Next Article in Journal
Improved Diver Communication System by Combining Optical and Electromagnetic Trackers
Previous Article in Journal
EEG-Based BCI Emotion Recognition: A Survey
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Sensor Location Optimization of Wireless Wearable fNIRS System for Cognitive Workload Monitoring Using a Data-Driven Approach for Improved Wearability

Human Cyber-Physical Systems Laboratory, Florida International University, Miami, FL 33174, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sensors 2020, 20(18), 5082; https://doi.org/10.3390/s20185082
Submission received: 26 July 2020 / Revised: 31 August 2020 / Accepted: 31 August 2020 / Published: 7 September 2020
(This article belongs to the Section Wearables)

Abstract

:
Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS) is a hemodynamic modality in human cognitive workload assessment receiving popularity due to its easier implementation, non-invasiveness, low cost and other benefits from the signal-processing point of view. Wearable wireless fNIRS systems used in research have promisingly shown that fNIRS could be used in cognitive workload assessment in out-of-the-lab scenarios, such as in operators’ cognitive workload monitoring. In such a scenario, the wearability of the system is a significant factor affecting user comfort. In this respect, the wearability of the system can be improved if it is possible to minimize an fNIRS system without much compromise of the cognitive workload detection accuracy. In this study, cognitive workload-related hemodynamic changes were acquired using an fNIRS system covering the whole forehead, which is the region of interest in most cognitive workload-monitoring studies. A machine learning approach was applied to explore how the mean accuracy of the cognitive workload classification accuracy varied across various sensing locations on the forehead such as the Left, Mid, Right, Left-Mid, Right-Mid and Whole forehead. The statistical significance analysis result showed that the Mid location could result in significant cognitive workload classification accuracy compared to Whole forehead sensing, with a statistically insignificant difference in the mean accuracy. Thus, the wearable fNIRS system can be improved in terms of wearability by optimizing the sensor location, considering the sensing of the Mid location on the forehead for cognitive workload monitoring.

1. Introduction

The invention of the wheel introduced a new role for humankind, the operator. This job requires humans to take into account the current state of the system being operated and the current environmental situation the system is in, and to then perform cognitively assessed actuating commands to operate the system effectively and safely. From riding a bicycle to operating an aircraft, these jobs exert various levels of cognitive load on the operator depending on the systems. The safety of the human users accompanying the operator is highly dependent on the continuous cognitive effort of the operator, which is also denoted as the cognitive workload on the operator. For instance, a study on aviation crashes [1] based on 329 major airline crashes claimed that 38% of the crashes were probably caused due to pilot error. Thus, ensuring a balanced and continuous human operator’s cognitive effort via monitoring an operator’s cognitive workload could improve the safety of such mission-critical operations by reducing the probability of human error.
In an effort to improve safety by assessing and balancing operators’ cognitive workload, subjective tests, such as [2] NASA-TLX, have been used widely, which can only be performed after the operator has completed the task and based on his experience during the task. The technological advancement in the modalities named Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI), Electroencephalography (EEG) and Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS) in about the last three decades brings a new dimension to cognitive workload monitoring. Now, it can be even monitored while the operator is performing the task. Several studies [3,4,5,6] on cognitive workload assessment have already promisingly showed that physiological signals acquired using fMRI, EEG, and fNIRS or a combination of these modalities [7,8], which are faster than the subjective tests, can be used to monitor cognitive workloads.
The increased neural activity [9] due to the increased cognitive workload increases the oxygen consumption in the cerebral cortex, which is later regulated by the brain’s control mechanism called glial regulation [10]. This blood oxygen-dependent phenomenon can be measured using fMRI [11]. Although fMRI is considered as the de facto standard in detecting cerebral regional blood oxygen concentration change, the technological limitations such as the huge size, high cost, high system complexity, high artefact susceptibility, and low temporal resolution greatly reduce its usability in out-of-the-lab environments. On the other hand, fNIRS is a less expensive technology with higher portability and higher temporal resolution but with a comparable result to fMRI [12,13,14,15,16] in the detection of local cerebral oxygenation changes, although there is some compromise of the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) and spatial resolution. In the case of EEG application in cognitive workload assessment [5,17,18], though it is capable of a high temporal resolution, it is less portable and less immune to ambient electrical noise. This drawback due to electrical noise is not present in fNIRS due to its optical nature of technological implementation.
In research into operators’ cognitive workload assessment, fNIRS has successfully been used in various studies in actual or simulated environments, such as car driving [19,20], train driving [21] and flight simulation [22]. The recent studies relating to cognitive tasks [23,24] demonstrated that even wearable fNIRS systems can detect hemodynamic changes associated with the cognitive workload. Moreover, the wearability of the fNIRS system enables such experiments to be conducted in real-life scenarios, such as during walking, driving a vehicle and outdoor navigation [19,23,24,25,26]. These studies are highly influential in the field of engineering applications utilizing the knowledge from cognitive science via fNIRS and also experimentally prove that fNIRS can be implemented in out-of-the-lab situations. All the above experiments were conducted using systems that sensed the whole forehead for cognitive workload detection. In that case, the wearability of the system would significantly degrade the applicability of the modality. In other words, wearing a system on the forehead for several minutes or hours may be acceptable in the case of experiments. However, in the case of the practical application of fNIRS for operators’ cognitive workload detection during their whole-day working period, the wearability of the system with respect to the system dimensions, weight and duration of operation on a single battery recharge are challenges to be taken into account. The research question is how to improve the wearability of fNIRS devices for lengthy cognitive workload monitoring while maintaining the assessment accuracy. In this regard, we hypothesized that the size of a wearable fNIRS system could be minimized while maintaining a high cognitive workload detection accuracy. The graphical abstract of this study is depicted by Figure 1.
During the cognitive load, the portion of the cognitive system that retains the information in the short term, necessary for processing to accomplish the cognitive tasks, is known as the working memory [27], which is widely assumed to be served by the Prefrontal Cortex (PFC) along with central executive function. Several studies related to cognitive function conform with this assumption from the observation of high hemodynamic activities in the PFC during the cognitive workloads [3,9,14,28,29]. This observation of regional cerebral activation during cognitive tasks leads to the implementation of the fNIRS modality in assessing cognitive workload [6,30,31,32] by sensing the PFC optically. These studies focused mainly on anatomically exploring the activation of the brain region by various cognitive processes at deep or shallow depth from the skin. Thus, in such studies, the optimization of sensor locations regarding the wearability of the fNIRS system was not considered. Thereafter, cognitive workload assessment studies focused on fNIRS typically sense the entire front area to identify the workload. To the best of the knowledge of the authors, there is no study addressing this problem of optimization and determining whether it is necessary to sense the whole forehead for the successful detection of cognitive workloads. Furthermore, the compromise of the precision of cognitive workload detection if only a certain smaller portion of the forehead is sensed remains unaddressed.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Eight healthy volunteers (six males and two females), with no history of neurovascular and cognitive disorders, participated in this study. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB-19-0091) of Florida International University, and signed informed consent was obtained from all the subjects prior to the study.

2.2. Experimental Design

To induce a cognitive workload in human subjects, the n-back task is a widely used [6,7,28] paradigm related to cognitive study in research, which was first demonstrated by Kirchner, W.K. [33]. In this paradigm, the subject observes a series of events during the testing period. If any of the events match with n-events before then, the subject provides feedback, where n could be 1, 2, 3 and so forth, depending on the requirement. As the target of this study is to explore how the detection accuracy for cognitive workload varies due to the sensing location on the human forehead, a moderate level of cognitive workload induction was applied, which is assumed to be represented by the 2-back test [34]. A free, open-source piece of software [35] named “Brain Workshop” was used to simulate the positional 2-back task, where a solid colored square changed its position randomly within a 3-by-3 grid every two seconds (Figure 2 depicts this task paradigm). If the current position of the square matched its position two events before, the subject pressed a button on the keyboard, and they did nothing if the position did not match. In each session of recording, there were 24 events of the positional 2-back task, which spanned 48 s, and afterward, there was 25 s of relaxing, when the subjects did nothing. This relaxing period was considered as the Rest state [7]. The recording of each session started 10 s prior to the start of the 2-back task, and this 10 s was used as the baseline of the diffused optical signal in the conversion of the absorption of optical signals to hemodynamic change. Each subject did 10 such sessions, where the subjects relaxed for about 30 s between the sessions, and during these periods, data were not recorded and the subjects were free to move. The sessions with less than 90% accuracy for 2-back task performance were rejected during the recording [7], assuming less cognitive involvement of the subject in the task.

2.3. Data Acquisition and Signal Processing

A wearable wireless fNIRS system developed at the Human Cyber-Physical Systems Lab at Florida International University was used for data acquisition in the experiment. The system architecture was based on the sensor system developed in [36,37,38] and modified to accommodate more channels required to sense the whole forehead. The improved system consists of three light-emitting diodes (LED) as a source of near-infrared (NIR) light, capable of multiwavelength (770 and 850 nm) emission, and eight photodetectors (PD) as light detectors, where the source–detector distance is 3 cm, with 0.3 cm variability. The differential path length (DPF) was 6.2 cm for 770 nm and 5.1 cm for 850 nm [39]. The LED and PD together form twelve channels for sensing, which are marked by channel numbers in Figure 3a. The sensitivity map for the depicted channel arrangement is presented in Figure 3b, derived from Homer2 Atlas Viewer [40,41]. The system covers the whole forehead for sensing, which is the region of interest (ROI) in this study. This ROI is subdivided into five sub-locations named Left, Mid, Right, Left-Mid and Right-Mid. Channels 1 to 4 sense the Left location on the forehead, Channels 5 to 8 sense the Mid location on the forehead, and the remaining Channels 9 to 12 sense the Right location on the forehead. The locations stated as Left-Mid and Right-Mid consist of Channels 1 to 10 and Channels 3 to 12, respectively. The location name for the whole forehead sensing area is Whole in subsequent descriptions. Each channel was sampled at a 25 Hz sampling rate. Additionally, the headband that houses the system is equipped with a nine-channel inertial measurement unit (IMU) and records the movement data concurrently with NIR data. IMU data were checked immediately after each session from each subject for movements during the recording, and the sessions that showed movements were discarded. The raw NIR signals were low-pass filtered with a third-order Butterworth bandpass filter with a 0.01–0.5 Hz cut-off frequency [42], and afterward, a two-second windowed moving-average filter was applied to further remove any physiological interference in the detected NIR signal, such as Mayer waves, respiration and heart rate [43,44]. Afterward, the modified Beer–Lambert [45] law was applied to convert the multiband raw NIR signal to oxygenation change signals, known as the change in oxygenated hemoglobin (ΔHbO2) and deoxygenated hemoglobin (ΔHbR).
To study how the workload detection accuracy varied with the length of sensing period along with the sensing location on the forehead, the oxygenation change signals were segmented with various window lengths, such as 5, 10, 20, 25 and 48 s [7]. There was about 50% overlap [7] in the windowing when the segmentation window length was less than the whole period of the 2-back or Rest state. The overlapping is assumed to be necessary to reduce inter-subject variability in the statistical temporal features of the signal [7,46]. After segmentation, each signal segment was labelled as 2-back or Resting-state accordingly.
This segmentation process resulted in n segments of 2-back state signal and m segments of resting-state signals. Here the values of n and m were dependent on the segmentation window lengths used in this study. For each session of any subject, it resulted in (n, m) = (1, 11) when segmenting with a 5 s window, (n, m) = (9, 5) for a 10 s window, (n, m) = (4, 2) for a 20 s window, (n, m) = (2, 1) for a 25 s window and (n, m) = (1, 1) when using the whole period of each state. These values of n and m also signify the number of samples in each class at different segmentation window lengths. All sessions of each subject, after segmentation and appropriate class-label assignment, resulted in 290, 140, 60, 30 and 20 samples for the segmentation window lengths of 5, 10, 20 and 25 s and the whole state period, respectively.

2.4. Feature Extraction

From each segmented hemodynamic change signal sample, commonly used statistical features were extracted, such as the mean [47,48], variance [49], slope [47,48] of polynomial fit, skewness [49,50], kurtosis [49,50] and correlation [7] of ΔHbO2 and ΔHbR. The extraction of these features from each channel resulted in a total of 11 features per channel for any samples under any segmentation window length. As there were various numbers of channels involved in sensing different locations on the forehead, various numbers of features resulted from each sample depending on the sensing location on the forehead. For example, there were 44 features for the Left, Mid and Right locations, whereas there were 110 features for the Left-Mid and Right-Mid locations for any length of segmentation. As there were 12 channels involved in the whole forehead location, there were 132 features recorded in the location denoted by Whole. The supplementary file to this article contains all the feature values segregated according to different sensing locations and segmentation window lengths.

2.5. Feature Selection and Classification

In the case of the fNIRS-based classification problem, several classification methods have been used, such as Discriminant analysis, Support Vector Machine (SVM), Artificial Neural Network (ANN) and so on [50,51,52,53,54,55,56]. As the pivotal point of the study was to assess the cognitive workload monitoring accuracy variability using wearable fNIRS at various sensing locations on the forehead, the speed of the classifier was a crucial property for consideration. Thus, the Linear SVM, which had already been used in several other fNIRS-based classification studies [7,52,57], was selected as a classifier in this study. In this respect, the generic implementation of Linear SVM in the MATLAB platform with a box constraint of 1 and auto kernel scale was used for classification. For feature selection, two algorithms were used, namely, the Sequential Forward Selection (SFS) Wrapper algorithm [58] for feature subset selection and Relief algorithm [59,60]. Each of these algorithms has its own merits with respect to the statistical relevance (Relief) of the features to the classes [59] and the interaction of the training feature set (SFS) with the classifier algorithm [58]. Thus, both of the selected feature sets returned by these two algorithms were individually used in the classification using the linear SVM. The classification with the best accuracy among these two classification results was used in the statistical analysis. In this respect, as the SFS claims to return an optimal feature subset by heuristic search, the whole subset of the returned features was used in classification. On the other hand, as the Relief algorithm instead returns the ranks and weights for all the features, only the features with positive weights were used in the classification.
In the classification process, ten-fold testing cross-validation was applied [61]. In other words, there were 240 datasets resulting from segmentation, and in each of the classification processes on these datasets, the dataset was partitioned into ten subsets using a random selection of the observations for each partition. Then, the SVM was trained using the nine subsets of this dataset, leaving out the remaining one subset for testing. This leave-out subset is never seen by the classifier during the training phase. The training of the SVM was performed using ten-fold training cross-validation using those nine subsets of the partitioned data. Afterward, the trained SVM classifier was used to test the classification accuracy using the subset not used in training. Subsequently, this same training and testing procedure was applied on the other remaining nine subsets of this dataset in a nested cross-validation. The final accuracy of the classification on this dataset was calculated as the mean of these ten testing accuracies.

2.6. Statistical Analysis

For all the eight subjects, cognitive workload classification accuracies were calculated for the sensing locations of Left, Mid, Right, Left-Mid, Right-Mid and Whole, under several segmentation windows. The mean classification accuracies for each location across all the subjects were different from each other. Thus, to find the statistical significance of the differences in the mean classification accuracies for all subjects for each location (Left, Mid, Right, Left-Mid and Right-Mid) from the accuracy for the whole forehead location (Whole), a two-sample t-test was applied with a 5% significance level, which decided whether the means were statistically equal or not at that significance level.

3. Results and Discussion

All the classification accuracies from all the datasets are presented in Table 1. For the 5 s segmentation window, the lowest mean accuracy for all the subjects was 83.4%, with a standard deviation of 6.7%, which occurred when only the Right location was used. On the other hand, the highest mean accuracy occurred when the Whole forehead dataset was used to classify, which was 94.0%, with a 3.9% standard error. Like in the 5 s segmentation window, the Right location in all the other segmentation windows also resulted in the lowest classification accuracies, which were 84.0%, 86.5%, 90.8% and 95.0% for the 10, 15, 20, 25 and 48 s window lengths, respectively. For the 48 s segmentation window, the Left location also resulted in the lowest classification accuracy, like the Right location, but with a higher standard deviation, which was 6.5%. In the case of the highest mean accuracy of classifications beyond the 5 s segmentation window length, the Left-Mid location yielded the highest mean accuracy for the 20, 25 and 48 s segmentation window lengths, which were 93.6%, 95.9 and 98.3% respectively, and for the remaining 10 s segmentation window length, Right-Mid resulted in the highest mean accuracy of classification, which was 93.0%. The other details of the classification process, such as the F1 score, sensitivity, specificity and precision are listed in the table in Appendix A for each classification performed in this study. Similar to the mean classification accuracy results from the calculation process mentioned before, the classifier details described in Appendix A were also calculated.
Regarding the statistical analysis aforementioned, the two-sample t-test was used to test the statistical significance of the differences in the mean classification accuracies for each location (Left, Mid, Right, Left-Mid and Right-Mid) from that for the whole forehead location, denoted by Whole. The t-test hypothesis-testing decisions indicated that the mean classification accuracies were statistically different from the mean classification accuracy of the location Whole in the case of only the 10 and 20 s segmentation window lengths for location Right and the 5 s segmentation window length for locations Left and Right. In other words, except for these four cases, the mean classification accuracy for any location or segmentation window lengths, statistical significance was not found for the difference in the mean classification accuracies. Moreover, these results indicate that the Mid location, which is one of the three smallest sensing locations (Left, Mid, Right), resulted in a mean classification accuracy with a statistically insignificant difference, compared to the largest location Whole for any segmentation window lengths.
The mean classification accuracies are depicted by the bar plot in Figure 4 for a qualitative assessment of how the classification accuracies varied along with the change in the sensing location on the forehead, which are grouped into segmentation windows. The error bars in the plot show the standard errors of the mean accuracies, which were estimated by dividing the standard deviation by the square root of the number of subjects in this study. From this plot, it is apparent that the differences in the mean accuracies between the largest location Whole and the other smaller locations were reduced with the increase in the segmentation window length.
Optical methods, such as fNIRS, which can reach a depth of a few centimeters only from the skin to the human brain, could only sense hemodynamic changes of the brain tissue that are closer to the cerebral surface, such as the PFC under the forehead. The PFC is one of the most functionally correlated subsystems [62] among other cerebral subsystems such as the Hippocampal Formation (HF), Inferior Parietal Lobule (IPL) and so on, which form the default network with respect to cognitive states. Moreover, research based on other methods such as fMRI and Positron Emission Tomography (PET) showed that the Medial PFC in this network is the most involved region of the PFC related to the cognitive states [63]. In this study, the n-back task was used to induce cognitive workload, which entails the involvement of the brain function named working memory. However, the outcome of any task execution by the human brain is accompanied by the other parts of human cognition such as memory retrieval, relational reasoning and multitasking behaviors. Other studies suggest that the frontopolar cortex (FPC), which we assumed to be sensed by the Mid location depicted in this study, is responsible for carrying out these parts of human cognition [64,65,66,67]. In congruence with those physiological study-based findings, the statistical results presented in this study also showed that sensing the Mid location only can result in significant accuracy in cognitive workload classification compared to using the location Whole on the forehead. Thus, from the system design point of view, the fNIRS system could be minimized to sense only the Mid location on the forehead with a statistically insignificant compromise of the cognitive workload detection accuracy. Similarly to the Mid location, the other two smallest sensing locations, Left and Right, could also be targeted for cognitive workload detection using a minimized fNIRS system but with higher latencies for obtaining significantly comparable accuracy.

4. Conclusions

The fNIRS is a promising modality for the ubiquitous monitoring of human cognitive workload. For the potential deployment of fNIRS, device design optimization regarding sensor position to boost wearability toward this aim of cognitive workload tracking is essential. In this respect, statistically significant classification accuracy for cognitive workloads can be achieved by sensing the Mid location on the human forehead rather than the entire forehead using fNIRS. This finding can be utilized by researchers to optimize their wearable wireless fNIRS systems, which are resource and power constrained by nature, as well as user comfort being a concern. While the purpose of this study was to investigate how the cognitive workload monitoring accuracy varied across the sensing locations on the forehead, the variability arising from the human subjects was assumed to be minimal, and the number of participants in this study was decided by considering common practice in other relevant studies. Future studies may investigate whether there are any subject-dependent variabilities in sensor location optimization for wearable fNIRS system design. The extracerebral tissue layer signal interferences were assumed to be minimal and similar in both the 2-back and resting-state signals. A future study may utilize a short separation channel to investigate these interferences. In this study, only the immobile state of the subject was considered. Thus, another promising future direction might be to investigate the effect of motion on sensor location optimization.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/1424-8220/20/18/5082/s1. Data file D1: feature values.

Author Contributions

M.R.S., R.A., J.S.M., S.M.S.H. and O.B. conceived and designed the experiments; M.R.S., R.A., J.S.M. and R.R. performed the experiments; M.R.S., R.R. and O.B. contributed by designing and building the hardware; M.R.S., S.M.S.H. and O.B. analyzed the data; M.R.S. wrote the paper. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research is partly supported by National Science Foundation (CNS-1552163).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Details of Classifiers
Table A1. Details of Classifiers (Subjects 1–4).
Table A1. Details of Classifiers (Subjects 1–4).
Subject 1Subject 2Subject 3Subject 4
AccuracyF1 ScoreSensitivitySpecificityPrecisionAccuracyF1 ScoreSensitivitySpecificityPrecisionAccuracyF1 ScoreSensitivitySpecificityPrecisionAccuracyF1 ScoreSensitivitySpecificityPrecision
05 second windowLeft82.48689728487.2909475868089.887879384.188897586
Mid88.39091808995.5999996988789.887879376.988897586
Right84.59091808980.0999996987280.189447385.288897586
Left-Mid89.39294818998.3999996988789.887879383.888897586
Right-Mid89.09193829097.6999996989089.887879385.288897586
ALL92.19091808998.6999996989189.887879389.392948290
10 second windowLeft82.99497829188.69910096987989.083929589.390927688
Mid92.19497829194.39910096987179.989387375.090927688
Right85.09497829190.79910096987289.083929580.086925880
Left-Mid91.49497829198.69910096988689.083929586.490927688
Right-Mid89.39497829193.69910096988989.083929590.790927688
ALL91.49497829199.39910096988189.083929585.790927688
20 second windowLeft86.79293759193.39910095988392.1100608593.394958594
Mid95.092937591100.09910095988292.1100608570.094958594
Right83.39293759193.39910095987080.485407686.794958594
Left-Mid88.39395759198.39910095988792.1100608591.794958594
Right-Mid90.09293759195.09910095988592.1100608593.394958594
ALL93.39293759195.09910095988592.1100608590.094958594
25 second windowLeft100.0979510010096.71001001001009090.695608793.3971008093
Mid93.39795100100100.01001001001008390.695608786.7971008093
Right83.3979510010096.71001001001007790.695608790.0971008093
Left-Mid96.79795100100100.01001001001008390.695608793.3971008093
Right-Mid100.09795100100100.01001001001008090.695608793.3971008093
ALL100.09795100100100.01001001001008390.695608790.0971008093
48 second windowLeft100.010010010010095.01001001001008597.41009095100.0100100100100
Mid100.0100100100100100.01001001001009097.4100909590.0100100100100
Right90.010010010010095.01001001001009097.4100909595.0100100100100
Left-Mid100.0100100100100100.01001001001009597.41009095100.0100100100100
Right-Mid100.0100100100100100.01001001001009097.4100909595.0100100100100
ALL100.0100100100100100.01001001001009097.41009095100.0100100100100
Table A2. Details of Classifiers (Subjects 5–8).
Table A2. Details of Classifiers (Subjects 5–8).
Subject 5Subject 6Subject 7Subject 8
AccuracyF1 ScoreSensitivitySpecificityPrecisionAccuracyF1 ScoreSensitivitySpecificityPrecisionAccuracyF1 ScoreSensitivitySpecificityPrecisionAccuracyF1 ScoreSensitivitySpecificityPrecision
05 second windowLeft94.19897969891.49797959790.09494909578.383837082
Mid92.19897969897.99797959788.99494909584.188927184
Right93.19897969887.69797959787.99494909577.282856580
Left-Mid96.99897969896.29797959792.49494909590.793948591
Right-Mid99.39897969896.99797959793.49494909588.391957788
ALL98.69897969898.39797959793.19494909590.393968189
10 second windowLeft94.39696929691.49494869390.79697879484.387916684
Mid91.49493889497.99899949792.29697879487.187916684
Right96.49796969885.08989788882.29697879480.787916684
Left-Mid97.19898969897.19899949793.69697879487.992937890
Right-Mid99.39796969897.99899949794.49697879490.087916684
ALL97.99796969898.69899949792.79697879491.487916684
20 second windowLeft98.3999810010096.79898959886.69795959888.396988594
Mid98.39998100100100.09898959894.79795959893.396988594
Right93.3999810010090.09898959885.29795959890.096988594
Left-Mid98.3999810010096.79898959895.29795959893.396988594
Right-Mid100.0999810010096.79898959894.79795959888.396988594
ALL100.0999810010093.39898959895.29796959893.396988594
25 second windowLeft100.010010010010086.79295709096.798979010093.3981009097
Mid100.0100100100100100.010010010010096.798979010090.0981009097
Right100.010010010010093.310010010010096.798979010090.0981009097
Left-Mid100.0100100100100100.010010010010097.598979010096.7981009097
Right-Mid100.0100100100100100.010010010010096.798979010093.3981009097
ALL96.7100100100100100.010010010010093.398979010090.0951007090
48 second windowLeft100.010010010010085.010010010010095.0979590100100.0909010090
Mid100.0100100100100100.010010010010090.097959010095.0909010090
Right100.010010010010095.0100100100100100.01001009010095.0909010090
Left-Mid100.0100100100100100.010010010010096.797959010095.0909010090
Right-Mid100.0100100100100100.010010010010095.0979590100100.0909010090
ALL100.0100100100100100.010010010010095.0979590100100.0909010090

References

  1. Li, G.; Baker, S.P.; Grabowski, J.G.; Rebok, G.W. Factors Associated with Pilot Error in Aviation Crashes. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 2001, 72, 52–58. [Google Scholar]
  2. Hart, S.G.; Staveland, L.E. Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): Results of empirical and theoretical research. Adv. Psychol. 1988, 52, 139–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Cohen, J.D.; Perlstein, W.M.; Braver, T.S.; Nystrom, L.E.; Noll, D.C.; Jonides, J.; Smith, E.E. Temporal dynamics of Brain activation during a working memory task. Nature 1997, 386, 604–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Heekeren, H.R.; Marrett, S.; Bandettini, P.A.; Ungerleider, L.G. A general mechanism for perceptual decision-making in the human brain. Nature 2004, 431, 859–862. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Berka, C.; Levendowski, D.J.; Lumicao, M.N.; Yau, A.; Davis, G.; Zivkovic, V.T.; Olmstead, R.E.; Tremoulet, P.D.; Craven, P.L. EEG Correlates of Task Engagement and Mental Workload in Vigilance, Learning, and Memory Tasks. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 2007, 78, B231–B244. [Google Scholar]
  6. Herff, C.; Heger, D.; Fortmann, O.; Hennrich, J.; Putze, F.; Schultz, T. Mental workload during n-back task—quantified in the prefrontal cortex using fNIRS. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014, 7, 935. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  7. Aghajani, H.; Garbey, M.; Omurtag, A. Measuring Mental Workload with EEG+fNIRS. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2017, 11, 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Coffey, E.B.J.; Brouwer, A.-M.; Van Erp, J.B.F. Measuring workload using a combination of electroencephalography and near infrared spectroscopy. Proc. Hum. Factors Ergon. Soc. Annu. Meet. 2012, 56, 1822–1826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Rypma, B.; D’Esposito, M. The roles of prefrontal brain regions in components of working memory: Effects of memory load and individual differences. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1999, 96, 6558–6563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Iadecola, C.; Nedergaard, M. Glial regulation of the cerebral microvasculature. Nat. Neurosci. 2007, 10, 1369–1376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Ogawa, S.; Lee, T.M.; Kay, A.R.; Tank, D.W. Brain magnetic resonance imaging with contrast dependent on blood oxygenation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1990, 87, 9868–9872. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Cui, X.; Bray, S.; Bryant, D.M.; Glover, G.H.; Reiss, A.L. A quantitative comparison of NIRS and fMRI across multiple cognitive tasks. Neuroimage 2011, 54, 2808–2821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Erdoğan, S.B.; Yücel, M.A.; Akın, A. Analysis of task-evoked systemic interference in fNIRS measurements: Insights from fMRI. Neuroimage 2014, 87, 490–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Fishburn, F.A.; Norr, M.E.; Medvedev, A.V.; Vaidya, C.J. Sensitivity of fNIRS to cognitive state and load. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Barker, J.W.; Aarabi, A.; Huppert, T.J. Autoregressive model based algorithm for correcting motion and serially correlated errors in fNIRS. Biomed. Opt. Express 2013, 4, 1366–1379. Available online: https://www.osapublishing.org/boe/abstract.cfm?uri=boe-4-8-1366 (accessed on 15 July 2020). [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Gagnon, L.; Yücel, M.A.; Dehaes, M.; Cooper, R.J.; Perdue, K.L.; Selb, J.; Huppert, T.J.; Hoge, R.d.; Boas, D.A. Quantification of the cortical contribution to the NIRS signal over the motor cortex using concurrent NIRS-fMRI measurements. Neuroimage 2012, 59, 3933–3940. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Gevins, A.; Smith, M.E.; McEvoy, L.; Yu, D. High-resolution EEG mapping of cortical activation related to working memory: Effects of task difficulty, type of processing, and practice. Cereb. Cortex 1997, 7, 374–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Gevins, A.; Smith, M.E.; Leong, H.; McEvoy, L.; Whitfield, S.; Du, R.; Rush, G. Monitoring working memory load during computer-based tasks with EEG pattern recognition methods. Hum. Factors 1998, 40, 79–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Yamamoto, K.; Takahashi, H.; Kato, T.; Oka, N.; Yoshino, K. Functional brain imaging using near-infrared spectroscopy during actual driving on an expressway. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 882. [Google Scholar]
  20. Tsunashima, H.; Yanagisawa, K. Measurement of Brain Function of Car Driver Using Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS). Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2009, 2009, 164958. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Kojima, T.; Tsunashima, H.; Shiozawa, T.Y. Measurement of train driver’s brain activity by functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). Opt. Quantum Electron. 2005, 37, 13–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Causse, M.; Chua, Z.; Peysakhovich, V.; Del Campo, N.; Matton, N. Mental workload and neural efficiency quantified in the prefrontal cortex using fNIRS. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Paczynski, M.; Formwalt, A.; Parasuraman, R.; Baccus, W.; Murtza, R.; Ayaz, H.; McKendrick, R. Into the Wild: Neuroergonomic Differentiation of Hand-Held and Augmented Reality Wearable Displays during Outdoor Navigation with Functional Near Infrared Spectroscopy. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 216. [Google Scholar]
  24. Pinti, P.; Aichelburg, C.; Lind, F.; Power, S.; Swingler, E.; Merla, A.; Hamilton, A.; Gilbert, S.; Burgess, P. Ilias Tachtsidis1Using Fiberless, Wearable fNIRS to Monitor Brain Activity in Real-world Cognitive Tasks. J. Vis. Exp. 2015, 53336. [Google Scholar]
  25. Atsumori, H. Noninvasive imaging of prefrontal activation during attention-demanding tasks performed while walking using a wearable optical topography system. J. Biomed. Opt. 2010, 15, 046002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Siddiquee, M.R.; Hasan, S.M.S.; Marquez, J.S.; Ramon, R.N.; Bai, O. Accurate Vigilance Detection During Gait by Using Movement Artifact Removal. IEEE Access 2020, 8, 51179–51188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Baddeley, A. Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2003, 4, 829–839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Owen, A.M.; McMillan, K.M.; Laird, A.R.; Bullmore, E. N-back working memory paradigm: A meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2005, 25, 46–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Sato, H.; Yahata, N.; Funane, T.; Takizawa, R.; Katura, T.; Atsumori, H.; Nishimura, Y.; Kinoshita, A.; Kiguchi, M.; Koizumi, H.; et al. A NIRS–fMRI investigation of prefrontal cortex activity during a working memory task. Neuroimage 2013, 83, 158–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Mandrick, K.; Derosiere, G.; Dray, G.; Coulon, D.; Micallef, J.-P.; Perrey, S. Prefrontal cortex activity during motor tasks with additional mental load requiring attentional demand: A near-infrared spectroscopy study. Neurosci. Res. 2013, 76, 156–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Ayaz, H.; Shewokis, P.A.; Bunce, S.; Izzetoglu, K.; Willems, B.; Onaral, B. Optical brain monitoring for operator training and mental workload assessment. Neuroimage 2012, 59, 36–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. De Joux, N.R.; Wilson, K.M.; Russell, P.N.; Finkbeiner, K.M.; Helton, W.S. A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study of the effects of configural properties on sustained attention. Neuropsychologia 2017, 94, 106–117. Available online: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0028393216304390 (accessed on 15 July 2020). [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Kirchner, W.K. Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. J. Exp. Psychol. 1958, 55, 352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Curtin, A.; Ayaz, H. Neural efficiency metrics in neuroergonomics: Theory and applications. In Neuroergonomics; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2019; pp. 133–140. [Google Scholar]
  35. Hoskinson, P.; Toomim, J. Brain Workshop—A Dual N-Back Game. 2009. Available online: http://brainworkshop.sourceforge.net/ (accessed on 15 July 2020).
  36. Wang, Y.; Zheng, Y.; Bai, O.; Wang, Q.; Liu, D.; Liu, X.; Sun, J. A multifunctional wireless body area sensors network with real time embedded data analysis. In Proceedings of the 2016 IEEE Biomedical Circuits and Systems Conference (BioCAS), Shanghai, China, 17–19 October 2016; pp. 508–511. [Google Scholar]
  37. Siddiquee, M.R.; Marquez, J.S.; Atri, R.; Ramon, R.; Mayrand, R.P.; Bai, O. Movement artefact removal from NIRS signal using multi-channel IMU data. Biomed. Eng. Online 2018, 17, 120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Siddiquee, M.R.; Xue, T.; Marquez, J.S.; Atri, R.; Ramon, R.; Mayrand, R.; PLeung, C.; Bai, O. Sensor Fusion in Human Cyber Sensor System for Motion Artifact Removal from NIRS Signal. In Proceedings of the 2019 12th International Conference on Human System Interaction (HSI), Richmond, VA, USA, 25–27 June 2019; pp. 192–196. [Google Scholar]
  39. Scholkmann, F.; Wolf, M. General equation for the differential pathlength factor of the frontal human head depending on wavelength and age. J. Biomed. Opt. 2013, 18, 105004. Available online: www.zora.uzh.ch (accessed on 15 July 2020). [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  40. Aasted, C.M.; Yücel, M.A.; Cooper, R.J.; Dubb, J.; Tsuzuki, D.; Becerra, L.; Petkov, M.P.; Borsook, D.; Dan, I.; Boas, D.A. Anatomical guidance for functional near-infrared spectroscopy: AtlasViewer tutorial. Neurophotonics 2015, 2, 020801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Huppert, T.J.; Diamond, S.G. HomER: A review of time-series analysis methods for near-infrared spectroscopy of the brain. Appl. Opt. 2009, 48, 280–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Tomita, Y.; Vialatte, F.B.; Dreyfus, G.; Mitsukura, Y.; Bakardjian, H.; Cichocki, A. Bimodal BCI using simultaneously NIRS and EEG. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 2014, 61, 1274–1284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Elwell, C.E.; Springett, R.; Hillman, E.; Delpy, D.T. Oscillations in cerebral haemodynamics. In Oxygen Transport to Tissue XXI; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1999; pp. 57–65. [Google Scholar]
  44. Rahman, M.A.; Rashid, M.A.; Ahmad, M. Selecting the optimal conditions of Savitzky—Golay filter for fNIRS signal. Biocybern. Biomed. Eng. 2019, 39, 624–637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Cope, M. The Development of a Near Infrared Spectroscopy System and Its Application for Non Invasive Monitory of Cerebral Blood and Tissue Oxygenation in the Newborn Infants. Ph.D. Thesis, University of London, London, UK, 1991. Available online: http://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/1317956/ (accessed on 15 July 2020).
  46. Power, S.D.; Kushki, A.; Chau, T. Intersession Consistency of Single-Trial Classification of the Prefrontal Response to Mental Arithmetic and the No-Control State by NIRS. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e37791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Faress, A.; Chau, T. Towards a multimodal brain-computer interface: Combining fNIRS and fTCD measurements to enable higher classification accuracy. Neuroimage 2013, 77, 186–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Moghimi, S.; Kushki, A.; Power, S.; Guerguerian, A.M.; Chau, T. Automatic detection of a prefrontal cortical response to emotionally rated music using multi-channel near-infrared spectroscopy. J. Neural Eng. 2012, 9, 026022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Holper, L.; Wolf, M. Single-trial classification of motor imagery differing in task complexity: A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 2011, 8, 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Khan, M.J.; Hong, K.-S. Passive BCI based on drowsiness detection: An fNIRS study. Biomed. Opt. Express 2015, 6, 4063–4078. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Hong, K.S.; Naseer, N.; Kim, Y.H. Classification of prefrontal and motor cortex signals for three-class fNIRS-BCI. Neurosci. Lett. 2015, 587, 87–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Cui, X.; Bray, S.; Reiss, A.L. Speeded Near Infrared Spectroscopy (NIRS) Response Detection. PLoS ONE 2010, 5, e15474. Available online: www.plosone.org (accessed on 15 July 2020). [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Newaz, A.I.; Sikder, A.K.; Rahman, M.A.; Uluagac, A.S. HealthGuard: A Machine Learning-Based Security Framework for Smart Healthcare Systems. In Proceedings of the 2019 6th International Conference on Social Networks Analysis, Management and Security, SNAMS 2019, Granada, Spain, 22–25 October 2019; pp. 389–396. [Google Scholar]
  54. Sitaram, R.; Zhang, H.; Guan, C.; Thulasidas, M.; Hoshi, Y.; Ishikawa, A.; Shimizu, K.; Birbaumer, N. Temporal classification of multichannel near-infrared spectroscopy signals of motor imagery for developing a brain-computer interface. NeuroImage 2007, 34, 1416–1427. Available online: http://www.nmr.mgh (accessed on 15 July 2020). [CrossRef]
  55. Nguyen, H.T.; Ngo, C.Q.; Truong, D.K.Q.; Vo, T.V. Temporal hemodynamic classification of two hands tapping using functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 1–12. [Google Scholar]
  56. Abibullaev, B.; An, J.; Moon, J.-I. Neural Network Classification of Brain Hemodynamic Responses from Four Mental Tasks. Int. J. Optomechatron. 2011, 5, 340–359. Available online: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/15599612.2011.633209 (accessed on 15 July 2020). [CrossRef]
  57. Naseer, N.; Qureshi, N.K.; Noori, F.M.; Hong, K.S. Analysis of Different Classification Techniques for Two-Class Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy-Based Brain-Computer Interface. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2016, 2016, 5480760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Kohavi, R.; John, G.H. Wrappers for feature subset selection. Artif. Intell. 1997, 97, 273–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Kira, K.; Rendell, L.A. A practical approach to feature selection. In Machine Learning Proceedings 1992; Morgan Kaufmann: San Francisco, CA, USA, 1992; pp. 249–256. [Google Scholar]
  60. Robnik-Šikonja, M.; Kononenko, I. Theoretical and Empirical Analysis of ReliefF and RReliefF. Mach. Learn. 2003, 53, 23–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Kohavi, R. A study of cross-validation and bootstrap for accuracy estimation and model selection. In Proceedings of the International Joint Conference on Articial Intelligence, Montreal, QC, Canada, 20–25 August 1995; Volume 14, pp. 1137–1145. [Google Scholar]
  62. Buckner, R.L.; Andrews-Hanna, J.R.; Schacter, D.L. The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2008, 1124, 1–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Gusnard, D.A.; Raichle, M.E. Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2001, 2, 685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Koechlin, E.; Hyafil, A. Anterior prefrontal function and the limits of human decision-making. Science 2007, 318, 594–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Velanova, K.; Jacoby, L.L.; Wheeler, M.E.; McAvoy, M.P.; Petersen, S.E.; Buckner, R.L. Functional-anatomic correlates of sustained and transient processing components engaged during controlled retrieval. J. Neurosci. 2003, 23, 8460–8470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Reynolds, J.R.; Mcdermott, K.B.; Braver, T.S. A Direct Comparison of Anterior Prefrontal Cortex Involvement in Episodic Retrieval and Integration. Cereb. Cortex 2006, 16, 519–528. Available online: https://academic.oup.com/cercor/article-abstract/16/4/519/382234 (accessed on 15 July 2020). [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Gilbert, S.J.; Spengler, S.; Simons, J.S.; Steele, J.D.; Lawrie, S.M.; Frith, C.D.; Burgess, P.W. Functional specialization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10): A meta-analysis. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2006, 18, 932–948. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Graphical abstract of the study.
Figure 1. Graphical abstract of the study.
Sensors 20 05082 g001
Figure 2. Positional 2-back test. Each event is 2 s long, and the task state lasts for 48 s. Afterward, a 25 s Rest state followed, when the subjects did not move and remained visually affixed to the blank computer screen.
Figure 2. Positional 2-back test. Each event is 2 s long, and the task state lasts for 48 s. Afterward, a 25 s Rest state followed, when the subjects did not move and remained visually affixed to the blank computer screen.
Sensors 20 05082 g002
Figure 3. (a) Photodetector placement and channel positions. All the distances between the detector and LED are the same, 3 cm. The distances between the adjacent detectors are 5.5 cm horizontally and 4.5 cm vertically. Similarly, the distance between adjacent LEDs is 5.5 cm. (b) Sensitivity map for the depicted channel arrangement.
Figure 3. (a) Photodetector placement and channel positions. All the distances between the detector and LED are the same, 3 cm. The distances between the adjacent detectors are 5.5 cm horizontally and 4.5 cm vertically. Similarly, the distance between adjacent LEDs is 5.5 cm. (b) Sensitivity map for the depicted channel arrangement.
Sensors 20 05082 g003
Figure 4. Mean accuracy of classifications across various location for different segmentation window lengths. The standard errors of the mean classification accuracies are presented by the error bars. The four classification accuracy means whose differences are statistically significant are highlighted for significance with stars.
Figure 4. Mean accuracy of classifications across various location for different segmentation window lengths. The standard errors of the mean classification accuracies are presented by the error bars. The four classification accuracy means whose differences are statistically significant are highlighted for significance with stars.
Sensors 20 05082 g004
Table 1. Classification accuracies along with means and standard deviations (SDs) across all subjects for each location on the forehead.
Table 1. Classification accuracies along with means and standard deviations (SDs) across all subjects for each location on the forehead.
Classification Accuracy (%)
LeftMidRightLeft-MidRight-MidWhole
5 s windowSub182.488.384.589.389.092.1
Sub287.295.580.098.397.698.6
Sub380.087.272.187.290.091.4
Sub484.176.985.283.885.289.3
Sub594.192.193.196.999.398.6
Sub691.497.987.696.296.998.3
Sub790.088.987.992.493.493.1
Sub878.384.177.290.788.390.3
Mean ± SD85.9 ± 5.788.9 ± 6.683.4 ± 6.791.9 ± 5.092.5 ± 5.194.0 ± 3.9
10 s windowSub182.992.185.091.489.391.4
Sub288.694.390.798.693.699.3
Sub379.370.772.186.489.381.4
Sub489.375.080.086.490.785.7
Sub594.391.496.497.199.397.9
Sub691.497.985.097.197.998.6
Sub790.792.282.293.694.492.7
Sub884.387.180.787.990.091.4
Mean ± SD87.6 ± 5.087.6 ± 9.684.0 ± 7.392.3 ± 5.093.0 ± 3.992.3 ± 6.3
20 s windowSub186.795.083.388.390.093.3
Sub293.3100.093.398.395.095.0
Sub383.381.770.086.785.085.0
Sub493.370.086.791.793.390.0
Sub598.398.393.398.3100.0100.0
Sub696.7100.090.096.796.793.3
Sub786.694.785.295.294.795.2
Sub888.393.390.093.388.393.3
Mean ± SD90.8 ± 5.491.6 ± 10.586.49 ± 7.693.6 ± 4.492.9 ± 4.893.2 ± 4.3
25 s windowSub1100.093.383.396.7100.0100.0
Sub296.7100.096.7100.0100.0100.0
Sub390.083.376.783.380.083.3
Sub493.386.790.093.393.390.0
Sub5100.0100.0100.0100.0100.096.7
Sub686.7100.093.3100.0100.0100.0
Sub796.796.796.797.596.793.3
Sub893.390.090.096.793.390.0
Mean ± SD94.6 ± 4.793.8 ± 6.590.8 ± 7.795.9 ± 5.695.4 ± 6.994.2 ± 6.1
48 s windowSub1100.0100.090.0100.0100.0100.0
Sub295.0100.095.0100.0100.0100.0
Sub385.090.090.095.090.090.0
Sub4100.090.095.0100.095.0100.0
Sub5100.0100.0100.0100.0100.0100.0
Sub685.0100.095.0100.0100.0100.0
Sub795.090.0100.096.795.095.0
Sub8100.095.095.095.0100.0100.0
Mean ± SD95 ± 6.595.6 ± 5.095 ± 3.898.3 ± 2.497.5 ± 3.898.1 ± 3.7

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Siddiquee, M.R.; Atri, R.; Marquez, J.S.; Hasan, S.M.S.; Ramon, R.; Bai, O. Sensor Location Optimization of Wireless Wearable fNIRS System for Cognitive Workload Monitoring Using a Data-Driven Approach for Improved Wearability. Sensors 2020, 20, 5082. https://doi.org/10.3390/s20185082

AMA Style

Siddiquee MR, Atri R, Marquez JS, Hasan SMS, Ramon R, Bai O. Sensor Location Optimization of Wireless Wearable fNIRS System for Cognitive Workload Monitoring Using a Data-Driven Approach for Improved Wearability. Sensors. 2020; 20(18):5082. https://doi.org/10.3390/s20185082

Chicago/Turabian Style

Siddiquee, Masudur R., Roozbeh Atri, J. Sebastian Marquez, S. M. Shafiul Hasan, Rodrigo Ramon, and Ou Bai. 2020. "Sensor Location Optimization of Wireless Wearable fNIRS System for Cognitive Workload Monitoring Using a Data-Driven Approach for Improved Wearability" Sensors 20, no. 18: 5082. https://doi.org/10.3390/s20185082

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop