (36, −1, 7)
Precuneus70.6/0.35.7 (−9, −54, 48)/4.8 (22, −63, 46)
Superior parietal lobule70.0/0.40 (0, 0, 0)/5.1 (25, −60, 44)
Lentiform nucleus*0.6/0.34.5 (−15, 3, −5)/4.0 (15, 7, −4)
Cingulate gyrus23, 240.4/0.44.3 (−1, 3, 27)/4.5 (3, 1, 28)
Anterior cingulate24, 320.3/0.93.7 (−3, 35, 1)/4.3 (3, 35, 1)
(D) IC2 Decreased White Matter Concentration
Inferior parietal lobule7, 39, 400.8/2.78.1 (−28, −47, 56)/10.3 (34, −48, 56)
Sub-gyral7, 20, 401.9/2.29.6 (−28, −50, 54)/8.0 (31, −44, 51)
Superior parietal lobule71.0/0.69.5 (−30, −51, 58)/9.1 (34, −49, 61)
Precuneus7, 311.4/0.79.0 (−28, −50, 49)/6.0 (30, −47, 48)
Fusiform gyrus20, 36, 370.4/0.17.8 (−40, −17, −24)/4.1 (50, −42, −18)
Cuneus7, 17, 18, 19, 303.6/3.36.6 (−10, −76, 31)/6.7 (28, −83, 26)
Middle temporal gyrus19, 21, 390.1/1.33.7 (−62, −52, 0)/6.6 (55, −56, 8)
Lingual gyrus18, 191.5/0.85.8 (−21, −64, 1)/4.6 (22, −63, 2)
Postcentral gyrus1, 3, 50.8/0.05.6 (−43, −30, 62)/−999.0 (0, 0, 0)
Inferior occipital gyrus180.6/0.15.6 (−34, −89, −3)/4.2 (48, −80, −2)
Superior frontal gyrus9, 10, 111.2/1.05.5 (−15, 65, −10)/5.2 (16, 66, −13)
Inferior temporal gyrus200.5/0.15.5 (−43, −17, −27)/4.1 (50, −56, −12)
Middle occipital gyrus18, 19, 370.8/1.95.2 (−34, −89, 1)/5.2 (36, −79, 14)
Posterior cingulate300.5/0.75.1 (−21, −64, 6)/4.8 (19, −58, 7)
The first column indicates the name of the brain area, the second column indicates the name according to the Brodmann classification, the third column indicates the volume of grey or white matter concentration, and the fourth column indicates the coordinates of the peak for the area. * No Brodmann area was detected by the system.
Table 3. Brain areas of IC6.
Table 3. Brain areas of IC6.
AreaBrodmann AreaVolume (cc)Random Effects: Max Value (x, y, z)
(A). ICGM6 Increased Grey Matter Concentration
Postcentral gyrus1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 7, 40, 436.1/7.49.3 (−52, −20, 30)/9.8 (53, −17, 31)
Middle temporal gyrus21, 22, 392.9/1.09.7 (−49, −41, 5)/6.2 (50, −43, 9)
Precuneus7, 19, 391.1/2.25.0 (−21, −62, 42)/9.0 (30, −61, 35)
Parahippocampal gyrus19, 36, 373.3/2.08.7 (−28, −43, −7)/7.7 (30, −44, −5)
Inferior parietal lobule404.2/5.45.9 (−55, −23, 30)/8.7 (55, −28, 26)
Precentral gyrus4, 6, 9, 13, 430.9/2.26.0 (−36, 4, 29)/8.4 (50, −17, 34)
Superior temporal gyrus22, 39, 41, 423.4/3.48.2 (−48, −49, 13)/8.1 (53, −45, 12)
Superior parietal lobule70.4/1.05.9 (−22, −59, 44)/8.2 (27, −58, 43)
Sub-gyral431.6/3.17.0 (−46, −43, 5)/8.1 (30, −64, 32)
Inferior frontal gyrus90.5/0.07.8 (−39, 4, 32)/0 (0, 0, 0)
Fusiform gyrus19, 20, 371.2/0.67.6 (−30, −36, −12)/5.6 (28, −47, −8)
Insula13, 410.1/1.93.8 (−46, −15, 12)/7.1 (46, −22, 16)
Angular gyrus390.9/0.06.5 (−42, −58, 32)/0 (0, 0, 0)
Transverse temporal gyrus41, 420.1/0.73.9 (−53, −14, 12)/6.4 (48, −21, 12)
Supramarginal gyrus401.3/0.26.4 (−39, −53, 27)/4.8 (52, −48, 22)
Anterior cingulate24, 321.3/0.46.3 (−3, 32, −7)/4.9 (3, 29, −10)
Cuneus18, 190.2/1.34.0 (−7, −79, 14)/5.1 (10, −88, 14)
Culmen*1.3/2.64.7 (−22, −41, −12)/4.9 (31, −39, −22)
Cingulate gyrus310.9/0.34.7 (−1, −32, 36)/4.4 (1, −32, 39)
Lingual gyrus18, 190.8/0.14.6 (−18, −55, −2)/3.6 (22, −53, −2)
Declive*0.8/0.04.1 (−18, −59, −13)/0 (0, 0, 0)
(B). ICGM6 Decreased Grey Matter Concentration
Sub-gyral60.8/0.86.4 (−21, 4, 51)/5.2 (25, −6, 56)
Middle temporal gyrus*0.1/0.43.8 (−48, −63, 2)/5.8 (42, −66, 16)
Middle frontal gyrus6, 80.9/1.94.7 (−33, −2, 43)/5.6 (28, −6, 53)
Fusiform gyrus200.1/0.54.0 (−42, −33, −16)/5.0 (43, −27, −18)
Medial frontal gyrus60.6/0.24.9 (−18, 7, 51)/4.5 (10, −10, 61)
Superior frontal gyrus60.5/0.34.4 (−13, −11, 63)/4.1 (18, −7, 63)
(C). ICWM6 Increased White Matter Concentration
Middle frontal gyrus9, 461.4/0.110.2 (−46, 31, 33)/4.1 (48, 39, 26)
Precuneus7, 192.8/2.05.8 (−22, −74, 41)/9.9 (24, −70, 49)
Superior parietal lobule70.2/0.84.8 (−39, −57, 51)/8.8 (25, −69, 45)
Inferior parietal lobule39, 401.2/4.46.2 (−62, −40, 24)/8.4 (49, −42, 56)
Middle occipital gyrus190.9/0.07.3 (−39, −85, 17)/0 (0, 0, 0)
Precentral gyrus4, 6, 440.8/4.15.8 (−61, −6, 21)/7.3 (55, −17, 35)
Supramarginal gyrus400.7/0.27.1 (−62, −42, 27)/4.5 (49, −36, 34)
Postcentral gyrus1, 2, 3, 7, 40, 432.0/3.95.6 (−43, −29, 53)/7.1 (53, −16, 31)
Superior temporal gyrus21, 22, 41, 420.4/2.25.0 (−65, −40, 21)/7.0 (67, −27, 15)
Cuneus17, 190.4/0.64.2 (−7, −92, 3)/6.1 (31, −82, 30)
Sub-gyral*0.7/0.36.1 (−46, −36, −13)/5.0 (18, −45, 62)
Transverse temporal gyrus410.0/0.40 (0, 0, 0)/5.9 (48, −29, 12)
Insula130.0/0.60 (0, 0, 0)/5.8 (52, −30, 18)
Middle temporal gyrus19, 21, 390.8/0.35.7 (−50, −75, 19)/5.3 (49, −63, 21)
Lingual gyrus17, 190.8/0.05.1 (−22, −64, 1)/0 (0, 0, 0)
Superior frontal gyrus6, 8, 9, 100.4/0.14.6 (−22, 46, 39)/3.7 (12, 14, 50)
(D). ICWM6 Decreased White Matter Concentration
Precentral gyrus6, 91.3/0.18.1 (−37, −6, 62)/3.7 (39, 16, 35)
Superior frontal gyrus6, 8, 9, 10, 115.1/3.07.3 (−10, −4, 67)/8.0 (25, −1, 66)
Middle frontal gyrus6, 8, 9, 10, 467.7/2.67.6 (−34, −3, 62)/7.0 (31, −3, 61)
Middle occipital gyrus19, 370.4/0.16.5 (−49, −67, 5)/3.8 (27, −70, 6)
Superior parietal lobule70.4/0.06.4 (−28, −54, 61)/0 (0, 0, 0)
Medial frontal gyrus6, 111.3/0.75.2 (−13, −4, 61)/6.4 (15, 1, 58)
Middle temporal gyrus21, 37, 390.2/0.65.5 (−46, −64, 6)/5.2 (68, −32, −7)
Sub-gyral61.6/0.34.6 (−25, 15, 41)/5.3 (18, −2, 57)
Insula130.4/0.44.5 (−36, −5, 10)/4.6 (37, 0, 8)
Cingulate gyrus320.3/0.53.8 (−18, 8, 44)/4.4 (15, 16, 36)
The first column indicates the name of the brain area, the second column indicates the name according to the Brodmann classification, the third column indicates the volume of grey or white matter concentration, and the fourth column indicates the coordinates of the peak for the area. * No Brodmann area was detected by the system.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Grecucci, A.; Dadomo, H.; Salvato, G.; Lapomarda, G.; Sorella, S.; Messina, I. Abnormal Brain Circuits Characterize Borderline Personality and Mediate the Relationship between Childhood Traumas and Symptoms: A mCCA+jICA and Random Forest Approach. Sensors 2023, 23, 2862. https://doi.org/10.3390/s23052862

AMA Style

Grecucci A, Dadomo H, Salvato G, Lapomarda G, Sorella S, Messina I. Abnormal Brain Circuits Characterize Borderline Personality and Mediate the Relationship between Childhood Traumas and Symptoms: A mCCA+jICA and Random Forest Approach. Sensors. 2023; 23(5):2862. https://doi.org/10.3390/s23052862

Chicago/Turabian Style

Grecucci, Alessandro, Harold Dadomo, Gerardo Salvato, Gaia Lapomarda, Sara Sorella, and Irene Messina. 2023. "Abnormal Brain Circuits Characterize Borderline Personality and Mediate the Relationship between Childhood Traumas and Symptoms: A mCCA+jICA and Random Forest Approach" Sensors 23, no. 5: 2862. https://doi.org/10.3390/s23052862

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop