Next Article in Journal
Salt-Sensitive Ileal Microbiota Plays a Role in Atrial Natriuretic Peptide Deficiency-Induced Cardiac Injury
Previous Article in Journal
Associations between the Nutrient Profiling System Underlying the Nutri-Score Nutrition Label and Biomarkers of Chronic Low-Grade Inflammation: A Cross-Sectional Analysis of a Middle- to Older-Aged Population
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Front-of-Package Labels on Unhealthy Packaged Foods in India: Evidence from a Randomized Field Experiment

1
Department of Survey Research and Data Analytics, International Institute for Population Sciences, Deemed University, Mumbai 400088, India
2
Department of Nutrition, Gillings School of Global Public Health, Carolina Population Center, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC 27516, USA
3
Vital Strategies, New York, NY 27599, USA
4
Carolina Population Center, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC 27516, USA
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Nutrients 2022, 14(15), 3128; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14153128
Submission received: 4 May 2022 / Revised: 9 June 2022 / Accepted: 11 June 2022 / Published: 29 July 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Nutrition and Public Health)

Abstract

:
Policies to require front-of-package labels (FOPLs) on packaged foods may help Indian consumers to better identify foods high in nutrients of concern, including sugar, saturated fat, and sodium, and discourage their consumption, which are outcomes that are critical for preventing rises in diet-related non-communicable disease. The objective was to test whether FOPLs helped Indian consumers identify “high-in” packaged foods and reduce intentions to purchase them. We conducted an in-person randomized experiment (n = 2869 adults between ages 18 and 60 years old) in six states of India in 2022. Participants were randomized to one of five FOPLs: a control label (barcode), warning label (octagon with “High in [nutrient]”), Health Star Rating (HSR), Guideline Daily Amount (GDA), or traffic light label. Participants then viewed a series of packaged foods high in sugar, saturated fat, or sodium with the assigned FOPL, and rated product perceptions and label reactions. Fewer than half of participants in the control group (39.1%) correctly identified all products high in nutrient(s) of concern. All FOPLs led to an increase in this outcome, with the biggest differences observed for the warning label (60.8%, p < 0.001), followed by the traffic light label (54.8%, p < 0.001), GDA (55.0%, p < 0.001), and HSR (45.0%, p < 0.01). While no FOPLs led to a reduction in intentions to purchase the packaged foods, the overall pattern of results suggested that warning labels are the most effective FOPL to help Indian consumers identify unhealthy foods.

1. Introduction

Over the past several decades, with the emergence of the epidemiological transition, India has experienced a growing problem of overweight and obesity and all the major nutrition-related noncommunicable diseases, especially diabetes and hypertension [1,2,3]. According to the latest National Family Health survey, nearly 1 in 4 adults and 1 in 20 children are classified as overweight or obese [4]. Rates are increasing faster in India than the world average, and obesity prevalence is expected to more than triple by the year 2040 without intervention [5,6,7]. At the same time, India faces a major double burden of malnutrition, as stunting and other forms of undernutrition remain high among the rural poor, in particular [4,8,9,10].
These changes have occurred at a time when a remarkable diet transformation is occurring in India, which affects rich and poor, young and old. In particular, the growth of ultra-processed food consumption in India is significant. As shown in neighboring Nepal, even preschoolers are increasingly being fed these foods, as one study found that 25% of preschoolers’ caloric intake came from ultra-processed food and this was linked with higher levels of stunting [11]. From 2006 to 2019, sales of ultra-processed snack food and sweetened beverages in India grew from 1 billion USD to 38 billion USD [12].
Many ready-to-eat or ready-to-heat foods and drinks are high in added sugars, sodium, saturated fats, and refined carbohydrates. Excessive consumption of these nutrients and ultra-processed foods increases the risk of obesity and related NCDs [13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25]. Indeed, a growing literature including both a large randomized controlled trial and over 45 longitudinal cohort studies linked ultra-processed food with increased risk of overweight/obesity, diet-related NCDs, and total and heart disease-linked mortality [16,26].
To reduce the consumption of packaged foods high in added sugar, sodium, saturated fat, and trans fats, front-of-package labels (FOPLs) have been recommended by the World Health Organization (WHO), the World Bank, and others [24,25,27,28,29,30,31,32,33]. The primary goals of front-of-package labels (FOPLs) are to inform consumers about the nutritional quality of food in a way that is quick and easy to understand and improve the nutritional quality of food purchases, with a secondary goal of stimulating reformulation in the food supply [34]. Interpretive FOPLs are particularly promising because they not only provide information about nutritional content but also help consumers to judge the healthfulness (or unhealthfulness) of products and provide guidance (encouragement or discouragement) regarding the decision to purchase. These FOPLs are important to reduce the intake of the major unhealthy processed foods consumed in India. By synthesizing complex nutrition facts into interpretable information, these labels may be especially valuable for populations with low literacy.
The evidence base on FOPLs is growing rapidly. Warning labels perhaps have the strongest evidence with regard to discouraging purchases of foods high in nutrients of concern, with recent systematic reviews of experimental and quasi-experimental data showing that warnings reduce the selection of unhealthy products by 26% to 36% [35,36]. Another recent review, which was focused on sugar, found that warnings were the most effective at increasing consumers’ understanding of the high nutrient content in foods [37]. Real-world evidence from Chile, the first country to implement mandatory front-of-pack warnings, found that warning labels were linked to a 24% decrease in purchases of unhealthy foods [38] and helped both parents and children identify unhealthy food and drinks and discourage their consumption [39].
In contrast, there is limited real-world data about the effectiveness of other common interpretive FOPLs, such as traffic light labels or the Health Star Rating (HSR) system. Real-world data on traffic light labeling systems has been mixed: one UK-based study found a sizeable reduction in calories purchased linked to the traffic light policy [40], while another study found no association with purchases [41]. Data from Ecuador, which implemented a mandatory traffic light labeling system in 2014, found low self-reported use of the labeling system [42] and no evidence that traffic light labels influenced purchasing behaviors [43,44]. Real-world evidence of the effectiveness of the HSR system has also been scarce. Data from Australia, which implemented voluntary HSR schemes in 2014, show low uptake of the HSR, with implementation skewed toward products considered to be healthier (i.e., with higher ratings) [45]. To our knowledge, there is no evidence from experimental studies or real-world evaluation studies that HSR leads to healthier food purchases [46,47,48]. Evidence in favor of the industry-promoted Guideline Daily Amount (GDA) system is the weakest of all, with an array of both experimental and real-world evaluation studies from across the globe finding that, relative to almost all other FOPL types, the GDAs are poorly understood, take the most time to evaluate, and are the least effective at influencing purchases [37,47,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58].
However, at the time this study was planned, there was virtually no evidence about what FOPL system will work best to inform Indian consumers about packaged foods that are excessive in nutrients of concern and discourage purchases of these products. A literature review of recent studies found narrative reviews [59] and commentaries [60] supporting the introduction of FOPLs as a policy to curb ultra-processed food intake in India. The only empirical study was an online report that found that, in a randomized experiment of 20,564 Indian consumers, warning labels appeared to perform best at reducing purchasing intentions toward unhealthy packaged foods [61]. The study found mixed results on an array of secondary outcomes, but the pattern of results suggested that warning labels and the HSR label performed better than other FOPL types (traffic lights, GDA) with regard to participants’ ranking of labels on various dimensions, such as ease of identification and ease of understanding. However, the lack of transparency about the methodology used and selection of outcomes warrants additional empirical research to understand which FOPLs work best in the Indian population.
In this context, the objective of this study was to experimentally evaluate the impact of FOPLs on consumers’ ability to correctly identify packaged products as containing excess levels of nutrients of concern and intentions to purchase them relative to a control label in a sample of Indian adults across six states. Secondary outcomes included consumers’ reactions to the FOPLs and perceptions of unhealthy packaged products.
Given the diversity of the Indian population in terms of language, culture, dietary intake, and educational attainment, it is also essential to ensure that any FOPL regulation works well across the entire population, as well as for different food categories. To address this, we explored whether the impact of FOPLs varied by product type, educational level, and state.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Institutional Review Board Approvals

This study was reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) at the International Institute for Population Sciences (IIPS) in Mumbai, India, and by the Biomedical Research Alliance of New York (BRANY), which is a national organization that provides IRB services.
This study was pre-registered at the Open Science Framework on 23 December 2021: https://osf.io/8kx3e. De-identified data is available here: https://osf.io/r3h9w/. Participants provided written consent or, for those who could not provide a signature, verbal consent.

2.2. Setting

We carried out an in-person field experiment in rural and urban areas of 6 states (Assam, Delhi, Gujarat, Odisha, Karnataka, Uttar Pradesh) from January to March of 2022. These states were chosen purposively as sentinel sites to represent the geographic areas of India, as well as key associated sociodemographic variations. From each of these states, one district was selected (Delhi, Mysuru, Bhubaneshwar, Lucknow, Ahmedabad, Guwahati).
First, four wards (two urban, one semi-urban, one peri-urban) were randomly chosen from each district. Next, a listing of potential survey locations was undertaken in each of these selected wards. Each location was classified into four clusters, namely, peak-day peak-time, peak-day lean-time, lean-day lean-time, and lean-day peak-time. This generated a sampling frame of time-location clusters (TLCs). Four TLCs per ward were randomly selected from the list for the survey. The locations were the places that sold packaged food items. These could either be a shop/retail outlet (called Kirana shops locally), large grocery store (in a shopping mall or on a high street), group of small shops, or smaller petty shops (tea stalls, shops selling paan—betel leaves wrapped around tobacco, fruits, etc.).

2.3. Participants

The participants were 2869 adults between the ages of 18 and 60 years. Recruitment of participants was done via customer interception. Once the nth person was intercepted, they were checked for eligibility and a request was made for an interview. Interviews were conducted with those who consented. After the interview, the next nth person was intercepted, whereas, after a refusal, the immediate next person was intercepted. Each person who agreed to the interview was taken to a close place for the interview where disturbances from the street were minimal.
Within each state, quota were used to obtain approximately 50% of participants who were women and with an educational level of 12 years or less. Eligibility criteria included being between the ages of 18 and 60 years and being involved in decision making related to grocery purchases for their household at least half the time.

2.4. Stimuli

Four FOPLs were selected for testing based on conversations with Indian health advocacy organizations and governmental organizations indicating that these labels were of interest for informing an impending FOPL regulation. In addition, the GDA was selected because it has already been voluntarily implemented on some products in India. Images of the FOPLs (as mocked up on sweet biscuits) are depicted in Figure 1.
Warning label: The main design was modeled on the proposed warning label used in South Africa [62]. A design agency adapted the warning for India through design testing with 15 adults in five cities of India to ensure that the label was noticeable and understandable in a socioeconomically diverse population. The warning label was comprised of a white holding strap with the marker word ALERT! and at least one triangle-shaped warning and up to three warnings, depending on the nutrient content of the product (with text: HIGH IN SUGAR, HIGH IN SODIUM, or HIGH IN SATURATED FAT). Based on prior evidence that icons increase perceived effectiveness and comprehension of the label across populations speaking different languages [63] and literacy levels [62], icons depicting sugar, salt, and saturated fat were also used.
HSR: The HSR was modeled after the existing HSR system used in Australia and New Zealand. The circular label stated HEALTH STAR RATING and depicted a number of stars from 0.5 to 5 shaded in black to indicate the healthfulness of the product, with fewer stars indicating less healthy and more stars indicating healthier.
Traffic light labels: The traffic light label was based on a simplified version of the system used in the UK and Ecuador. The label presented color-coded information on sugar, sodium, and saturated fat for each product, with red signaling high, amber signaling medium, and green signaling low content of that nutrient.
GDA: The GDA was based on the existing GDA used voluntarily by the food industry in many countries. The GDA contained four blue shaded shapes containing nutritional information on calories, saturated fat, sugar, and salt (both the absolute content in calories or grams, as well as the percent of an adult’s guideline daily amount).
Control label: Similar to previous FOPL experimental studies [64,65], a barcode label was used as a control label because it serves as a piece of visual information on the front of the food package while conveying neutral information about the product’s nutritional content.
The labels were displayed on a series of products, including a savory biscuit, a loaf of bread, a fruit drink, a sweet biscuit, and a package of instant noodles (Appendix A Figure A1). These product categories were chosen because they are commonly consumed in India, are often high in nutrients of concern, and represent categories where there may be high levels of consumer confusion about the nutritional content of the products. A professional designer designed mock products to avoid the influence of brand preferences, though to increase realism, the mock products and their nutritional information were based on popular Indian brands.
For each product, one commercial brand within each food category was selected. A mock nutrient profile was created based on this brand (±2% of the original nutrient profile model). Each label was then based on the relevant nutrient profile model: for HSR, Australia’s HSR calculator was used [66]; for warning labels and traffic light labels, the thresholds specified in the 2019 draft regulation for Food Safety Standards and Authority (FSSAI) were used; and for the GDA, national dietary guidelines were used [67].

2.5. Cognitive Testing and Protocol Development

The study protocol and measures used were developed and refined through an iterative process to ensure acceptability among diverse participants. First, the study items were translated from English into five languages (Assamese, Gujarati, Hindi, Kannada, and Odia). Two rounds of cognitive interviews were completed to make sure the measures were properly adapted to the Indian context and well-understood in each language while maintaining consistency with the underlying construct [68]. The interviews were completed in two phases, with each phase including four participants in each language (40 interviews total), with a refinement of the study measures occurring between phases one and two. After the cognitive interviews were complete, the items were refined and new additions were translated and back-translated to English before being reviewed by study co-authors. The field methodology (including recruitment and study implementation) was then pilot tested in a sample of 20 adults in an urban area of Delhi State in December of 2021 before further finalization of the study protocol.

2.6. Procedure

Participants were randomized to one of 5 arms: control label, HSR, warning, GDA, or traffic light label using an allocation ratio of 1:1:1:1:1. Participants then viewed a series of images of products in random order with a FOPL on the product according to the assigned arm. In the control condition, all products had the barcode label. In the HSR condition, all products displayed stars. In the warning label condition, products displayed the relevant warning(s) for sugar, sodium, and/or saturated fat. In the GDA condition, all products had a GDA with the relevant nutritional information. In the traffic light condition, products displayed multiple traffic lights with the relevant color code (green, yellow, or red) for each nutrient.
Interviewers showed participants images of products using an A5 size booklet in random order and asked them to assess the product and their reactions to the label. The participant viewed images of all 5 FOPLs and answered questions about which label they preferred. All data were entered into a smartphone app during the interview.
At the end of the study, participants provided demographic information.

2.7. Measures

Socio-demographic and behavioral covariates were specified as follows: gender (man/woman), age (18–30 years, 31–40 years, 41 years and older), education (≤12 years of education, >12 years), urbanicity (defined as peri-rural, semi-urban, and urban), and state (Assam, Delhi, Gujarat, Odisha, Karnataka, Uttar Pradesh). Languages included Assamese (Assam), Gujarati (Gujarat), Hindi (Delhi, Uttar Pradesh), Kannada (Karnataka), and Odia (Odisha). Participants were also able to conduct the survey bilingually (in the language of the state and English) if they preferred. The participant’s financial situation was defined as a four-level variable: (1) can pay the bills and buy necessary and additional things, (2) can pay the bills and buy necessary things only, (3) can pay the bills but not buy necessary things, and (4) cannot pay bills. Household income was categorized as <INR 100,000; INR 10,001–25,000; INR 250,001–50,000, and over INR 50,000. The consumption of “high-in” food (sweet biscuits, salty biscuits, bread, and instant noodles) was categorized as never or less than one time per week, 1 time per week, or more than 2 times per week.
The codebook including product assessment items and label assessment items is available in Appendix A Table A1).
For all five products, participants rated their perceptions of the product. First, to assess their ability to correctly identify that the product had high contents of nutrients of concern, they answered the question, “Do you think this product has high [nutrient of concern]?” (yes/no). For two products, the sweet biscuit and the instant noodles, the respondents were asked this question twice, one for each nutrient of concern.
Next, they were asked, “Is this product unhealthy?” (yes/no). If they answered yes, they were asked, “how unhealthy is it?”, with response options ranging from 1 to 3 (very much, somewhat, very little). They were then asked about visual attractiveness (“Do you think this product is visually attractive?”) and intentions to purchase (“Will you purchase this product next week, if it were available?”) with a yes/no response. Those who answered “yes” were asked the follow-up question (“How visually attractive,” or “How likely,” respectively), with two options again ranging from (1) very much to (3) very little.
Participants also completed a label assessment for three of five products (randomly selected). Participants answered whether the label grabbed their attention, made them feel concerned about the health problems of consuming the product, was understandable, taught them anything, was truthful, and was likable. For perceived message effectiveness, participants were asked whether the label made them concerned about the health consequences of consuming the product, made the product seem unpleasant, and made them feel discouraged from wanting to consume the product. For all items, response options were yes/no. If the respondent answered yes, they were then asked “how much…”, with responses ranging from (1) very much to (3) very little.
Finally, participants were asked to compare their labels and select which label (a) most discouraged them from consuming the product, (b) most discouraged them from feeding the product to a child aged 1–12 years, (c) best informed them that the product has high [nutrient], and (d) was the easiest to understand.

2.8. Statistical Analysis

First, we recombined some measures. For all items measured on the Likert scale, we combined the agreement item (yes/no) with the strength of agreement item for each person to create a 4-point Likert scale, subsequently recoded from 1 (not at all) to 4 (very much) for a more intuitive interpretation. For perceived message effectiveness, Cronbach’s alpha for the three items was >0.7; therefore, we created a scale that was the average of the three items for each product type. For the primary outcome, the ability to correctly identify products high in nutrients of concern, we specified this a priori as correctly identifying all nutrients since two products had multiple nutrients.
We descriptively reported sociodemographic characteristics and examined whether participant demographics differed by study arm using the chi-square test. Then, we descriptively reported unadjusted percentages and means (and standard deviations) for the two primary outcomes by product type.
For all main analyses, we examined differences between the control label and each of the other FOPL conditions. For all outcomes with multiple measurements for each person, we used mixed-effects logistic regression for the correct identification of all “high-in” nutrients and mixed-effects linear regression for all other outcomes, with respondent-level random intercepts to account for repeated measures. Standard errors were clustered by interviewer. While this is not standard procedure (and was not pre-registered as part of our original study plan), this was done as a conservative measure against any violation of the assumption of independence within interviewer, as some interviewers had very high or very low outcome means in their groups of participants for certain outcomes. We included indicator variables for label type (between-subjects) and product type (within-subjects), as well as an interaction of the label type and product type, if significant at the 5% level (as stated in the pre-registered analysis plan). The Holm procedure was used to adjust the p-values for multiple comparisons within each outcome (four tests for the four label types compared to the neutral label). This was done so the familywise error rate across the four tests within each outcome would not exceed 0.05, which was the nominal significance level.
To evaluate the most discouraging label, the most informative label, and the label that was easiest to understand, we descriptively reported the percentage of participants that selected each label type as the most discouraging from consuming the product, most discouraging from feeding the product to a child, most informative, and the easiest to understand.
To assess whether the effect of FOPLs on the primary outcomes differed by socio-demographic and behavioral covariates, we conducted exploratory moderation analyses by adding, in turn, each moderator of interest and its interaction with label type to the main model. For potential moderators, we included education and state. We explored moderation by state instead of language (as stated in the pre-registration) since most states used their own language and state-level differences were of interest conceptually due to regional variation in the food supply and dietary behaviors. We also included urbanicity, gender, whether the survey was conducted bilingually, and weekly consumption of the five product types presented to the participants as additional exploratory moderators. As Cronbach’s alpha was < 0.7 for the five consumption measures, separate models were estimated for each product using the corresponding consumption measure as the exploratory moderator. Since separate models were fit for each product, there were no repeated measures and linear and logistic regressions were used. We tested for overall differences in the effect of each label (relative to the control, i.e., the difference in means between a given FOPL group and the control arm) across the levels of each moderator. In line with the main analyses, the standard errors were clustered by interviewer. Unadjusted p-Values were used for these exploratory analyses.
Finally, we conducted two sets of sensitivity analyses on the primary outcomes. Because clustering the standard errors by interviewer is not a standard procedure, we present results where we did not cluster the standard errors. Secondly, as an alternative to clustering the standard errors by interviewer, for each primary outcome, we excluded participants who had been interviewed by one of the six interviewers with the highest or lowest three outcome means in their groups of participants.
We used a two-sided significance level of 0.05 to conduct all statistical tests using Stata version 16.1.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Results

The socio-demographic characteristics of the sample are reported in Table 1. No covariates were found to be unbalanced between the study arms. The sample was roughly distributed proportionately across all six states, with about half of the sample in urban areas and a quarter each in semi-urban and peri-rural areas. The sample was comprised of approximately half women and half with an education <12 years, and the majority of the sample were able to pay the bills and buy what they need. Approximately 40% completed the interview in a mixed language.
Overall descriptive results for all label and product assessment outcomes can be found in Appendix A Table A2. Descriptive results for label choice can be found in Appendix A Table A3.
Descriptive results for the primary outcomes by product type can be found in Appendix A Table A4. For the percent of participants who correctly identified all products high in nutrient(s) of concern, there were some observable differences by product type, with the highest percentage of participants in the control group correctly identifying the fruit drink (60.5%) and the lowest percentage identifying the sweet biscuit (24.7%). Thus, the difference between each FOPL and the control arm was smaller for all FOPL types for fruit drinks relative to most other product types. In contrast, the difference between each FOPL and the control arm tended to be higher for noodles and savory biscuits relative to the other products, though this varied somewhat by FOPL type. There were minimal differences by product type regarding intentions to purchase.
Descriptive results for the primary outcomes by state can be found in Appendix A Table A5. In the control group, Assam and Delhi were the states that had the lowest percentage of participants that correctly identified products as containing high levels of nutrient(s) of concern (25.9% and 24.4%, respectively), while Uttar Pradesh and Karnataka were the highest (54.0% and 56.7%, respectively). For all FOPL types, the difference between each FOPL and the control arm was the smallest for the state of Odisha. For most FOPLs, the difference between each FOPL and the control arm was the greatest for the state of Uttar Pradesh, followed by Gujarat.
There were also observable differences in intentions to purchase products high in nutrients of concern by state. In the control group, intentions to purchase were highest in Assam (3.1, SD 0.8) and lowest in Delhi (2.3, SD 1.1), Gujarat (2.4, SD 1.0), and Odisha (2.4, SD 1.1).

3.2. Main Results

Results of the primary outcomes can be found in Figure 2 and Figure 3. Relative to the control group (39.1% of participants; 95% CI 32.0, 46.2), each FOPL led to an increase in the percentage of participants who correctly identified all products with high levels of nutrient(s) of concern, with the biggest differences observed for the warning label (60.8%, 95% CI 53.5, 68.0; p < 0.001), followed by the traffic light label (54.8%, 95% CI 47.9, 61.8; p < 0.001), GDA label (55.0%, 95% CI 47.1, 62.9; p < 0.001), and HSR label (45.0%, 95% CI 37.2, 52.8; p < 0.01). Relative to the control label (2.6, 95% CI 2.5, 2.8), no FOPL led to a statistically significant difference in participants’ intentions to purchase. Full numerical results for both outcomes are shown in Appendix A Table A6.
Results on other secondary outcomes can be found in Table 2. Relative to the control group (1.7, 95% CI 1.5, 1.8), each FOPL led to an increase in perceived message effectiveness, with the biggest difference observed for the warning label (2.1, 95% CI 1.9, 2.3; p < 0.001), followed by the traffic light label (2.0, 95% CI 1.8, 2.2; p < 0.001), GDA label (1.9, 95% CI 1.7, 2.0; p < 0.001), and HSR label (1.9, 95% CI 1.8, 2.1; p < 0.001).
Relative to the control group, all FOPL types led to increases in perceptions that products were unhealthy, while only the GDA label led to increases in perceptions that products were visually attractive (p < 0.05 for all comparisons). With regard to label perceptions, relative to the control group, all FOPLs were rated higher as being attention-grabbing, causing the labels to make participants concerned about the health consequences of consuming the product, be understandable, and teach them something new (p < 0.05 for all comparisons). Relative to the control label, all FOPLs except for the HSR were rated higher on being true, while only the GDA and traffic light label were rated higher on being likable.

3.3. Moderation by Sociodemographic and Behavioral Characteristics

Results on moderation of the main outcomes by FOPL type can be found in Table 3 and Table 4. For the ability to correctly identify all products high in nutrient(s) of concern, there was no moderation by any variable except for state (Table 3). For the impact of FOPLs on participants’ ability to correctly identify products high in nutrients of concern, the pattern of results suggested that the impact of FOPLs was greatest in Uttar Pradesh. This state had the biggest differences between the FOPL types (GDA, HSR, MTL) and the control (or second-biggest differences, for warning labels). In contrast, the impact of FOPLs was the smallest in Odisha, with FOPLs either leading to no statistical difference compared with the control (warning labels, GDA, MTL) or a negative difference (HSR). Despite these differences, there was some degree of consistency in the difference between FOPLs and the control across states, with the pattern of results generally showing the biggest differences for warnings, then for GDA or MTL, and relatively small differences for HSR.
For intentions to purchase, there was no moderation by most variables (Table 4). For the HSR label, there was moderation by urbanicity such that the effect of HSR was greater for semi-urban and peri-rural areas than for urban areas (p = 0.004).

3.4. Label Selection

The results for when participants were asked which label they preferred are shown in Figure 4. Warning labels were most often selected as most likely to discourage consumption of the high-in products by adults, and warnings, GDA, and HSR were most often selected as the most likely to discourage feeding the products to children. Participants selected MTL as the easiest to understand label and GDA as the most informative label.

3.5. Sensitivity Analyses

Results from the sensitivity analyses of main outcomes are found in Appendix A Table A7 and Table A8. In the analyses which removed clustering of standard errors by interviewer, the magnitude and direction of effects remained similar but the estimates became more precise. Relative to the control, warning labels led to a small but statistically significant reduction in participants’ intentions to purchase unhealthy products (p < 0.018). There were no differences in the pattern of results when interviewers with the highest or lowest values were excluded.

4. Discussion

This experimental in-person study of adults in six states of India found that, relative to a control label, FOPLs improved the participants’ ability to correctly identify packaged foods and drinks high in nutrients of concern, including sugar, saturated fat, and sodium. The warning label showed the biggest impact on this outcome. Warning labels also showed the biggest impact on several secondary outcomes, including perceived message effectiveness, an outcome that was shown to predict behavioral change [69]. In the main analyses, no FOPL statistically significantly affected intentions to purchase the packaged products, suggesting more research is needed to understand the potential behavior effects of FOPLs.
The finding that warning labels were the most effective FOPL on the pre-specified primary outcomes in terms of helping consumers identify products high in nutrients of concern was consistent with our prior conceptual framework. This framework for nutrient warnings posits that they are particularly well suited to reducing the consumption of “high-in” products because of their binary nature, which facilitates quick decisions, and their ability to signal a warning, which communicates action (to discourage consumption) [34,70].
However, in the main analyses, warning labels failed to statistically significantly reduce intentions to purchase unhealthy packaged products, a finding that was not in line with other empirical evidence in India and elsewhere. For example, a separate recent study conducted on FOPLs among Indian consumers found that, compared with GDA and HSR labels, the warnings led to the biggest impact on intentions to purchase unhealthy products [61]. In addition, a recently published systematic review of randomized controlled trials and quasi-experiments found that warning labels were more effective than color-coded labels (e.g., traffic lights) in discouraging unhealthy food purchasing behavior [35].
In contrast, in this study, we found a pattern of results that suggested warning labels had a small effect on reduced purchasing intentions, but these differences did not reach statistical significance after clustering standard errors at the interviewer level. However, these null results should be interpreted with caution given that participants were only exposed to the FOPLs one time in an artificial setting, whereas in a real-world setting, they would be exposed many times. The fact that warnings increased participants’ ability to identify ‘high-in’ products but was not large enough to change behavioral intentions suggests the need to reinforce any FOPL policy with a robust and focused communications campaign to increase consumer awareness and understanding. The lack of statistically significant results may also reflect a lack of precision in our statistical analysis, as warnings statistically significantly reduced intentions to purchase in our sensitivity analysis, when clustering of standard errors was not used (resulting in narrower confidence intervals). Finally, the lack of results could also be due to the use of mock products and lack of realism, which would have affected all arms and potentially blunted any effect of FOPLs on participants’ desire to buy the products. Future research on FOPLs and actual food purchases will be important for understanding their impact on consumer behavior in India.
It is worth noting that there is some controversy in the field about which public health goals should be prioritized when it comes to the desired outcome of an FOPL system. The current study was designed to test FOPLs’ impacts on antecedents to reducing purchases of foods and drinks high in nutrients of concern because global dietary recommendations consistently agree on the importance of preventing or reducing the consumption of excess amounts of sugar, sodium, saturated fat, and added sugar [71,72,73]. These results suggest that warning labels hold the most promise for helping Indian consumers identify “high-in” products, though these findings should be replicated in a behavioral trial in which actual purchases or intake behaviors are measured.
In contrast, this study found that the HSR system was the lowest-performing FOPL (relative to the control) with regard to helping consumers identify “high-in” products. These findings are consistent with a recent experiment among Colombian adults found that the HSR consistently performed worse than the warning label on multiple outcomes, including identifying “high in” products [74].
One likely reason for the HSR’s low performance is that it was designed with different public goals in mind. The HSR is designed to nudge consumers toward “healthier” purchases, not to identify unhealthy products. Previous studies showed that the HSR helps consumers to rank products based on healthfulness [75] and to help nudge them toward healthier choices [76]. Yet, other studies have shown that for unhealthy products (i.e., those receiving one star or a warning), HSR is less effective than warning labels [77].
In addition, it is unclear from a health perspective whether shifting consumers from a lower-scoring product to a higher-scoring product will result in meaningful gains for health. This is of particular concern because in a mandatory HSR system, ultra-processed products could be eligible to carry up to five stars, implying that these products are healthier and should be encouraged, despite a rapidly growing body of evidence from controlled feeding studies and from many prospective cohort studies showing that ultra-processed foods are linked to weight gain, overweight/obesity, and an array of adverse cardiometabolic effects [16,26]. In addition, others have criticized the HSR system for misrepresenting nutrition science [26], in part due to the use of an algorithm that does not reflect how human metabolism works (e.g., the presence of beneficial ingredients such as fiber or protein does not offset the harms of sugar, sodium, or saturated fat). To design an effective FOPL system for Indian consumers, policymakers should consider not only the design of the FOPL itself but also the underlying nutritional profile and health goal (e.g., reducing consumption of the most unhealthy items vs. encouraging healthier (yet likely to be ultra-processed) options).
With regards to secondary outcomes on product perceptions and label reactions, outcomes were mixed, though the pattern suggested a strong performance of warning labels and poor performance for HSR. Warning labels performed best on perceived message effectiveness, a scale that reflects both message perceptions (judgments about how well the message will lead to persuasion) and effects perceptions (how well the message will change behavioral antecedents or the actual behavior) [78,79]. Perceived message effectiveness was used in the development of many health messages across a range of products [80,81,82], and is predictive of behavioral change [69], offering further support for warnings as a strong FOPL to discourage the consumption of “high-in” products among Indian consumers. Warning labels also performed best on other outcomes (identifying products as unhealthy, making participants concerned about health consequences) and similarly to the GDA and/or the MTL on other outcomes (understandable, taught me something new, is true, liking), though the magnitude of difference between the FOPLs was small across all outcomes. The GDA and MTL performed best at grabbing attention. The HSR performed worse than all other FOPL types tested (except the control) on most secondary outcome.
When shown all the FOPLs and asked to select which one they most preferred on a range of outcomes, the results were mixed. Warning labels were most frequently chosen as the label that would most discourage consumption by adults or feeding the products to children. In contrast, the GDA label and MTL label were selected as the most informative and easiest to understand. However, there is no evidence that these preference measures are predictive of behavior change. Conceptually, preference for or “liking” the label may be inversely associated with the intended behavioral change (discouraging purchases), as they may attract consumers toward selecting the product.
With regard to effect modification by socio-demographic and dietary factors, our study found mixed results. Importantly, we found that the impact of FOPLs did not vary for high vs. low educated populations, which suggested that FOPLs hold promise as a population intervention across populations. On the other hand, this study did not assess literacy, and thus, we were not able to understand whether the FOPLs performed well among illiterate populations, which is especially important considering that approximately a quarter of the Indian population is illiterate (and this figure is higher amongst women and in rural areas) [83]. Few studies have looked at FOPLs among illiterate populations. However, principles of visual communication suggest and empirical data shows that imagery can better convey health risk information than can text or numerical information [63,84,85,86], particularly to low literacy groups. This suggests that the warning label (which, in this study, carried icons depicting sugar, salt, and saturated fat) and the HSR (which uses stars) would hold an advantage over labels such as the traffic light or GDA label. Limited empirical data also illustrates the promise of using icons with warnings; in particular, one focus group study in South Africa suggested that warnings with icons would work well among an illiterate population [62]. Other work from the United States found that warnings with icons were perceived as more effective among populations with lower English language literacy [63]. Future research in India should test the effectiveness of FOPLs in populations with low literacy.
We did observe some differences in the impact of FOPL by state. While the pattern of results consistently found that warning labels performed best, the impact of FOPLs was the smallest in the state of Odisha, where FOPLs either had no impact (warnings, GDA, or MTL) or a negative impact (HSR) relative to the control. An effective FOPL policy for India should consider using a state-based educational campaign to ensure that the FOPL is well-understood and used across different populations.
Limitations of this study included that it measured only participants’ self-reported perceptions and reactions. Future experimental trials with more realistic products in more realistic settings that more closely mirror real-world food environments will be necessary. An additional limitation is that we were unable to examine whether FOPLs’ impact varied for people with low incomes or low literacy. Further testing in populations with low incomes and low literacy will be important to ensure that an FOPL system works well for all Indian consumers. The strengths of this study included the large sample and the inclusion of six states (and five languages).

5. Conclusions

This randomized field experiment found that, relative to a control label, all FOPLs helped consumers to identify unhealthy packaged products high in sugar, sodium, and saturated fat. The pattern of results suggested that the warning label is the optimal FOPL to achieve the goal of informing consumers about packaged foods and drinks high in nutrients of concern. Replication of this study with behavioral outcomes would provide stronger evidence to support FOPL policies in the Indian population and understand whether such policies would impact consumers’ purchasing behaviors.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, L.S.T., N.M., S.K.S., and A.G.; methodology, L.S.T., N.M., S.K.S., A.G., and M.B.; investigation S.K.S., N.M., and A.G.; formal analysis, M.B., writing—original draft preparation L.S.T.; writing—review and editing, L.S.T., B.P., N.M., S.K.S., A.G., and M.B. All authors read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study received funding from Resolve to Save Lives and Bloomberg Philanthropies.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board at the International Institute for Population Sciences (IIPS) in Mumbai, India, and under applicable U.S. regulations about human-subjects research by a duly constituted Institutional Review Board.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

De-identified data can be found at Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Changescape, a survey research company, for the implementation of the fieldwork. Additionally, the authors thank Melissa Lam-McCarthy for her administrative assistance.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

Appendix A

Figure A1. Images of products used without FOPLs.
Figure A1. Images of products used without FOPLs.
Nutrients 14 03128 g0a1
Table A1. Codebook.
Table A1. Codebook.
Product Assessment
Excs_sfB10. Do you think this product has high [sugar/sodium/saturated fat]?1 = Yes
0 = No
Unhealthy_sfB11a. Is this product unhealthy?1 = Yes (Go to C11b)
0 = No (Go to C12a)
B11b. How unhealthy it is?1 = Very much
2 = Somewhat
3 = Very little
Ppa_sfB12a. Do you think this product is visually attractive?1 = Yes (Go to C12b)
0 = No (Go to C13a)
B12b. How visually attractive is this product?1 = Very much
2 = Somewhat
3 = Very little
Buy_lkly_sfB13a. Will you purchase this product next week, if it were available?1 = Yes (Go to C13b)
0 = No
B13b. How likely is it for you to want to purchase this product next week, if it were available?1 = Very much
2 = Somewhat
3 = Very little
Label Assessment
Attention_sfB1a. Does this label grab your attention?1 = Yes (Go to C1b)
0 = No (Go to C2a)
B1b. How much does this label grab your attention?1 = Very much
2 = Somewhat
3 = Very little
PME_conc_sfB2a. Does the label make you feel concerned about the health consequences of consuming this product?1 = Yes (Go to C2b)
0 = No (Go to C3a)
B2b. How concerned would you be about the health consequences of consuming this product?1 = Very much
2 = Somewhat
3 = Very little
PME_unpl_sfB3a. Does the label make this product seem unpleasant to you?1 = Yes (Go to C3b)
0 = No (Go to C4a)
B3b. How unpleasant does this product seem to you?1 = Very much
2 = Somewhat
3 = Very little
Pme_disc_sfB4a. Does the label make you feel discouraged from wanting to consume this product?1 = Yes (Got to C4b)
0 = No (Go to C5a)
B4b. How discouraged do you feel from wanting to consume this product?1 = Very much
2 = Somewhat
3 = Very little
Cog_elab_sfB5a. Does the label make you think about the health problems caused by consuming this product?1 = Yes (Go to C5b)
0 = No (Go to C6a)
B5b. How much does the label make you think about the health problems caused by consuming this product?1 = Very much
2 = Somewhat
3 = Very little
Understand_sfB6a. Do you understand what the label means?1 = Yes (Go to C6b)
0 = No (Go to C7a)
B6b. How much do you understand what the label means?1 = Very much
2 = Somewhat
3 = Very little
Learn_new_sfB7a. Has the label taught you anything?1 = Yes (Go to C7b)
0 = No (Go to C8a)
B7b. How much has the label taught you?1 = Very much
2 = Somewhat
3 = Very little
Trust_sfB8a. Do you think what label says is true?1 = Yes (Go to C8b)
0 = No (Go to C9a)
B8b. How much do you think what the label says is true?1 = Very much
2 = Somewhat
3 = Very little
Liking_sfB9a. Do you like to have this label on the products?1 = Yes (Go to C9b)
0 = No (Go to 10a)
B9b. How much would you like for products to have the label?1 = Very much
2 = Somewhat
3 = Very little
Table A2. Descriptive statistics for the product and label assessment outcomes by arm.
Table A2. Descriptive statistics for the product and label assessment outcomes by arm.
ControlWarningGDAHSRMTL
Product perceptions
Identified all “high-in” nutrients, n (%)1125 (39.2)1819 (60.8)1517 (54.8)1363 (45.4)1495 (55.2)
How likely would you be to buy this product next week?2.6 (1.1)2.5 (1.0)2.6 (1.0)2.6 (1.0)2.5 (1.1)
How unhealthy is this product?1.7 (1.0)2.1 (1.2)1.9 (1.2)1.8 (1.1)2.0 (1.2)
How visually attractive is this product?2.7 (1.1)2.8 (1.0)2.9 (1.0)2.8 (1.0)2.9 (1.0)
Label reactions
Does the label grab your attention?2.7 (1.0)2.9 (1.0)3.0 (1.0)2.8 (1.0)3.0 (1.0)
Perceived message effectiveness1.7 (0.8)2.1 (0.9)1.9 (0.8)1.9 (0.9)2.0 (0.9)
Does the label make you think about health problems?
caused by this product?
1.9 (1.1)2.4 (1.2)2.3 (1.2)2.2 (1.2)2.3 (1.2)
Do you understand the label?2.4 (1.1)2.8 (1.1)2.8 (1.1)2.7 (1.1)2.8 (1.1)
Does the label teach you anything?2.3 (1.2)2.8 (1.1)2.8 (1.1)2.7 (1.1)2.7 (1.2)
Do you think what the label says is true?2.6 (1.1)2.9 (1.1)2.9 (1.0)2.7 (1.1)2.8 (1.1)
Do you like to have the label on this product?2.7 (1.1)2.9 (1.0)2.9 (1.0)2.8 (1.0)2.9 (1.0)
Product perceptions were assessed on all 5 products (n = 14,345), label reactions were assessed on 3 products (n = 8607). Means and SDs unless otherwise stated.
Table A3. Descriptive statistics for the label choice outcomes by arm (no repeated measures), n (%) (n = 2869).
Table A3. Descriptive statistics for the label choice outcomes by arm (no repeated measures), n (%) (n = 2869).
ControlWarningGDAHSRMTL
Which label discourages you most from consuming this product?
Control66 (11.5)73 (12.2)61 (11.0)80 (13.3)65 (12.0)
Warning174 (30.3)171 (28.6)137 (24.7)161 (26.8)158 (29.2)
GDA113 (19.7)124 (20.7)125 (22.6)149 (24.8)117 (21.6)
HSR124 (21.6)131 (21.9)110 (19.9)126 (21.0)102 (18.8)
MTL97 (16.9)99 (16.6)121 (21.8)85 (14.1)100 (18.5)
Which label discourages you most from feeding this product to a child?
Control52 (9.1)54 (9.0)43 (7.8)41 (6.8)41 (7.6)
Warning140 (24.4)146 (24.4)138 (24.9)174 (29.0)140 (25.8)
GDA148 (25.8)164 (27.4)153 (27.6)139 (23.1)125 (23.1)
HSR136 (23.7)139 (23.2)126 (22.7)148 (24.6)126 (23.2)
MTL98 (17.1)95 (15.9)94 (17.0)99 (16.5)110 (20.3)
Which label best informs you that this product has high [nutrient]?
Control44 (7.7)37 (6.2)37 (6.7)43 (7.2)30 (5.5)
Warning70 (12.2)99 (16.6)91 (16.4)78 (13.0)78 (14.4)
GDA234 (40.8)237 (39.6)221 (39.9)234 (38.9)207 (38.2)
HSR133 (23.2)149 (24.9)125 (22.6)147 (24.5)135 (24.9)
MTL93 (16.2)76 (12.7)80 (14.4)99 (16.5)92 (17.0)
Which label is easiest to understand?
Control77 (13.4)45 (7.5)50 (9.0)56 (9.3)45 (8.3)
Warning66 (11.5)97 (16.2)75 (13.5)67 (11.1)74 (13.7)
GDA131 (22.8)152 (25.4)148 (26.7)134 (22.3)105 (19.4)
HSR120 (20.9)130 (21.7)122 (22.0)142 (23.6)112 (20.7)
MTL180 (31.4)174 (29.1)159 (28.7)202 (33.6)206 (38.0)
Table A4. Descriptive statistics on the primary outcomes by study arm and product type (n = 14,345).
Table A4. Descriptive statistics on the primary outcomes by study arm and product type (n = 14,345).
ControlWarningGDAHSRMTL
Correctly identified all high-in nutrients, n (%)
Sweet biscuits142 (24.7)282 (47.2)216 (39.0)176 (29.3)190 (35.1)
Bread262 (45.6)410 (68.6)314 (56.7)295 (49.1)335 (61.8)
Fruit drink347 (60.5)459 (76.8)408 (73.6)385 (64.1)399 (73.6)
Noodles168 (29.3)323 (54)262 (47.3)244 (40.6)276 (50.9)
Savory biscuits206 (35.9)345 (57.7)317 (57.2)263 (43.8)295 (54.4)
Purchase intentions, mean (SD)
Sweet biscuits2.6 (1.1)2.5 (1.0)2.7 (1.0)2.6 (1.0)2.6 (1.1)
Bread2.6 (1.1)2.5 (1.1)2.6 (1.0)2.7 (1.0)2.5 (1.1)
Fruit drink2.6 (1.1)2.4 (1.0)2.6 (1.0)2.6 (1.0)2.5 (1.1)
Noodles2.5 (1.1)2.4 (1.0)2.6 (1.0)2.5 (1.1)2.5 (1.0)
Savory biscuits2.6 (1.0)2.5 (1.0)2.6 (1.0)2.6 (1.0)2.6 (1.1)
Table A5. Descriptive statistics on the primary outcomes by study arm and state (n = 14,345).
Table A5. Descriptive statistics on the primary outcomes by study arm and state (n = 14,345).
ControlWarningGDAHSRMTL
Correctly identified all high-in nutrients, n (%)
Odisha189 (47.8)221 (47)233 (50.7)173 (39.3)180 (43.4)
Uttar Pradesh224 (54)382 (85.8)438 (85.9)305 (67.8)363 (79.8)
Assam128 (25.9)260 (47.3)154 (33.5)101 (22.4)172 (45.9)
Delhi134 (24.4)241 (58.8)201 (42.8)168 (31.1)208 (42.9)
Karnataka272 (56.7)429 (72.1)283 (60.2)380 (63.3)272 (64.8)
Gujarat178 (33.3)286 (55)208 (52)236 (45)300 (53.6)
Purchase intentions, mean (SD)
Odisha2.4 (1.1)2.2 (1.1)2.2 (1.1)2.2 (1.1)2.1 (1.1)
Uttar Pradesh2.7 (1.1)2.6 (1.0)2.8 (1.0)2.7 (1.0)2.8 (1.0)
Assam3.1 (0.8)2.8 (1.0)3.1 (0.8)3.1 (0.9)3 (0.9)
Delhi2.3 (1.1)2 (1.1)2.5 (1.1)2.4 (1.1)2.3 (1.1)
Karnataka2.7 (0.9)2.8 (0.9)2.8 (0.9)2.7 (0.9)2.7 (0.9)
Gujarat2.4 (1.0)2.2 (0.9)2.4 (0.9)2.5 (0.9)2.4 (1.0)
Table A6. Results for primary outcome by label arm (n = 14,345).
Table A6. Results for primary outcome by label arm (n = 14,345).
ControlWarningGDAHSRMTL
Mean
(95% CI)
Mean
(95% CI)
pMean
(95% CI)
pMean
(95% CI)
pMean
(95% CI)
p
Identified all “high-in” nutrients, %39.1
(32.0, 46.2)
60.8
(53.5, 68.0)
<0.001
55.0
(47.1, 62.9)
<0.001
45.0
(37.1, 52.8)
0.008
54.8
(47.9, 61.8)
<0.001
How likely would you be to buy this product next week?2.6
(2.5, 2.8)
2.5
(2.3, 2.6)
0.110
2.6
(2.5, 2.8)
1.000
2.6
(2.5, 2.7)
0.758
2.5
(2.4, 2.7)
0.664
Table A7. Sensitivity results for the primary outcomes using non-clustered standard errors (n = 14,345).
Table A7. Sensitivity results for the primary outcomes using non-clustered standard errors (n = 14,345).
ControlWarningGDAHSRMTL
Mean
(95% CI)
Mean
(95% CI)
pMean
(95% CI)
pMean
(95% CI)
pMean
(95% CI)
p
Identified all “high-in” nutrients, %39.1
(36.3, 41.9)
60.8
(58.0, 63.6)
<0.001
55.0
(52.0, 58.0)
<0.001
45.0
(42.2, 47.9)
0.004
54.8
(51.8, 57.8)
<0.001
How likely would you be to buy this product next week?2.6
(2.5, 2.7)
2.5
(2.4, 2.5)
0.018
2.6
(2.6, 2.7)
1.000
2.6
(2.5, 2.7)
0.763
2.5
(2.5, 2.6)
0.393
Table A8. Sensitivity results for the primary outcomes excluding participants who had been interviewed by one of the six interviewers with the highest or lowest three outcome means in their groups of participants (n = 354 and 303 excluded participants, or 1770 and 1515 observations, for identifying all excess nutrients and purchase intentions, respectively).
Table A8. Sensitivity results for the primary outcomes excluding participants who had been interviewed by one of the six interviewers with the highest or lowest three outcome means in their groups of participants (n = 354 and 303 excluded participants, or 1770 and 1515 observations, for identifying all excess nutrients and purchase intentions, respectively).
ControlWarningGDAHSRMTL
Mean
(95% CI)
Mean
(95% CI)
pMean
(95% CI)
pMean
(95% CI)
pMean
(95% CI)
p
Identified all “high-in” nutrients, %38.6
(35.8, 41.4)
62.7
(59.8, 65.5)
<0.001
54.8
(51.8, 57.8)
<0.001
43.9
(41.0, 46.8)
0.010
56.8
(53.8, 59.8)
<0.001
How likely would you be to buy this product next week?2.6
(2.5, 2.7)
2.5
(2.4, 2.6)
0.083
2.6
(2.5, 2.7)
0.886
2.6
(2.5, 2.6)
0.915
2.6
(2.5, 2.6)
0.967

References

  1. Shetty, P. Public health: India’s diabetes time bomb. Nature 2012, 485, S14–S16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Misra, A.; Khurana, L. Obesity-related non-communicable diseases: South Asians vs. White Caucasians. Int. J. Obes. 2011, 35, 167–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Misra, A.; Khurana, L. The metabolic syndrome in South Asians: Epidemiology, determinants, and prevention. Metab. Syndr. Relat. Disord. 2009, 7, 497–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Government of India, International Institute for Population Sciences (IIPS) and ICF. National Family Health Survey (NFHS-5), 2019–2021; IIPS: Mumbai, India, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  5. World Obesity Federation. Global Obesity Observatory: India. Available online: https://data.worldobesity.org/country/india-95/#data_prevalence (accessed on 6 March 2022).
  6. Luhar, S.; Timæus, I.M.; Jones, R.; Cunningham, S.; Patel, S.A.; Kinra, S.; Clarke, L.; Houben, R. Forecasting the prevalence of overweight and obesity in India to 2040. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0229438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Aiyar, A.; Rahman, A.; Pingali, P. India’s rural transformation and rising obesity burden. World Dev. 2021, 138, 105258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Popkin, B.M.; Corvalan, C.; Grummer-Strawn, L.M. Dynamics of the double burden of malnutrition and the changing nutrition reality. Lancet 2020, 395, 65–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Subramanian, S.V.; Kawachi, I.; Smith, G.D. Income inequality and the double burden of under- and overnutrition in India. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2007, 61, 802–809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Meenakshi, J. Trends and patterns in the triple burden of malnutrition in India. Agric. Econ. 2016, 47, 115–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Pries, A.M.; Rehman, A.M.; Filteau, S.; Sharma, N.; Upadhyay, A.; Ferguson, E.L. Unhealthy Snack Food and Beverage Consumption Is Associated with Lower Dietary Adequacy and Length-for-Age z-Scores among 12–23-Month-Olds in Kathmandu Valley, Nepal. J. Nutr. 2019, 149, 1843–1851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Euromonitor International. Passport Nutrition. Available online: http://www.euromonitor.com/. (accessed on 15 June 2022).
  13. Anand, S.S.; Hawkes, C.; de Souza, R.J.; Mente, A.; Dehghan, M.; Nugent, R.; Zulyniak, M.A.; Weis, T.; Bernstein, A.M.; Krauss, R.M.; et al. Food Consumption and its Impact on Cardiovascular Disease: Importance of Solutions Focused on the Globalized Food System: A Report From the Workshop Convened by the World Heart Federation. J. Am. Coll. Cardiol. 2015, 66, 1590–1614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. World Health Organization. Report of a WHO Forum and Technical Meeting. Reducing Salt Intake in Populations; WHO Press: Paris, France, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  15. World Health Organization; Food and Agricultural Organization. Diet, Nutrition and the Prevention of Chronic Diseases: Report of a Joint WHO/FAO Expert Consultation; Technical Report Series 916; WHO Press: Geneva, Switzerland, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  16. Pagliai, G.; Dinu, M.; Madarena, M.P.; Bonaccio, M.; Iacoviello, L.; Sofi, F. Consumption of ultra-processed foods and health status: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Br. J. Nutr. 2021, 125, 308–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Lane, M.M.; Davis, J.A.; Beattie, S.; Gómez-Donoso, C.; Loughman, A.; O’Neil, A.; Jacka, F.; Berk, M.; Page, R.; Marx, W. Ultraprocessed food and chronic noncommunicable diseases: A systematic review and meta-analysis of 43 observational studies. Obes. Rev. 2020, 22, e13146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Askari, M.; Heshmati, J.; Shahinfar, H.; Tripathi, N.; Daneshzad, E. Ultra-processed food and the risk of overweight and obesity: A systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. Int. J. Obes. 2020, 44, 2080–2091. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Chen, X.; Zhang, Z.; Yang, H.; Qiu, P.; Wang, H.; Wang, F.; Zhao, Q.; Fang, J.; Nie, J. Consumption of ultra-processed foods and health outcomes: A systematic review of epidemiological studies. Nutr. J. 2020, 19, 86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Meneguelli, T.S.; Hinkelmann, J.V.; Hermsdorff, H.H.M.; Zulet, M.Á.; Martínez, J.A.; Bressan, J. Food consumption by degree of processing and cardiometabolic risk: A systematic review. Int. J. Food Sci. Nutr. 2020, 71, 678–692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Elizabeth, L.; Machado, P.; Zinöcker, M.; Baker, P.; Lawrence, M. Ultra-Processed Foods and Health Outcomes: A Narrative Review. Nutrients 2020, 12, 1955. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Santos, F.S.D.; Dias, M.D.S.; Mintem, G.C.; Oliveira, I.O.D.; Gigante, D.P. Food processing and cardiometabolic risk factors: A systematic review. Rev. De Saúde Pública 2020, 54, 70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services; U.S. Department of Agriculture. Scientific Report of the 2015 Dietary Guidelines Advisory Committee; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services: Washington, DC, USA; U.S. Department of Agriculture: Washington, DC, USA, 2015.
  24. World Health Organization. Guideline: Sugars Intake for Adults and Children; WHO Press: Geneva, Switzerland, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  25. World Cancer Research Fund International. Curbing Global Sugar Consumption: Effective Food Policy Actions to Help Promote Healthy Diets and Tackle Obesity; World Cancer Research Fund International: London, UK, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  26. Hall, K.D.; Ayuketah, A.; Brychta, R.; Cai, H.; Cassimatis, T.; Chen, K.Y.; Chung, S.T.; Costa, E.; Courville, A.; Darcey, V.; et al. Ultra-processed diets cause excess calorie intake and weight gain: A one-month inpatient randomized controlled trial of ad libitum food intake. Cell Metab. 2019, 30, 67–77.e3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. World Health Organization. Global Strategy on Diet, Physical Activity and Health; WHO Press: Paris, France, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  28. World Health Organization. Guiding Principles and Framework Manual for Front-of-Pack Labelling for Promoting Healthy Diets. Available online: https://apps.who.int/nutrition/publications/policies/guidingprinciples-labelling-promoting-healthydiet/en/index.html (accessed on 29 September 2021).
  29. World Health Organization. Sodium Intake for Adults and Children: Guidance Summary. Available online: https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/9789240025097 (accessed on 29 October 2021).
  30. World Health Organization Western Pacific. Reducing Sodium in Food and Excessive Salt Intake. Available online: https://www.who.int/china/activities/reducing-sodium-in-food-and-excessive-salt-intake (accessed on 2 November 2021).
  31. World Health Organization. Draft Guidelines on Saturated Fatty Acid and Trans-Fatty Acid Intake for Adults and Children; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2018; p. 103. [Google Scholar]
  32. Shekar, M.; Popkin, B. (Eds.) Obesity: Health and Economic Consequences of an Impending Global Challenge; Human Development Perspectives Series; The World Bank: Washington, DC, USA, 2020; p. 204. [Google Scholar]
  33. World Cancer Research Fund/American Institute for Cancer Research. Diet, Nutrition, Physical Activity and Cancer: A Global Perspective; Continuous Update Project Expert Report; WCRF International: London, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  34. Roberto, C.A.; Ng, S.W.; Ganderats-Fuentes, M.; Hammond, D.; Barquera, S.; Jauregui, A.; Taillie, L.S. The Influence of Front-of-Package Nutrition Labeling on Consumer Behavior and Product Reformulation. Annu. Rev. Nutr. 2021, 41, 529–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Song, J.; Brown, M.K.; Tan, M.; MacGregor, G.A.; Webster, J.; Campbell, N.R.; Trieu, K.; Ni Mhurchu, C.; Cobb, L.K.; He, F.J. Impact of color-coded and warning nutrition labelling schemes: A systematic review and network meta-analysis. PLoS Med. 2021, 18, e1003765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Grummon, A.H.; Hall, M.G. Sugary drink warnings: A meta-analysis of experimental studies. PLoS Med. 2020, 17, e1003120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Scapin, T.; Fernandes, A.C.; Curioni, C.C.; Pettigrew, S.; Neal, B.; Coyle, D.H.; Rodrigues, V.M.; Bernardo, G.L.; Uggioni, P.L.; Proença, R.P. Influence of sugar label formats on consumer understanding and amount of sugar in food choices: A systematic review and meta-analyses. Nutr. Rev. 2021, 79, 788–801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Taillie, L.; Bercholz, M.; Popkin, B.; Reyes, M.; Colchero, A.; Corvalan, C. Changes in food purchases after Chile’s polices on food labeling, marketing, and sales in schools: A before and after study. Lancet Planet. Health 2021, in press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Correa, T.; Fierro, C.; Reyes, M.; Carpentier, F.R.D.; Taillie, L.S.; Corvalan, C. Responses to the Chilean law of food labeling and advertising: Exploring knowledge, perceptions and behaviors of mothers of young children. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2019, 16, 21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Fichera, E.; von Hinke, S. The response to nutritional labels: Evidence from a quasi-experiment. J. Health Econ. 2020, 72, 102326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Sacks, G.; Rayner, M.; Swinburn, B. Impact of front-of-pack ‘traffic-light’ nutrition labelling on consumer food purchases in the UK. Health Promot. Int. 2009, 24, 344–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Radosevich, A.; Mendes, F.D.C.; Villegas, R.; Mora-Garcia, G.; Garcia-Larsen, V. Awareness, Understanding and Use of the ‘Traffic Light’ Food Labelling Policy and Educational Level in Ecuador—Findings from the National Nutrition Survey 2018. Curr. Dev. Nutr. 2020, 4, 1731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Sandoval, L.A.; Carpio, C.E.; Sanchez-Plata, M. The effect of ‘Traffic-Light’nutritional labelling in carbonated soft drink purchases in Ecuador. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0222866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Peñaherrera, V.; Carpio, C.; Sandoval, L.; Sánchez, M.; Cabrera, T.; Guerrero, P.; Borja, I. Effect of traffic-light labeling on nutritional content and on consumption of carbonated beverages in EcuadorEfeito da rotulagem nutricional com modelo de semáforo no consumo de refrigerantes no Equador. Rev. Panam. De Salud Publica = Pan Am. J. Public Health 2018, 42, e177. [Google Scholar]
  45. Shahid, M.; Neal, B.; Jones, A. Uptake of Australia’s Health Star Rating System 2014–2019. Nutrients 2020, 12, 1791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Croker, H.; Packer, J.; Russell, S.J.; Stansfield, C.; Viner, R.M. Front of pack nutritional labelling schemes: A systematic review and meta-analysis of recent evidence relating to objectively measured consumption and purchasing. J. Hum. Nutr. Diet. 2020, 33, 518–537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Neal, B.; Crino, M.; Dunford, E.; Gao, A.; Greenland, R.; Li, N.; Ngai, J.; Ni Mhurchu, C.; Pettigrew, S.; Sacks, G. Effects of different types of front-of-pack labelling information on the healthiness of food purchases—A randomised controlled trial. Nutrients 2017, 9, 1284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Ni Mhurchu, C.; Volkova, E.; Jiang, Y.; Eyles, H.; Michie, J.; Neal, B.; Blakely, T.; Swinburn, B.; Rayner, M. Effects of interpretive nutrition labels on consumer food purchases: The Starlight randomized controlled trial. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2017, 105, 695–704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  49. Siegrist, M.; Leins-Hess, R.; Keller, C. Which front-of-pack nutrition label is the most efficient one? The results of an eye-tracker study. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 39, 183–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Ducrot, P.; Méjean, C.; Julia, C.; Kesse-Guyot, E.; Touvier, M.; Fezeu, L.; Hercberg, S.; Péneau, S. Effectiveness of Front-Of-Pack Nutrition Labels in French Adults: Results from the NutriNet-Santé Cohort Study. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0140898. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Ducrot, P.; Julia, C.; Mejean, C.; Kesse-Guyot, E.; Touvier, M.; Fezeu, L.K.; Hercberg, S.; Peneau, S. Impact of Different Front-of-Pack Nutrition Labels on Consumer Purchasing Intentions: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2016, 50, 627–636. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Julia, C.; Péneau, S.; Buscail, C.; Gonzalez, R.; Touvier, M.; Hercberg, S.; Kesse-Guyot, E. Perception of different formats of front-of-pack nutrition labels according to sociodemographic, lifestyle and dietary factors in a French population: Cross-sectional study among the NutriNet-Santé cohort participants. BMJ Open 2017, 7, e016108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Talati, Z.; Norman, R.; Pettigrew, S.; Neal, B.; Kelly, B.; Dixon, H.; Ball, K.; Miller, C.; Shilton, T. The impact of interpretive and reductive front-of-pack labels on food choice and willingness to pay. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2017, 14, 171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  54. Vargas-Meza, J.; Jáuregui, A.; Contreras-Manzano, A.; Nieto, C.; Barquera, S. Acceptability and understanding of front-of-pack nutritional labels: An experimental study in Mexican consumers. BMC Public Health 2019, 19, 1751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Jáuregui, A.; Vargas-Meza, J.; Nieto, C.; Contreras-Manzano, A.; Alejandro, N.Z.; Tolentino-Mayo, L.; Hall, M.G.; Barquera, S. Impact of front-of-pack nutrition labels on consumer purchasing intentions: A randomized experiment in low- and middle-income Mexican adults. BMC Public Health 2020, 20, 463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Deliza, R.; de Alcantara, M.; Pereira, R.; Ares, G. How do different warning signs compare with the guideline daily amount and traffic-light system? Food Qual. Prefer. 2020, 80, 103821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Temple, N.J. Front-of-package food labels: A narrative review. Appetite 2020, 144, 104485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Hock, K.; Acton, R.B.; Jáuregui, A.; Vanderlee, L.; White, C.M.; Hammond, D. Experimental study of front-of-package nutrition labels’ efficacy on perceived healthfulness of sugar-sweetened beverages among youth in six countries. Prev. Med. Rep. 2021, 24, 101577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Saleem, S.M.; Bhattacharya, S.; Deshpande, N. Non-communicable diseases, type 2 diabetes, and influence of front of package nutrition labels on consumer’s behaviour: Reformulations and future scope. Diabetes Metab. Syndr. Clin. Res. Rev. 2022, 16, 102422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Pande, R.; Gavaravarapu, S.M.; Kulkarni, B. Front-of-pack nutrition labelling in India. Lancet Public Health 2020, 5, e195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Sahay, A.; Sanghvi, R.; Ghosh, R.K. Consumer Preferences for Different Nutrition Front-of-Pack Labels in India. Available online: https://fssai.gov.in/upload/advisories/2022/02/6214c8459e38dIIMA_FoPL_Report_22_02_2022.pdf (accessed on 1 July 2022).
  62. Bopape, M.; Taillie, L.S.; Frank, T.; Murukutla, N.; Cotter, T.; Majija, L.; Swart, R. South African consumers’ perceptions of front-of-package warning labels on unhealthy foods and drinks. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0257626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Hall, M.G.; Lazard, A.J.; Grummon, A.H.; Higgins, I.C.A.; Bercholz, M.; Richter, A.P.C.; Taillie, L.S. Designing warnings for sugary drinks: A randomized experiment with Latino parents and non-Latino parents. Prev. Med. 2021, 148, 106562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Grummon, A.H.; Taillie, L.S.; Golden, S.D.; Hall, M.G.; Ranney, L.M.; Brewer, N.T. Sugar-Sweetened Beverage Health Warnings and Purchases: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2019, 57, 601–610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Hall, M.G.; Grummon, A.H.; Higgins, I.C.A.; Lazard, A.J.; Prestemon, C.E.; Avendano-Galdamez, M.I.; Taillie, L.S. The impact of pictorial health warnings on purchases of sugary drinks for children: A randomized controlled trial. PLoS Med. 2022, 19, e1003885. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Commonwealth of Australia, Department of Health. Health Star Rating Calculator. Available online: http://www.healthstarrating.gov.au/internet/healthstarrating/publishing.nsf/Content/excel-calculator (accessed on 1 January 2022).
  67. Indian Council of Medical Research-National Institute of Nutrition, Department of Health Research, Ministry of Health and Family Welfare, Government of India. Nutrient Requirements for Indians. Available online: https://www.nin.res.in/RDA_Full_Report_2020.html (accessed on 1 January 2022).
  68. Beatty, P.C.; Willis, G.B. Research Synthesis: The Practice of Cognitive Interviewing. Public Opin. Q. 2007, 71, 287–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Noar, S.M.; Barker, J.; Bell, T.; Yzer, M. Does Perceived Message Effectiveness Predict the Actual Effectiveness of Tobacco Education Messages? A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Health Commun. 2020, 35, 148–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Taillie, L.S.; Hall, M.G.; Popkin, B.M.; Ng, S.W.; Murukutla, N. Experimental Studies of Front-of-Package Nutrient Warning Labels on Sugar-Sweetened Beverages and Ultra-Processed Foods: A Scoping Review. Nutrients 2020, 12, 569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  71. PAHO. Recommendations from a Pan American Health Organization Expert Consultation on the Marketing of Food and Non-Alcoholic Beverages to Children in the Americas; Pan American Health Organization: Washington, DC, USA, 2012; pp. 1–41. [Google Scholar]
  72. Regional Office for South-East Asia, World Health Organization. WHO Nutrient Profile Model for South-East Asia Region; WHO Regional Office for South-East Asia: New Delhi, India, 2017; p. 22. [Google Scholar]
  73. Pan American Health Organization, World Health Organization. Pan American Health Organization Nutrient Profile Model; Pan American Health Organization: Washington, DC, USA, 2016; p. 32. [Google Scholar]
  74. Mora-Plazas, M.; Aida Higgins, I.C.; Gomez, L.F.; Hall, M.; Parra, M.F.; Bercholz, M.; Murukutla, N.; Taillie, L.S. Impact of nutrient warning labels on choice of ultra-processed food and drinks high in sugar, sodium, and saturated fat in Colombia: A randomized controlled trial. PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0263324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Egnell, M.; Talati, Z.; Hercberg, S.; Pettigrew, S.; Julia, C. Objective Understanding of Front-of-Package Nutrition Labels: An International Comparative Experimental Study across 12 Countries. Nutrients 2018, 10, 1542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  76. Vanderlee, L.; Franco-Arellano, B.; Ahmed, M.; Oh, A.; Lou, W.; L’Abbé, M.R. The efficacy of ‘high in’warning labels, health star and traffic light front-of-package labelling: An online randomised control trial. Public Health Nutr. 2021, 24, 62–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Anderson, C.L.; O’Connor, E.L. The effect of the health star rating on consumer decision-making. Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 73, 215–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Baig, S.A.; Noar, S.M.; Gottfredson, N.C.; Lazard, A.J.; Ribisl, K.M.; Brewer, N.T. Incremental criterion validity of message perceptions and effects perceptions in the context of anti-smoking messages. J. Behav. Med. 2021, 44, 74–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Baig, S.A.; Noar, S.M.; Gottfredson, N.C.; Boynton, M.H.; Ribisl, K.M.; Brewer, N.T. UNC Perceived Message Effectiveness: Validation of a brief scale. Ann. Behav. Med. A Publ. Soc. Behav. Med. 2018, 53, 732–742. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Grummon, A.H.; Hall, M.G.; Taillie, L.S.; Brewer, N.T. How should sugar-sweetened beverage health warnings be designed? A randomized experiment. Prev. Med. 2019, 121, 158–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Noar, S.M.; Bell, T.; Kelley, D.; Barker, J.; Yzer, M. Perceived message effectiveness measures in tobacco education campaigns: A systematic review. Commun. Methods Meas. 2018, 12, 295–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Hall, M.G.; Grummon, A.H.; Lazard, A.J.; Maynard, O.M.; Taillie, L.S. Reactions to graphic and text health warnings for cigarettes, sugar-sweetened beverages, and alcohol: An online randomized experiment of US adults. Prev. Med. 2020, 137, 106120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. The World Bank. Adult Literacy Rate in India, Age 15+; World Bank: Washington, DC, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  84. Houts, P.S.; Doak, C.C.; Doak, L.G.; Loscalzo, M.J. The role of pictures in improving health communication: A review of research on attention, comprehension, recall, and adherence. Patient Educ. Couns. 2006, 61, 173–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Mutti, S.; Reid, J.L.; Gupta, P.C.; Pednekar, M.S.; Dhumal, G.; Nargis, N.; Hussain, A.G.; Hammond, D. Perceived effectiveness of text and pictorial health warnings for smokeless tobacco packages in Navi Mumbai, India, and Dhaka, Bangladesh: Findings from an experimental study. Tob. Control 2016, 25, 437–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  86. Cantrell, J.; Vallone, D.M.; Thrasher, J.F.; Nagler, R.H.; Feirman, S.P.; Muenz, L.R.; He, D.Y.; Viswanath, K. Impact of tobacco-related health warning labels across socioeconomic, race and ethnic groups: Results from a randomized web-based experiment. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e52206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Front-of-package labels (FOPLs).
Figure 1. Front-of-package labels (FOPLs).
Nutrients 14 03128 g001
Figure 2. Percent of participants who correctly identified that products were high in nutrient(s) of concern by study arm. *** p-Value < 0.001 relative to the control label; ** p-Value < 0.01; GDA—Guideline Daily Amount, HSR—Health Star Rating, MTL—Multiple Traffic Light Label.
Figure 2. Percent of participants who correctly identified that products were high in nutrient(s) of concern by study arm. *** p-Value < 0.001 relative to the control label; ** p-Value < 0.01; GDA—Guideline Daily Amount, HSR—Health Star Rating, MTL—Multiple Traffic Light Label.
Nutrients 14 03128 g002
Figure 3. Mean purchase intentions by study arm. GDA—Guideline Daily Amount, HSR—Health Star Rating, MTL—Multiple Traffic Light Label.
Figure 3. Mean purchase intentions by study arm. GDA—Guideline Daily Amount, HSR—Health Star Rating, MTL—Multiple Traffic Light Label.
Nutrients 14 03128 g003
Figure 4. Percent of participants selecting a particular FOPL.
Figure 4. Percent of participants selecting a particular FOPL.
Nutrients 14 03128 g004
Table 1. Socio-demographic characteristics of the sample, n (%).
Table 1. Socio-demographic characteristics of the sample, n (%).
pControl
(n = 574)
Warning
(n = 598)
GDA
(n = 554)
HSR
(n = 601)
MTL
(n = 542)
Total
(n = 2869)
State0.114
Odisha 79 (13.8)94 (15.7)92 (16.6)88 (14.6)83 (15.3)436 (15.2)
Uttar Pradesh 83 (14.5)89 (14.9)102 (18.4)90 (15.0)91 (16.8)455 (15.9)
Assam 99 (17.2)110 (18.4)92 (16.6)90 (15.0)75 (13.8)466 (16.2)
Delhi 110 (19.2)82 (13.7)94 (17.0)108 (18.0)97 (17.9)491 (17.1)
Karnataka 96 (16.7)119 (19.9)94 (17.0)120 (20.0)84 (15.5)513 (17.9)
Gujarat 107 (18.6)104 (17.4)80 (14.4)105 (17.5)112 (20.7)508 (17.7)
Urbanicity0.603
Urban 307 (53.5)289 (48.3)286 (51.6)309 (51.4)285 (52.6)1476 (51.4)
Semi-urban 133 (23.2)168 (28.1)131 (23.6)149 (24.8)124 (22.9)705 (24.6)
Peri-rural 134 (23.3)141 (23.6)137 (24.7)143 (23.8)133 (24.5)688 (24.0)
Age0.880
18–30 year 195 (34.0)209 (34.9)176 (31.8)205 (34.1)190 (35.1)975 (34.0)
31–40 year 200 (34.8)220 (36.8)212 (38.3)211 (35.1)187 (34.5)1030 (35.9)
41–60 year 179 (31.2)169 (28.3)166 (30.0)185 (30.8)165 (30.4)864 (30.1)
Gender0.933
Man 290 (50.5)301 (50.3)286 (51.6)298 (49.6)266 (49.1)1441 (50.2)
Woman 284 (49.5)297 (49.7)268 (48.4)303 (50.4)276 (50.9)1428 (49.8)
Education level0.098
<12 years 256 (44.6)237 (39.6)254 (45.8)255 (42.4)254 (46.9)1256 (43.8)
≥12 years 318 (55.4)361 (60.4)300 (54.2)346 (57.6)288 (53.1)1613 (56.2)
Salty biscuit intake0.279
<1×/week 171 (29.8)167 (27.9)167 (30.1)188 (31.3)166 (30.6)859 (29.9)
1×/week 189 (32.9)211 (35.3)159 (28.7)200 (33.3)189 (34.9)948 (33.0)
>1×/week 214 (37.3)220 (36.8)228 (41.2)213 (35.4)187 (34.5)1062 (37.0)
Sweet biscuit intake0.068
<1×/week 112 (19.5)112 (18.7)119 (21.5)142 (23.6)118 (21.8)603 (21.0)
1×/week 164 (28.6)155 (25.9)164 (29.6)158 (26.3)122 (22.5)763 (26.6)
>1×/week 298 (51.9)331 (55.4)271 (48.9)301 (50.1)302 (55.7)1503 (52.4)
Bread intake0.696
<1×/week 137 (23.9)169 (28.3)146 (26.4)156 (26.0)130 (24.0)738 (25.7)
1×/week 148 (25.8)151 (25.3)145 (26.2)168 (28.0)148 (27.3)760 (26.5)
>1×/week 289 (50.3)278 (46.5)263 (47.5)277 (46.1)264 (48.7)1371 (47.8)
Fruit drink intake0.139
<1×/week 245 (42.7)231 (38.6)248 (44.8)251 (41.8)228 (42.1)1203 (41.9)
1×/week 141 (24.6)139 (23.2)139 (25.1)137 (22.8)145 (26.8)701 (24.4)
>1×/week 188 (32.8)228 (38.1)167 (30.1)213 (35.4)169 (31.2)965 (33.6)
Noodles intake0.515
<1×/week 201 (35.0)202 (33.8)206 (37.2)238 (39.6)190 (35.1)1037 (36.1)
1×/week 139 (24.2)144 (24.1)139 (25.1)145 (24.1)139 (25.6)706 (24.6)
>1×/week 234 (40.8)252 (42.1)209 (37.7)218 (36.3)213 (39.3)1126 (39.2)
Financial situation0.212
Excellent 216 (37.6)247 (41.3)208 (37.5)240 (39.9)200 (36.9)1111 (38.7)
Good 251 (43.7)251 (42.0)259 (46.8)257 (42.8)261 (48.2)1279 (44.6)
Moderate 86 (15.0)80 (13.4)57 (10.3)78 (13.0)60 (11.1)361 (12.6)
Poor 21 (3.7)20 (3.3)30 (5.4)26 (4.3)21 (3.9)118 (4.1)
Mixed language0.274
Yes 231 (40.2)242 (40.5)204 (36.8)251 (41.8)234 (43.2)1162 (40.5)
Intake was measured as the self-reported frequency of weekly consumption over the previous 30 days. Financial situation was rated as follows: excellent—can pay the bills, buy the necessary things, and additional things; good—can pay the bills and buy the necessary things; moderate—can pay the bills but not all the necessary things; poor—cannot pay the bills. Mixed language reflects whether the language was conducted bilingually in the native state language and English. The p-Values were from chi-square tests for differences by arm.
Table 2. Label reactions and product perceptions by arm, mean (95% CI).
Table 2. Label reactions and product perceptions by arm, mean (95% CI).
ControlWarningGDAHSRMTL
Mean
(95% CI)
Mean
(95% CI)
pMean
(95% CI)
pMean
(95% CI)
pMean
(95% CI)
p
Product perceptions
The product is
Unhealthy1.7
(1.5, 1.8)
2.1
(1.9, 2.3)
<0.0011.9
(1.7, 2.2)
0.0021.8
(1.7, 2.0)
0.0032.0
(1.8, 2.2)
<0.001
Visually attractive2.7
(2.5, 2.9)
2.8
(2.7, 3.0)
0.1712.9
(2.7, 3.1)
0.0262.8
(2.6, 2.9)
0.2332.9
(2.7, 3.0)
0.050
Label reactions
The label…
Grabs my attention2.7
(2.6, 2.9)
2.9
(2.8, 3.1)
0.0313.0
(2.8, 3.1)
0.0042.8
(2.7, 3.0)
0.0403.0
(2.8, 3.1)
0.004
Makes me concerned
about health consequences
1.9
(1.7, 2.1)
2.4
(2.2, 2.6)
<0.0012.3
(2.0, 2.5)
<0.0012.2
(2.0, 2.3)
<0.0012.3
(2.1, 2.5)
<0.001
Is easy to understand2.4
(2.2, 2.6)
2.8
(2.7, 3.0)
<0.0012.8
(2.7, 3.0)
<0.0012.7
(2.6, 2.9)
<0.0012.8
(2.6, 2.9)
<0.001
Taught me something new2.3
(2.1, 2.5)
2.8
(2.6, 3.0)
<0.0012.8
(2.6, 3.0)
<0.0012.7
(2.5, 2.8)
<0.0012.7
(2.5, 2.9)
<0.001
Is true2.6
(2.4, 2.7)
2.9
(2.7, 3.0)
<0.0012.9
(2.7, 3.1)
<0.0012.7
(2.5, 2.8)
0.0662.8
(2.6, 3.0)
0.006
Liking the label2.7
(2.5, 2.9)
2.9
(2.7, 3.0)
0.0722.9
(2.8, 3.1)
0.0352.8
(2.7, 3.0)
0.0542.9
(2.8, 3.1)
0.006
PME1.7
(1.5, 1.8)
2.1
(1.9, 2.3)
<0.0011.9
(1.7, 2.0)
<0.0011.9
(1.8, 2.1)
<0.0012.0
(1.8, 2.2)
<0.001
PME—perceived message effectiveness.
Table 3. Effect of FOPL type on the percent of participants who correctly identified products high in nutrients of concern, moderated by socio-demographic characteristics.
Table 3. Effect of FOPL type on the percent of participants who correctly identified products high in nutrients of concern, moderated by socio-demographic characteristics.
ControlWarningGDAHSRMTL
%
(95% CI)
%
(95% CI)
Pa%
(95% CI)
Pa%
(95% CI)
PaMean
(95% CI)
Pa
Education
<12 years
35.2
(27.3, 43.1)
51.5
(42.3, 60.6)
<0.00149.5
(40.3, 58.7)
<0.00145.2
(36.3, 54.2)
0.00246.7
(37.9, 55.6)
<0.001
≥12 years
42.3
(34.8, 49.8)
66.6
(59.5, 73.7)
<0.00159.6
(51.9, 67.3)
<0.00144.8
(36.3, 53.4)
0.43761.8
(54.7, 68.9)
<0.001
Pb 0.073 0.469 0.120 0.074
Language of interview
State language
41.5
(34.1, 48.8)
63.3
(55.6, 71.1)
<0.00154.9
(45.4, 64.4)
0.00144.6
(35.6, 53.6)
0.27957.6
(50.8, 64.4)
<0.001
Mixed (state language and English)
35.5
(23.8, 47.3)
57.0
(44.2, 69.7)
0.00155.2
(42.9, 67.5)
<0.00145.7
(33.4, 57.9)
0.00251.0
(38.0, 64.1)
0.001
Pb 0.955 0.354 0.103 0.910
Urbanicity
Urban
40.0
(32.2, 47.7)
59.9
(52.7, 67.1)
<0.00154.0
(44.4, 63.5)
0.00147.3
(39.2, 55.3)
0.01354.6
(47.0, 62.2)
<0.001
Semi-urban
44.1
(36.0, 52.2)
68.0
(59.9, 76.0)
<0.00158.7
(48.8, 68.7)
0.00444.8
(34.1, 55.4)
0.85855.7
(46.2, 65.1)
0.015
Peri-rural
32.2
(24.1, 40.3)
53.8
(44.0, 63.5)
<0.00153.6
(45.7, 61.5)
<0.00140.4
(30.4, 50.5)
0.05454.6
(46.7, 62.4)
<0.001
Pb 0.678 0.273 0.244 0.135
Gender
Men
40.6
(32.3, 48.9)
61.2
(54.4, 68.0)
<0.00154.2
(45.5, 62.9)
<0.00146.6
(37.9, 55.2)
0.09454.3
(46.0, 62.7)
<0.001
Women
37.6
(30.1, 45.2)
60.4
(51.4, 69.3)
<0.00155.9
(46.4, 65.4)
<0.00143.5
(34.8, 52.2)
0.05455.3
(47.5, 63.0)
<0.001
Pb 0.607 0.316 0.981 0.301
State
Odisha
47.1
(32.2, 62.1)
46.4
(30.9, 62.0)
0.92350.4
(36.7, 64.0)
0.52139.1
(24.0, 54.2)
0.02442.7
(25.5, 59.8)
0.322
Uttar Pradesh
54.1
(32.7, 75.5)
86.0
(76.7, 95.2)
0.00286.0
(77.7, 94.3)
0.00168.5
(47.1, 90.0)
0.01178.9
(68.0, 89.8)
0.012
Assam
25.8
(11.3, 40.3)
47.2
(34.7, 59.8)
0.00233.2
(19.5, 47.0)
0.13821.9
(6.0, 37.9)
0.12145.8
(31.2, 60.3)
0.002
Delhi
23.2
(9.1, 37.3)
59.5
(42.5, 76.5)
<0.00142.6
(27.6, 57.5)
0.03730.4
(23.1, 37.8)
0.22942.0
(27.2, 56.8)
0.014
Karnataka
57.4
(43.8, 71.0)
72.2
(58.4, 86.0)
<0.00160.7
(47.0, 74.4)
0.17863.4
(51.8, 75.0)
0.02664.8
(55.4, 74.3)
0.046
Gujarat
33.4
(18.2, 48.6)
53.8
(34.6, 73.1)
0.02352.4
(34.5, 70.3)
0.06344.5
(24.2, 64.8)
0.10953.0
(36.8, 69.2)
0.006
Pb 0.025 0.027 <0.001 0.002
Sweet biscuit intake
<1×/week
25.0
(13.1, 36.9)
51.8
(36.4, 67.2)
<0.00136.1
(22.7, 49.5)
0.07127.5
(16.4, 38.6)
0.64536.4
(27.0, 45.9)
0.050
1×/week
22.0
(14.0, 29.9)
39.4
(27.1, 51.6)
0.00235.4
(22.1, 48.6)
0.01327.8
(16.2, 39.5)
0.23129.5
(20.1, 38.9)
0.095
>1×/week
26.2
(15.9, 36.5)
49.2
(38.7, 59.8)
<0.00142.4
(29.7, 55.1)
0.00130.9
(19.6, 42.2)
0.21836.8
(26.4, 47.2)
0.006
Pb 0.347 0.732 0.894 0.788
Bread intake
<1×/week
42.3
(31.0, 53.7)
66.3
(54.8, 77.7)
<0.00150.0
(39.4, 60.6)
0.20550.0
(37.8, 62.2)
0.32061.5
(52.7, 70.4)
0.002
1×/week
50.0
(37.0, 63.0)
62.9
(48.6, 77.2)
0.09154.5
(41.9, 67.1)
0.55550.0
(38.0, 62.0)
1.00060.8
(48.1, 73.5)
0.110
>1×/week
45.0
(35.2, 54.8)
73.0
(64.8, 81.2)
<0.00161.6
(50.0, 73.2)
0.00148.0
(36.7, 59.3)
0.45562.5
(51.4, 73.6)
0.004
Pb 0.148 0.292 0.718 0.582
Fruit drink intake
<1×/week
58.4
(47.2, 69.6)
75.8
(66.0, 85.5)
<0.00173.8
(63.0, 84.5)
0.00569.7
(57.6, 81.8)
0.04175.0
(62.7, 87.3)
<0.001
1×/week
59.6
(46.6, 72.6)
79.1
(68.7, 89.6)
0.00477.0
(66.4, 87.6)
0.01261.3
(44.5, 78.1)
0.79875.2
(64.4, 86.0)
0.017
>1×/week
63.8
(52.2, 75.5)
76.3
(66.1, 86.5)
0.02070.7
(58.6, 82.7)
0.26459.2
(47.0, 71.3)
0.36570.4
(60.2, 80.6)
0.266
Pb 0.522 0.363 0.138 0.336
Noodle intake
<1×/week
31.3
(20.9, 41.8)
62.9
(52.5, 73.2)
<0.00148.5
(36.9, 60.2)
0.00444.1
(30.6, 57.6)
0.02854.7
(46.5, 63.0)
<0.001
1×/week
29.5
(18.2, 40.8)
54.2
(41.6, 66.8)
<0.00147.5
(36.7, 58.3)
0.00444.1
(34.0, 54.3)
0.00741.7
(30.1, 53.4)
0.070
>1×/week
27.4
(18.4, 36.3)
46.8
(35.0, 58.6)
<0.00145.9
(33.6, 58.3)
0.00234.4
(22.9, 45.9)
0.09753.5
(41.4, 65.6)
<0.001
Pb 0.207 0.983 0.559 0.218
Savory biscuit intake
<1×/week
32.7
(20.8, 44.7)
52.7
(38.9, 66.5)
0.00152.1
(40.7, 63.4)
<0.00146.3
(33.9, 58.6)
0.01353.6
(43.4, 63.9)
0.001
1×/week
40.2
(29.2, 51.2)
59.2
(48.8, 69.7)
0.00257.2
(45.2, 69.3)
0.01040.5
(29.6, 51.4)
0.95151.9
(41.5, 62.2)
0.037
>1×/week
34.6
(22.9, 46.3)
60.0
(47.4, 72.6)
<0.00161.0
(47.3, 74.7)
<0.00144.6
(30.3, 58.9)
0.01757.8
(42.2, 73.3)
0.001
Pb 0.673 0.344 0.096 0.267
Pa is the p-Value for no difference between a given FOPL and the control at a given moderator level. Pb is the p-Value for equal differences with the control mean across moderation levels within FOPL.
Table 4. Effect of FOPL type on intentions to consume products high in nutrients of concern, moderated by socio-demographic characteristics.
Table 4. Effect of FOPL type on intentions to consume products high in nutrients of concern, moderated by socio-demographic characteristics.
ControlWarningGDAHSRMTL
Mean
(95% CI)
Mean
(95% CI)
PaMean
(95% CI)
PaMean
(95% CI)
PaMean
(95% CI)
Pa
Education
<12 years2.6
(2.4, 2.8)
2.5
(2.3, 2.7)
0.4082.6
(2.5, 2.8)
0.5072.6
(2.5, 2.8)
0.7432.5
(2.4, 2.7)
0.405
≥12 years 2.6
(2.5, 2.8)
2.4
(2.3, 2.6)
0.0142.6
(2.5, 2.8)
0.8752.6
(2.4, 2.7)
0.4332.5
(2.4, 2.7)
0.256
Pb 0.298 0.647 0.503 0.941
Language of interview
State language
2.7
(2.5, 2.9)
2.6
(2.4, 2.8)
0.0862.7
(2.5, 2.9)
0.8012.7
(2.5, 2.8)
0.4462.6
(2.4, 2.8)
0.159
Mixed (state language and English)2.4
(2.2, 2.7)
2.3
(2.1, 2.5)
0.1152.5
(2.3, 2.7)
0.2842.5
(2.3, 2.6)
0.6062.4
(2.2, 2.6)
0.906
Pb 0.720 0.329 0.377 0.312
Urbanicity
Urban
2.6
(2.4, 2.8)
2.4
(2.3, 2.6)
0.0052.6
(2.5, 2.8)
0.7932.5
(2.3, 2.6)
0.0332.5
(2.4, 2.7)
0.256
Semi-urban
2.6
(2.4, 2.7)
2.6
(2.4, 2.8)
0.6432.7
(2.6, 2.9)
0.0532.7
(2.5, 2.8)
0.1392.5
(2.3, 2.6)
0.299
Peri-rural
2.6
(2.4, 2.9)
2.4
(2.2, 2.6)
0.0332.6
(2.4, 2.8)
0.5082.7
(2.6, 2.9)
0.3122.6
(2.4, 2.9)
0.944
Pb 0.055 0.208 0.004 0.780
Gender
Men
2.6
(2.4, 2.8)
2.5
(2.3, 2.7)
0.1132.7
(2.5, 2.9)
0.7052.6
(2.4, 2.8)
0.7412.6
(2.4, 2.8)
0.501
Women
2.6
(2.4, 2.8)
2.4
(2.3, 2.6)
0.0702.6
(2.5, 2.8)
0.7282.6
(2.4, 2.7)
0.9922.5
(2.3, 2.7)
0.235
Pb 0.765 0.916 0.806 0.788
State
Odisha
2.4
(2.1, 2.7)
2.2
(2.0, 2.5)
0.1232.2
(1.9, 2.4)
0.0612.2
(2.0, 2.5)
0.0332.1
(1.8, 2.4)
0.002
Uttar Pradesh
2.7
(2.2, 3.1)
2.6
(2.3, 3.0)
0.9452.8
(2.4, 3.1)
0.6102.7
(2.4, 3.0)
0.9762.8
(2.4, 3.1)
0.552
Assam
3.1
(2.9, 3.3)
2.8
(2.7, 3.0)
0.0433.1
(2.9, 3.3)
0.9513.1
(2.8, 3.3)
0.7143.0
(2.9, 3.1)
0.217
Delhi
2.3
(2.1, 2.6)
2.0
(1.6, 2.3)
0.0042.5
(2.2, 2.8)
0.2502.4
(2.1, 2.6)
0.6632.3
(2.1, 2.5)
0.910
Karnataka
2.7
(2.5, 2.9)
2.8
(2.5, 3.1)
0.3622.8
(2.6, 3.0)
0.3222.7
(2.5, 2.9)
0.7072.7
(2.4, 3.1)
0.847
Gujarat
2.4
(2.0, 2.9)
2.2
(1.8, 2.6)
0.0152.4
(2.1, 2.7)
0.9002.5
(2.1, 2.8)
0.8082.4
(2.1, 2.7)
0.608
Pb 0.056 0.304 0.492 0.177
Sweet biscuit intake
<1×/week
2.6
(2.3, 2.8)
2.5
(2.2, 2.7)
0.5552.5
(2.3, 2.8)
0.8452.5
(2.3, 2.7)
0.6542.3
(2.1, 2.5)
0.048
1×/week
2.6
(2.4, 2.8)
2.5
(2.3, 2.7)
0.3412.7
(2.5, 2.9)
0.4702.7
(2.5, 2.8)
0.3372.7
(2.5, 2.9)
0.407
>1×/week
2.7
(2.5, 2.9)
2.5
(2.3, 2.7)
0.0562.7
(2.5, 3.0)
0.6752.7
(2.5, 2.9)
0.8842.6
(2.4, 2.9)
0.500
Pb 0.820 0.718 0.627 0.070
Bread intake
<1×/week
2.5
(2.3, 2.8)
2.4
(2.1, 2.7)
0.4702.5
(2.3, 2.7)
0.7682.5
(2.2, 2.7)
0.5152.2
(2.0, 2.4)
0.029
1×/week
2.6
(2.3, 2.9)
2.4
(2.1, 2.7)
0.0792.5
(2.3, 2.6)
0.2072.6
(2.4, 2.7)
0.7162.6
(2.3, 2.8)
0.779
>1×/week
2.7
(2.5, 2.9)
2.6
(2.4, 2.8)
0.5272.8
(2.6, 3.0)
0.2122.8
(2.7, 3.0)
0.1122.7
(2.4, 2.9)
0.663
Pb 0.724 0.142 0.325 0.140
Fruit drink intake
<1×/week
2.5
(2.3, 2.8)
2.4
(2.1, 2.7)
0.2712.6
(2.4, 2.8)
0.4372.5
(2.3, 2.7)
0.9962.3
(2.1, 2.6)
0.100
1×/week
2.5
(2.3, 2.8)
2.4
(2.2, 2.6)
0.4852.5
(2.3, 2.7)
0.8762.6
(2.4, 2.8)
0.4702.6
(2.4, 2.8)
0.491
>1×/week
2.7
(2.5, 2.8)
2.5
(2.3, 2.7)
0.0942.7
(2.5, 3.0)
0.4532.7
(2.5, 2.8)
0.9742.6
(2.4, 2.9)
0.940
Pb 0.765 0.886 0.790 0.220
Noodle intake
<1×/week
2.5
(2.3, 2.7)
2.4
(2.1, 2.7)
0.5562.4
(2.2, 2.7)
0.8262.4
(2.2, 2.6)
0.3372.3
(2.1, 2.5)
0.033
1×/week
2.6
(2.3, 2.8)
2.4
(2.2, 2.6)
0.0962.7
(2.5, 2.9)
0.1592.5
(2.3, 2.7)
0.5592.6
(2.4, 2.8)
0.950
>1×/week
2.6
(2.3, 2.9)
2.5
(2.3, 2.6)
0.2032.6
(2.5, 2.8)
0.7462.6
(2.3, 2.8)
0.8822.6
(2.4, 2.8)
0.818
Pb 0.782 0.500 0.840 0.208
Savory biscuit intake
<1×/week
2.5
(2.3, 2.7)
2.5
(2.3, 2.7)
0.8862.5
(2.3, 2.8)
0.6062.4
(2.2, 2.7)
0.6392.5
(2.3, 2.7)
0.710
1×/week
2.7
(2.4, 2.9)
2.5
(2.4, 2.7)
0.2632.6
(2.4, 2.8)
0.5922.6
(2.4, 2.8)
0.4492.7
(2.5, 2.9)
0.959
>1×/week
2.8
(2.5, 3.0)
2.5
(2.3, 2.8)
0.0122.7
(2.5, 3.0)
0.9572.8
(2.6, 3.0)
0.8342.6
(2.3, 3.0)
0.325
Pb 0.329 0.739 0.783 0.636
Pa is the p-Value for no difference between a given FOPL and the control at a given moderator level. Pb is the p-Value for equal differences with the control mean across moderation levels within FOPL.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Singh, S.K.; Taillie, L.S.; Gupta, A.; Bercholz, M.; Popkin, B.; Murukutla, N. Front-of-Package Labels on Unhealthy Packaged Foods in India: Evidence from a Randomized Field Experiment. Nutrients 2022, 14, 3128. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14153128

AMA Style

Singh SK, Taillie LS, Gupta A, Bercholz M, Popkin B, Murukutla N. Front-of-Package Labels on Unhealthy Packaged Foods in India: Evidence from a Randomized Field Experiment. Nutrients. 2022; 14(15):3128. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14153128

Chicago/Turabian Style

Singh, S. K., Lindsey Smith Taillie, Ashish Gupta, Maxime Bercholz, Barry Popkin, and Nandita Murukutla. 2022. "Front-of-Package Labels on Unhealthy Packaged Foods in India: Evidence from a Randomized Field Experiment" Nutrients 14, no. 15: 3128. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14153128

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop