Next Article in Journal
A Cross-Sectional Study on the Relationship between the Family Nutrition Climate and Children’s Nutrition Behavior
Previous Article in Journal
Comparative Investigation of Frankincense Nutraceuticals: Correlation of Boswellic and Lupeolic Acid Contents with Cytokine Release Inhibition and Toxicity against Triple-Negative Breast Cancer Cells
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Bidirectional Associations between Obesity and Cognitive Function in Midlife Adults: A Longitudinal Study

1
School of Social Sciences, Singapore Management University, Singapore 178903, Singapore
2
Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Singapore 117570, Singapore
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2019, 11(10), 2343; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11102343
Submission received: 31 July 2019 / Revised: 18 September 2019 / Accepted: 19 September 2019 / Published: 2 October 2019

Abstract

:
The links between obesity and cognition remain equivocal due to a variety of methodological limitations with current research, such as an overreliance on body mass index (BMI) as a measure of obesity, the use of cross-sectional designs, and inadequate specification over the domains of cognitive function to be examined. To address these issues, we used data from the Cognitive Project of the National Survey of Midlife Development in the United States, a large-scale, longitudinal dataset on non-institutionalized midlife adults (N = 2652), which enabled us to examine the long-term bidirectional relations between obesity and two latent factors of cognition—executive function and episodic memory—while controlling for potential confounds. Results showed that, over a span of nine years, an increase in obesity in Time 1 is associated with a decline in episodic memory in Time 2 (but not executive function), while an increase in executive function in Time 1 (but not episodic memory) is associated with a reduction in obesity in Time 2. These results were elucidated when obesity was indexed with waist-to-hip ratio but not with BMI. Our findings highlight important directions for further research, in particular the use of more valid obesity indices and a greater focus on the bidirectional effects between obesity and cognition.

1. Introduction

Obesity is a widely recognized risk factor for a variety of chronic physical health ailments including metabolic syndrome, hypertension, cardiovascular disease, diabetes, stroke, and cancer [1]. Less known, however, is the impact of obesity on cognitive functioning. On the one hand, excess adiposity has been implicated in neurocognitive decline, with some studies finding obesity to be associated with reduced cognitive ability [2,3,4], increased risk of dementia [5,6], and structural brain changes such as excess age-related atrophy and white matter disease [7,8,9]. On the other hand, findings on the cognitively detrimental effects of obesity appear to be mixed. For example, some recent studies failed to find evidence that obesity increases dementia risk [10,11], while others even found underweight individuals to be at greater risk instead, thereby counterintuitively presenting obesity as a protective factor [12,13].
Several methodological factors may account for these mixed findings. First, the influence of obesity on cognitive function may be obscured by the popular use of body mass index (BMI) as a measure of obesity, which has been increasingly shown to be suboptimal to alternative measures such as waist-to-hip ratio (WHR) [14]. For instance, in one large sample of midlife adults, WHR (but not BMI) was linked to cognitive deficits over and above the influence of demographics, health factors, personality, and self-perceived obesity [15]. Second, an overreliance on cross-sectional designs also limits our conclusions to static associations between obesity and cognitive functioning at single time points [16], and most studies neglect the possible bidirectional effects between obesity and cognition [17,18]. On the one hand, impaired cognition may hinder people’s ability to regulate weight [19,20]; on the other hand, obesity may produce neurological changes [21,22,23] that underlie reduced motivation, self-control, and cognitive ability [24]. Lastly, the impact of obesity on cognition appears to be domain specific. For instance, obesity has been documented to impair more memory-related cognitive functions, such as episodic memory, rather than executive functions [25].
To address these methodological gaps and establish greater precision over the links between obesity and cognition, the present study sought to investigate the bidirectional relationships between obesity and two key domains of cognitive ability, specifically executive functions and episodic memory, using a large-scale, longitudinal dataset on midlife adults. In so doing, we simultaneously shed light on the bidirectional nature and domain-specificity of the obesity–cognition relationship as well as how this relationship unfolds over time. To achieve these objectives, we draw from the Cognitive Project of the National Survey of Midlife Development in the United States (MIDUS), which affords an invaluable opportunity to examine our relationships of interest with large, non-clinical, and representative samples over multiple time points.

2. Method

Participants

The current study comprises 2683 midlife adults who participated in the Cognitive Project under the second (II) and third (III) waves of the MIDUS (Midlife Development in the United States). MIDUS II was conducted between 2004 and 2006 on a random-digit-dial sample of non-institutionalized, English-speaking adults. MIDUS II participants were aged between 33 and 84 (M = 55.04, SD = 11.21) with females making up 55% of the sample. MIDUS III was conducted between 2013 and 2017 as a follow-up to MIDUS II using the same methodology and assessments. In accordance with established exclusion criteria [25], we excluded underweight participants (BMI > 18.5) in both waves to omit effects associated with being underweight on cognition (e.g., health problems, malnourishment due to poverty, anorexia nervosa, etc.; n = 31), which resulted in a final sample size of 2652. Table 1 summarizes the descriptive statistics of our sample across both waves. All participants in the MIDUS project provided informed consent and data collection was approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Wisconsin-Madison. Data and materials of the current study can be accessed from the Inter-University Consortium for Political and Social Research website.

3. Measures

3.1. Cognitive Ability

Our focal subdomains of cognition—executive functions and episodic memory—were measured in the Cognitive Project of MIDUS II and MIDUS III with the Brief Test of Adult Cognition by Telephone (BTACT) [27]. The BTACT is a well-validated battery of cognitive function tests comprising the Immediate Word List Recall Task, Backward Digits Span, Categorical Fluency, Stop and Go Switch Task (SGST), Number Series, Backward Counting Task, and Delayed Word List Recall. Both exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses of the seven cognitive tests in the BTACT reveal that the data fits a two-factor model of executive function and episodic memory [28,29]. Executive function is best represented by performance on the Backward digit Span, Categorical Fluency, Number Series, Backward Counting, and SGST, while episodic memory is best represented by performance on the Immediate Word List Recall Task and Delayed Word List Recall.

3.2. Obesity

Obesity was indexed in terms of BMI and WHR. Participants’ BMI was computed with their self-reported weight and height based on the formula where BMI equals to kilograms per meters squared. Participants’ WHR was computed by taking the ratio of their waist around the navel to their hips at the wide point. To ensure the accuracy of the reported body measurements, participants were provided with a tape measure during the study and instructed to stand upright, keep the tape measure taut to the body, and measure at the level of their navel for the waist measurement and the wide point between their waist and thighs for the hip measurement. Based on the recommendations of previous studies [30], we operationalized BMI and WHR as continuous variables to minimize bias associated with treating obesity as a categorical variable.

4. Data Analysis

The present study aimed to examine the bidirectional longitudinal associations between obesity and two important domains underlying cognitive function: Executive functions and episodic memory. To accomplish this objective, we conducted our analyses with a two-wave cross-lagged design and structural equation modelling using maximum likelihood estimate with robust standard errors (MLR) on Mplus version 7.4, which is robust to non-normality and non-independence of observation [31]. The cross-lagged panel models included autoregressive paths, cross-sectional paths between obesity and cognitive ability, and cross-lagged reciprocal paths between obesity and cognitive ability. Separate models were conducted on BMI and WHR to provide greater precision over the estimates of each measure of obesity [15]. In addition, both BMI and WHR were winsorized to minimize the influence of outliers. Missing data were handled with the full information maximum likelihood method, which is a superior, unbiased, and more efficient data imputation method than traditional ad hoc missing-data techniques in structural equation models [32].
Consistent with previous studies that estimated cognitive function in terms of executive functions and episodic memory [28,29], we indexed executive function by performance on the backward digit span, categorical fluency, number series, backward counting, and SGST; and episodic memory by performance on the immediate word list recall and delayed word list recall. To evaluate model fit, we followed an established criterion where acceptable fit is indicated when the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) is less than 0.08, Bentler’s comparative fit index (CFI) and Tucker–Lewis index (TLI) values are above 0.90, and the standardized root mean-squared residual (SRMR) is less than 0.08 [33,34].
Prior to specifying the structural model, we tested for measurement invariance to ensure that our constructs are conceptually consistent over time and that changes in our latent variables are not an artifact of changing measurement properties. The measurement invariance test included examinations of configural (equality in factor structure), metric (equality in factor loadings), and scalar (equality in latent intercepts) invariances [35]. To establish invariance, a change in CFI of less than 0.01 (ΔCFI < 0.01) between models with constraints added is recommended [36].
While retaining the imposed equality constraints, we estimated three structural models each with an additional set of covariates to ensure the robustness of any longitudinal relations revealed by our analyses. First, we estimated the cross-lagged model without controlling for covariates to provide unadjusted estimates of the longitudinal bidirectional associations between obesity and cognition over a span of approximately 9 years. Next, we controlled for potential demographic and socioeconomic status (SES) confounds at Time 1, including age at assessment, gender, education attainment, and household income [37,38,39,40,41]. Third, we controlled for health-related variables that are known to covary with both obesity and cognitive decline, including history of smoking, alcohol abuse, hypertension, diabetes, and stroke [42,43].
Lastly, we conducted exploratory analyses with executive function and episodic memory as a single latent factor of global cognitive function and explored differential associations between obesity and cognitive functions across time and age group as well as across time and gender.

5. Results

5.1. Measurement Invariance

We first tested for measurement invariance to verify the temporal stability of the conceptual and measurement properties of our latent variables (see Table 2). All the autocorrelations among measurement residuals were estimated to account for indicator-specific effects over time [44]. In addition, our factors were specified to have nondirectional covariance relationships. Our analyses showed that the data fitted the configural, metric, and scalar invariance models well (RMSEAs < 0.08, CFIs > 0.90., TLIs > 0.90, and SRMRs < 0.08). Full invariance of the BTACT was supported in our configural and metric models (ΔCFIs ≤ 0.001) but not in the scalar model (ΔCFI ≥ 0.027). Thus, as recommended by Byrne, Shavelson, and Muthén [45], we tested for partial scalar invariance on the basis of full metric invariance by relaxing the constraints on the intercept of the performance on SGST. The modified model fitted the data well, thus confirming partial scalar invariance for the BTACT (ΔCFI = 0.001). Given that the cross-lagged panel model is robust to minor violations of scalar invariance when the majority of our indicators are established as invariant [44], we proceeded with our analyses. In our subsequent structural models, we retained the optimal equality constraints (i.e., partial scalar invariance) to ensure that the measurement properties of our latent variables are stable over time and that the changes in these latent variables are not a byproduct of changes in measurement properties.

5.2. Waist-To-Hip Ratio

With the quality constraints held, we conducted cross-lagged panel models to examine the bidirectional longitudinal relations between WHR and cognitive ability. All lagged paths were converted into directional predictive paths. The autoregressive and cross-lagged paths across all three structural models are summarized in Table 3. We found that WHR at Time 1 significantly predicted a negative change in episodic memory at Time 2 in the unadjusted model (β = −0.125, SE = 0.017, 95% CI [−0.153, −0.097], p < 0.001). Importantly, the cross-lagged path of WHR at Time 1 on episodic memory at Time 2 remained significant even after controlling for demographics and SES covariates (β = −0.045, SE = 0.019, 95% CI [−0.078, −0.013], p = 0.020) as well as health covariates (β = −0.044, SE = 0.019, 95% CI [−0.076, −0.013], p = 0.022), thus suggesting that an increase in WHR is uniquely associated with a higher rate of episodic memory decline after nine years. However, we did not find evidence that WHR at Time 1 predicted changes in executive function at Time 2 in any of our structural models (ps > 0.05). In contrast, we found that executive function at Time 1 significantly predicted reductions in WHR at Time 2 after controlling for demographics and SES (β = −0.073, SE = 0.034, 95% CI [−0.128, −0.018], p = 0.030) and health covariates (β = −0.067, SE = 0.034, 95% CI [−0.123, −0.011], p = 0.048), suggesting that executive function was uniquely associated with higher rate of decrease in WHR (See Figure 1). Lastly, episodic memory at Time 1 was not significantly associated with WHR at Time 2 (ps > 0.05)

5.3. Body Mass Index

Similar structural models were estimated to examine the bidirectional longitudinal relations between BMI and cognitive ability. As shown in Table 3, our analyses did not reveal any cross−lagged relations between BMI and our latent factors of cognitive ability, specifically executive function and episodic memory. In contrast to WHR, BMI at Time 1 did not predict any changes in episodic memory in the unadjusted cross−lagged panel model (β = 0.015, SE = 0.019, 95% CI [−0.016, 0.046], p = 0.426), the cross-lagged panel model with demographic and SES covariates (β = 0.007, SE = 0.018, 95% CI [−0.022, 0.037], p = 0.683), or the cross−lagged panel model with demographic, SES, and health covariates (β = 0.005, SE = 0.019, 95% CI [−0.026, 0.035], p = 0.805). Likewise, neither did executive function nor episodic memory at Time 1 predict changes in BMI at Time 2 across all structural models (ps > 0.05).

5.4. Global Cognitive Function

We further analyzed our data with executive function and episodic memory as a single latent factor of global cognitive function, which is indicated by performance on the backward digit span, categorical fluency, number series, backward counting, SGST, immediate word list recall, and delayed word list recall. Based on our modification indices, we allowed the error terms between immediate word list recall and delayed word list recall within and between two time points to be correlated as both tasks implicate highly overlapping cognitive abilities. As shown in Table 4, after the inclusion of the residual correlation between immediate word list recall and delayed word list recall, our data fitted the configural, metric, and scalar invariance models well (RMSEAs < 0.08, CFIs > 0.90, TLIs > 0.90, and SRMRs < 0.08). Full invariance was supported in our configural and metric models (ΔCFIs ≤ 0.001) but not in the scalar model (ΔCFI ≥ 0.026). After relaxing the constraints on the intercept of the performance on SGST, partial scalar invariance was achieved (ΔCFI = 0.001).
With the equality constraints held, we conducted cross-lagged panel model analyses to examine the bidirectional longitudinal relations between obesity (WHR and BMI) and global cognitive function. As shown in Table 5, our structural models revealed that the latent factor of global cognitive function at Time 1 was significantly associated with WHR at Time 2 after controlling for demographics and SES (β = −0.075, SE = 0.030, 95% CI [−0.133, −0.016], p = 0.012) and health covariates (β = −0.068, SE = 0.030, 95% CI [−0.127, −0.009], p = 0.023). However, WHR at Time 1 was not associated with global cognitive function at Time 2 (ps > 0.05). Similarly, we did not find any evidence of bidirectional longitudinal relations between BMI and global cognitive function after controlling for covariates (ps > 0.05).

5.5. Age and Gender as Moderators

Exploratory analyses were conducted to examine bidirectional associations between obesity (WHR and BMI) and cognitive function across age groups and gender. For age groups, participants were categorized as either younger (aged 30−45), middle−aged (aged 46−59), or older (above 59). Before conducting our exploratory analyses, a series of measurement invariance tests for both WHR and BMI models were conducted across time and age groups as well as across time and gender. Our analyses showed that the data fitted all the configural and metric invariance models well (RMSEAs < 0.08, CFIs > 0.90, TLIs > 0.90, and SRMRs < 0.08). However, our data only fitted well with scalar invariance models across time and gender for WHR (RMSEA = 0.065, CFI = 0.922., TLI = 0.908, and SRMR = 0.072) and BMI (RMSEA = 0.066, CFI = 0.931, TLI = 0.918, and SRMR = 0.073) but not across time and age groups for WHR (RMSEA = 0.074, CFI = 0.893., TLI = 0.875, and SRMR = 0.108) and BMI (RMSEA = 0.073, CFI = 0.907., TLI = 0.892, and SRMR = 0.107). Full invariance across time and age groups was supported in our configural and metric models (ΔCFI ≤ 0.005) but not in the scalar model (ΔCFI > 0.01). Similarly, full invariance across time and gender was supported in our configural and metrics models (ΔCFIs = 0.002) but not in the scalar model (ΔCFIs > 0.01). Inspection on our modification indices showed that majority of our tasks did not achieve scalar invariance across age groups and gender. Thus, for the purpose of our exploratory analyses, we only constrained factor structure and factor loadings in our subsequent multigroup cross-lagged panel models.
To explore age groups and gender as moderators of the bidirectional relations between obesity and cognitive functions, a series of Wald tests of parameter constraints were conducted between the coefficients of our structural models with demographics, SES, and health covariates across age groups. We did not find any significant differences in our cross-lagged paths across age groups (ps > 0.10), except for the relations between WHR at Time 1 and executive function at Time 2 (Wald χ2 (2) = 10.47, p = 0.005). Specifically, WHR at Time 1 was significantly associated with executive function at Time 2 among younger (β = −0.077, SE = 0.035, p = 0.030) and middle-aged (β = −0.062, SE = 0.025, p = 0.011) but not older adults (β = −0.004, SE = 0.031, p = 0.887). Similarly, we did not find any differences in our cross-lagged paths across gender for both WHR and BMI (ps > 0.10).

6. Discussion

The popularity of BMI as a measure of obesity, overreliance on cross-sectional designs, and lack of specificity over the domains that underlie cognitive function contribute to mixed findings on the obesity–cognition link. To address these issues, we used data from the Cognitive Project of the MIDUS, a large-scale, longitudinal dataset on midlife adults, which allowed us to examine the long-term and bidirectional relations between obesity and two underlying domains of cognitive ability, namely executive function and episodic memory.
Our analyses unveiled several key findings. WHR at Time 1 was found to be significantly predictive of poorer episodic memory at Time 2 and this relationship held after controlling for demographic, SES, and health covariates. However, WHR at Time 1 did not predict changes in executive function at Time 2. Our results also showed that executive function at Time 1 was significantly associated with reduced WHR at Time 2 after controlling for potential confounds, but no relationship was found between episodic memory at Time 1 and WHR at Time 2. In contrast to the complex longitudinal associations documented between WHR and cognition, we failed to find any significant associations between BMI at Time 1 and latent cognitive factors at Time 2 as well as between latent cognitive factors at Time 1 and BMI at Time 2. In summary, our investigation based on midlife adults indicated that an increase in WHR is uniquely associated with episodic memory decline after nine years, while an increase in executive function is uniquely associated with reduced WHR over the same time span.
Consistent with previous research, we found WHR to be superior to BMI as a measure of the influence of obesity on cognition [14,15]. Our results therefore suggest that the use of BMI may obscure important associations between excess adiposity and cognitive function. Indeed, BMI does not distinguish between muscle and adipose tissue or directly assess regional adiposity [46], and WHR has also been found to correlate more strongly than BMI does with health risk factors including diabetes and various forms of cardiovascular disease [47,48,49]. As such, further research on obesity should rely less on BMI or at least utilize other measures in addition to BMI.
Second, we replicated previous research showing that WHR is distinctly associated with episodic memory beyond potential demographic, SES, and health confounds [15]. More importantly, WHR at Time 1 was not associated with global cognitive function. The result may suggest that the overall negative effect of WHR is domain-specific to episodic memory. This lends support to the view that obesity has an independent detrimental influence on encoding or retrieval processes. Some candidate mechanisms of poorer episodic memory due to the accumulation of visceral fat include the release of pro-inflammatory cytokines, which can lead to insulin resistance and aggravate cerebrovascular reactivity [50,51,52], and central and adipose inflammation, which may undermine synaptic plasticity [21,53]. However, in contrast to some previous studies, we did not find any evidence of long-term effects of obesity on executive functions [2], nor did we find evidence suggesting that poorer episodic memory increases the likelihood of obesity [16,18].
Our analyses revealed a nuanced relationship between obesity and cognitive ability. Specifically, we found that an increase in obesity is associated with decline in episodic memory but not executive function, and interestingly, we also found an increase in executive function to be associated with a reduction in obesity. Some preliminary evidence indeed suggests that executive function may have an impact on consumption and weight. For instance, deficits in executive function (e.g., attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder) may foster dysregulated eating behaviors, such as binge eating or other forms of eating in the absence of hunger, or hamper regular and structured physical activity, which in turn may contribute to overeating and unsuccessful weight loss, thereby constituting a risk factor for obesity [54]. As these findings and suggestions are insightful but speculative, further research is warranted. Importantly, our results also highlight that the links between obesity and cognition are bidirectional. More attention should therefore be paid toward understanding their reciprocal dynamics.
In our multigroup analyses, we found significant associations between WHR at Time 1 and executive function at Time 2 among younger and middle-aged but not older adults. The results suggest that executive function may still be negatively implicated by WHR in specific age groups. These results are consistent with studies showing that weight gain is negatively associated with cognitive performance in relatively younger individuals, but this relationship is reversed for individuals with a mean age of above 73 [55,56]. As discussed by Smith and colleagues [57], two biological mechanisms may explain these findings. First, overweight men tend to retain more testosterone in their body fat, which may help to buffer against cognitive impairment from increased conversion to estrogen in old age [58]. Second, increased leptin due to higher adiposity in older adults may protect cognition [59]. However, this may be due to a survival effect in an elderly sample [57]. That is, obese middle-aged individuals are more likely to die at younger ages compared with individuals with elderly onset obesity, and those who did not die may have traits that increased survivability.
Some limitations of the present research should be noted. First, the overall effect size of the longitudinal relationships between obesity and cognitive functions is small, which suggests that cognitive function may not be a significant factor in the prevention of obesity and raises the need to interpret the current findings with caution. Second, although we had longitudinal data and were able to rule out numerous confounding factors, our findings are essentially correlational, which limits our ability to make causal claims. Further research that employs controlled comparisons between groups of obese and non-obese individuals over multiple time points are warranted. By increasing the resolution of the bidirectional effects between cognition and weight, we can gain a better understanding of the protective and risk factors of weight on cognitive decline at specific ages or life stages. Furthermore, adiposity fat percentage [60] and diet have a demonstrable effect on cognition [17,61], but we were unable to include these factors as covariates due to the lack of such information in the MIDUS dataset. In sum, future investigations should strive to make systematic comparisons of cognitive ability between groups of individuals varying in weight while accounting for percentage of adiposity fat, dietary habits, and other potential confounds.
Taken together, the current investigation reveals a set of nuanced long-term relationships between obesity and episodic memory as well as obesity and executive function, and that these associations are best illuminated with WHR rather than BMI. As such, we highlight the need for future studies to consider the complex bidirectional relations between obesity and cognition and use more sensitive measures of obesity, in particular WHR with more carefully controlled studies.

Author Contributions

A.H. developed the study concept and design. A.H. performed the statistical analysis. A.H., J.C.Y., and W.X.T. drafted the manuscript and provided critical revisions.

Funding

The MIDUS I study was supported by a grant from the John D. Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Successful Midlife Development. The MIDUS II and MIDUS III was supported by the National Institute on Aging (P01−AG020166, U19−AG051426, T32−AG000204) to conduct a longitudinal follow−up of the MIDUS I study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no competing interests. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Kopelman, P.G. Obesity as a medical problem. Nature 2000, 404, 635–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Gunstad, J.; Spitznagel, M.B.; Paul, R.H.; Cohen, R.A.; Kohn, M.; Luyster, F.S.; Clark, R.; Williams, L.M.; Gordon, E. Body mass index and neuropsychological function in healthy children and adolescents. Appetite 2008, 50, 246–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Jeong, S.-K.; Nam, H.-S.; Son, M.-H.; Son, E.-J.; Cho, K.-H. Interactive effect of obesity indexes on cognition. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 2005, 19, 91–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Waldstein, S.R.; Katzel, L.I. Interactive relations of central versus total obesity and blood pressure to cognitive function. Int. J. Obes. 2006, 30, 201–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Barnes, D.E.; Yaffe, K. The projected effect of risk factor reduction on Alzheimer’s disease prevalence. Lancet Neurol. 2011, 10, 819–828. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Norton, S.; Matthews, F.E.; Barnes, D.E.; Yaffe, K.; Brayne, C. Potential for primary prevention of Alzheimer’s disease: An analysis of population-based data. Lancet Neurol. 2014, 13, 788–794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Gustafson, D.; Lissner, L.; Bengtsson, C.; Björkelund, C.; Skoog, I. A 24-year follow-up of body mass index and cerebral atrophy. Neurology 2004, 63, 1876–1881. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Gustafson, D.R.; Steen, B.; Skoog, I. Body mass index and white matter lesions in elderly women. An 18-year longitudinal study. Int. Psychogeriatr. 2004, 16, 327–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Ward, M.A.; Carlsson, C.M.; Trivedi, M.A.; Sager, M.A.; Johnson, S.C. The effect of body mass index on global brain volume in middle-aged adults: A cross sectional study. BMC Neurol. 2005, 5, 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Albanese, E.; Davis, B.; Jonsson, P.V.; Chang, M.; Aspelund, T.; Garcia, M.; Harris, T.; Gudnason, V.; Launer, L.J. Overweight and Obesity in Midlife and Brain Structure and Dementia 26 Years Later. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2015, 181, 672–679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Aslan, A.K.D.; Starr, J.M.; Pattie, A.; Deary, I. Cognitive consequences of overweight and obesity in the ninth decade of life? Age Ageing 2015, 44, 59–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. De Bruijn, R.F.; Bos, M.J.; Portegies, M.L.; Hofman, A.; Franco, O.H.; Koudstaal, P.J.; Ikram, M.A. The potential for prevention of dementia across two decades: The prospective, population-based Rotterdam Study. BMC Med. 2015, 13, 132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Qizilbash, N.; Gregson, J.; Johnson, M.E.; Pearce, N.; Douglas, I.; Wing, K.; Evans, S.J.W.; Pocock, S.J. BMI and risk of dementia in two million people over two decades: A retrospective cohort study. Lancet Diabetes Endocrinol. 2015, 3, 431–436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Rothman, K.J. BMI-related errors in the measurement of obesity. Int. J. Obes. 2005 2008, 32 (Suppl. S3), S56–S59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Hartanto, A.; Yong, J.C. Measurement matters: Higher waist-to-hip ratio but not body mass index is associated with deficits in executive functions and episodic memory. Peer J. 2018, 6, e5624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Cheke, L.G.; Simons, J.S.; Clayton, N.S. Higher body mass index is associated with episodic memory deficits in young adults. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2016, 69, 2305–2316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Kanoski, S.E.; Davidson, T.L. Western diet consumption and cognitive impairment: Links to hippocampal dysfunction and obesity. Physiol. Behav. 2011, 103, 59–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Sellbom, K.S.; Gunstad, J. Cognitive function and decline in obesity. J. Alzheimers Dis. JAD 2012, 30 (Suppl. S2), S89–S95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Dohle, S.; Diel, K.; Hofmann, W. Executive functions and the self-regulation of eating behavior: A review. Appetite 2018, 124, 4–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Wu, M.; Brockmeyer, T.; Hartmann, M.; Skunde, M.; Herzog, W.; Friederich, H.-C. Reward-related decision making in eating and weight disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis of the evidence from neuropsychological studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2016, 61, 177–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Erion, J.R.; Wosiski-Kuhn, M.; Dey, A.; Hao, S.; Davis, C.L.; Pollock, N.K.; Stranahan, A.M. Obesity elicits interleukin 1-mediated deficits in hippocampal synaptic plasticity. J. Neurosci. 2014, 34, 2618–2631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Raji, C.A.; Ho, A.J.; Parikshak, N.N.; Becker, J.T.; Lopez, O.L.; Kuller, L.H.; Hua, X.; Leow, A.D.; Toga, A.W.; Thompson, P.M. Brain structure and obesity. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2010, 31, 353–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Volkow, N.D.; Wang, G.-J.; Telang, F.; Fowler, J.S.; Goldstein, R.Z.; Alia-Klein, N.; Logan, J.; Wong, C.; Thanos, P.K.; Ma, Y.; et al. Inverse association between BMI and prefrontal metabolic activity in healthy adults. Obes. Silver Spring Md 2009, 17, 60–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Figley, C.R.; Asem, J.S.A.; Levenbaum, E.L.; Courtney, S.M. Effects of body mass index and body fat percent on default mode, executive control, and salience network structure and function. Front. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Deckers, K.; Van Boxtel, M.P.J.; Verhey, F.R.J.; Köhler, S. Obesity and cognitive decline in adults: Effect of methodological choices and confounding by age in a longitudinal study. J. Nutr. Health Aging 2017, 21, 546–553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Selzer, M.L.; Vinokur, A.; van Rooijen, L. A self-administered Short Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test (SMAST). J. Stud. Alcohol 1975, 36, 117–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Tun, P.A.; Lachman, M.E. Telephone assessment of cognitive function in adulthood: The Brief Test of Adult Cognition by Telephone. Age Ageing 2006, 35, 629–632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Hughes, M.L.; Agrigoroaei, S.; Jeon, M.; Bruzzese, M.; Lachman, M.E. Change in cognitive performance from midlife into old age: Findings from the Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) Study. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. JINS 2018, 24, 805–820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Lachman, M.E.; Tun, P.A. Cognitive testing in large-scale surveys: Assessment by telephone. In Handbook of Cognitive Aging: Interdisciplinary Perspectives; Sage Publications, Inc.: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2008; pp. 506–523. ISBN 978-1-4129-6028-1. [Google Scholar]
  30. Preston, S.H.; Fishman, E.; Stokes, A. Effects of categorization and self-report bias on estimates of the association between obesity and mortality. Ann. Epidemiol. 2015, 25, 907–911.e2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Asparouhov, T. Sampling weights in latent variable modeling. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 2005, 12, 411–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Enders, C.K.; Bandalos, D.L. The Relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 2001, 8, 430–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Brown, T.A. Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research, 2nd ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2015; ISBN 978-1-4625-1779-4. [Google Scholar]
  34. Hair, J.F.; Black, W.C.; Babin, B.J.; Anderson, R.E. Multivariate Data Analysis; Prentice Hall: Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  35. Biesanz, J.C. Autoregressive longitudinal models. In Handbook of Structural Equation Modeling; Hoyle, R.H., Ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 459–471. ISBN 978-1-60623-077-0. [Google Scholar]
  36. Cheung, G.W.; Rensvold, R.B. Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 2002, 9, 233–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Clouston, S.A.P.; Smith, D.M.; Mukherjee, S.; Zhang, Y.; Hou, W.; Link, B.G.; Richards, M. Education and cognitive decline: An integrative analysis of global longitudinal studies of cognitive aging. J. Gerontol. Ser. B. 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Fradkin, C.; Wallander, J.L.; Elliott, M.N.; Tortolero, S.; Cuccaro, P.; Schuster, M.A. Associations between socioeconomic status and obesity in diverse, young adolescents: Variation across race/ethnicity and gender. Health Psychol. Off. J. Div. Health Psychol. Am. Psychol. Assoc. 2015, 34, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Hartanto, A.; Toh, W.X.; Yang, H. Bilingualism narrows socioeconomic disparities in executive functions and self-regulatory behaviors during early childhood: Evidence from the early childhood longitudinal study. Child. Dev. 2019, 90, 1215–1235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Lenehan, M.E.; Summers, M.J.; Saunders, N.L.; Summers, J.J.; Vickers, J.C. Relationship between education and age-related cognitive decline: A review of recent research. Psychogeriatrics 2015, 15, 154–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Hartanto, A.; Lee, S.T.H.; Yong, J.C. Dispositional gratitude moderates the association between socioeconomic status and interleukin-6. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Cukierman, T.; Gerstein, H.C.; Williamson, J.D. Cognitive decline and dementia in diabetes--systematic overview of prospective observational studies. Diabetologia 2005, 48, 2460–2469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Kalmijn, S.; van Boxtel, M.P.J.; Verschuren, M.W.M.; Jolles, J.; Launer, L.J. Cigarette smoking and alcohol consumption in relation to cognitive performance in middle age. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2002, 156, 936–944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Little, T.D. Longitudinal Structural Equation Modeling; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2013; ISBN 978-1-4625-1016-0. [Google Scholar]
  45. Byrne, B.M.; Shavelson, R.J.; Muthén, B. Testing for the equivalence of factor covariance and mean structures: The issue of partial measurement invariance. Psychol. Bull. 1989, 105, 456–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Stevens, J.; McClain, J.E.; Truesdale, K.P. Selection of measures in epidemiologic studies of the consequences of obesity. Int. J. Obes. 2005 2008, 32 (Suppl. S3), S60–S66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Janssen, I.; Katzmarzyk, P.T.; Ross, R. Waist circumference and not body mass index explains obesity-related health risk. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2004, 79, 379–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Price, G.M.; Uauy, R.; Breeze, E.; Bulpitt, C.J.; Fletcher, A.E. Weight, shape, and mortality risk in older persons: Elevated waist-hip ratio, not high body mass index, is associated with a greater risk of death. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2006, 84, 449–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Zhu, S.; Heymsfield, S.B.; Toyoshima, H.; Wang, Z.; Pietrobelli, A.; Heshka, S. Race-ethnicity-specific waist circumference cutoffs for identifying cardiovascular disease risk factors. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2005, 81, 409–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Adabimohazab, R.; Garfinkel, A.; Milam, E.C.; Frosch, O.; Mangone, A.; Convit, A. Does inflammation mediate the association between obesity and insulin resistance? Inflammation 2016, 39, 994–1003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Lambert, E.A.; Straznicky, N.E.; Dixon, J.B.; Lambert, G.W. Should the sympathetic nervous system be a target to improve cardiometabolic risk in obesity? Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol. 2015, 309, H244–H258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  52. Miller, A.A.; Spencer, S.J. Obesity and neuroinflammation: A pathway to cognitive impairment. Brain. Behav. Immun. 2014, 42, 10–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Willette, A.A.; Kapogiannis, D. Does the brain shrink as the waist expands? Ageing Res. Rev. 2015, 20, 86–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Cortese, S.; Comencini, E.; Vincenzi, B.; Speranza, M.; Angriman, M. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and impairment in executive functions: A barrier to weight loss in individuals with obesity? BMC Psychiatry 2013, 13, 286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Kuo, H.K.; Jones, R.N.; Milberg, W.P.; Tennstedt, S.; Talbot, L.; Morris, J.N.; Lipsitz, L.A. Cognitive function in normal-weight, overweight, and obese older adults: An analysis of the Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly cohort. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2006, 54, 97–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Nilsson, L.-G.; Nilsson, E. Overweight and cognition. Scand. J. Psychol. 2009, 50, 660–667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Smith, E.; Hay, P.; Campbell, L.; Trollor, J.N. A review of the association between obesity and cognitive function across the lifespan: Implications for novel approaches to prevention and treatment. Obes. Rev. Off. J. Int. Assoc. Study Obes. 2011, 12, 740–755. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Institute of Medicine, Board on Health Sciences Policy, Committee on Assessing the Need for Clinical Trials of Testosterone Replacement Therapy. Testosterone and Aging: Clinical Research Directions; Liverman, C.T., Blazer, D.G., Eds.; National Academies Press (US): Washington, DC, USA, 2004; ISBN 978-0-309-09063-6. [Google Scholar]
  59. Holden, K.F.; Lindquist, K.; Tylavsky, F.A.; Rosano, C.; Harris, T.B.; Yaffe, K. Health ABC study Serum leptin level and cognition in the elderly: Findings from the Health ABC Study. Neurobiol. Aging 2009, 30, 1483–1489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Kamijo, K.; Khan, N.A.; Pontifex, M.B.; Scudder, M.R.; Drollette, E.S.; Raine, L.B.; Evans, E.M.; Castelli, D.M.; Hillman, C.H. The relation of adiposity to cognitive control and scholastic achievement in preadolescent children. Obes. Silver Spring Md 2012, 20, 2406–2411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Yeomans, M.R. Adverse effects of consuming high fat-sugar diets on cognition: Implications for understanding obesity. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 2017, 76, 455–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Cross−lagged panel models after controlling for age, sex, education attainment, and household income. For the purposes of clarity, factor indicators, autocorrelations among indicator residuals, and control variables are not shown, and only significant cross−lagged paths are displayed. The analyses were conducted while holding the optimal equality constraints (partial scalar invariance). The numbers are standardized coefficient estimates. * p < 0.05 ** p < 0.001.
Figure 1. Cross−lagged panel models after controlling for age, sex, education attainment, and household income. For the purposes of clarity, factor indicators, autocorrelations among indicator residuals, and control variables are not shown, and only significant cross−lagged paths are displayed. The analyses were conducted while holding the optimal equality constraints (partial scalar invariance). The numbers are standardized coefficient estimates. * p < 0.05 ** p < 0.001.
Nutrients 11 02343 g001
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of MIDUS II and MIDUS III samples.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of MIDUS II and MIDUS III samples.
MIDUS II (2004–2006)MIDUS III (2013–2017)
M (SD)RangeM (SD)Range
Mean Age (years)55.01 (11.17)33–8364.10 (11.17)42–92
Sex (% of Male)45.8% 45.8%
Education Attainment 7.58 (2.51)1–127.61 (2.53)1–12
Household Income (in $1000)76.18 (62.50)0–30087.69 (72.52)0–300
Smoking History (%)45.2% 43.9%
Alcohol Problem (%)3.8% 7.5%
Chronic Disease
Diabetes (%)7.6% 15.1%
Hypertension (%)25.8% 39.4%
Stroke (%)0.5% 1.4%
Obesity
Body Mass Index28.02 (5.47)18.56–50.6628.37 (5.85)18.51–52.52
Waist-to-Hip Ratio0.91 (0.11)0.50–1.489.24 (0.10)0.52–1.38
Cognitive Functions
Immediate Word List Recall6.99 (2.18)0–156.69 (2.36)0–15
Backward Digit Span5.09 (1.46)0–84.97 (1.47)0–8
Categorical Fluency19.73 (5.99)1–4218.81 (6.06)0–40
Delayed Word List Recall4.68 (2.49)0–144.37 (2.67)0–14
Number Series2.51 (1.50)0–52.34 (1.56)0–5
Backward Counting Task38.69 (11.22)−2–9036.33 (11.52)−10–90
Stop and Go Switch Task1.07 (0.23)0.61–3.771.27 (0.39)0.42–7.67
Note: Mean (M) and standard deviation (SD); SDs are shown in parentheses. Education attainment was rated on a scale of 1 (No school) to 12 (Ph.D, ED. D, MD, LLB, LLD, JD, or other professional degree). Alcohol abuse was measured with a five-item modified Michigan Alcohol Screening Test (Selzer et al., 1975) [26]. Descriptive statistics were presented before imputation.
Table 2. Measurement invariance fit indices in waist-to-hip ratio and body mass index (BMI) across two waves.
Table 2. Measurement invariance fit indices in waist-to-hip ratio and body mass index (BMI) across two waves.
Modelχ2dfCFITLIRMSEASRMR
Waist-to-hip Ratio
Configural427.547840.9770.9680.0390.039
Metric 443.910890.9770.9690.0390.040
Scalar895.328940.9470.9330.0570.059
Partial Scalar459.243930.9760.9690.0390.040
Unadjusted Cross-Lagged Panel 459.243930.9760.9690.0390.040
Cross-Lagged with Demographics and SES Covariates718.1161330.9680.9540.0410.037
Cross-Lagged Panel with Demographics, SES, and Health Covariates774.5191830.9680.9530.0350.031
Body Mass Index
Configural430.484840.9800.9710.0390.037
Metric 446.231890.9790.9720.0390.038
Scalar899.458940.9520.9390.0570.058
Partial Scalar462.120930.9780.9720.0390.039
Cross-Lagged 462.120930.9780.9720.0390.039
Cross-Lagged with Demographics and SES Covariates727.1381330.9690.9560.0410.036
Cross-Lagged Panel with Demographics, SES, and Health Covariates784.5581830.9700.9560.0350.030
Note: df = degree of freedom, CFI = comparative fit index, TLI = Tucker-Lewis index, RMSEA = root square error of approximation, SRMR = standardized root mean square residual.
Table 3. Standardized autoregressive and cross-lagged paths coefficients of obesity (waist-to-hip ratio and BMI) and the latent variables of executive function and episodic memory.
Table 3. Standardized autoregressive and cross-lagged paths coefficients of obesity (waist-to-hip ratio and BMI) and the latent variables of executive function and episodic memory.
Unadjusted Cross-Lagged Panel ModelCross-Lagged Panel with Demographic and SES CovariatesCross-Lagged Panel with Demographic, SES, and Health Covariates
Waist-to-Hip Ratio
Autoregressive Paths
WHRT1 → WHRT20.551 (0.023) **0.420 (0.030) **0.415 (0.030) **
EFT1 → EFT20.943 (0.011) **0.890 (0.020) **0.887 (0.020) **
EMT1 → EMT20.473 (0.024) **0.409 (0.026) **0.407 (0.026) **
Cross-Lagged Paths
WHRT1 → EFT2−0.024 (0.012) −0.013 (0.014)−0.010 (0.014)
WHRT1 → EMT2−0.125 (0.017) **−0.045 (0.019) *−0.044 (0.019) *
EFT1 → WHRT20.009 (0.025)−0.073 (0.034) **−0.067 (0.033) *
EFT1 → EMT20.217 (0.024) **0.179 (0.033) **0.176 (0.033) **
EMT1 → WHRT2−0.074 (0.023) *−0.002 (0.024)−0.002 (0.024)
EMT1 → EFT2−0.002 (0.016)−0.021 (0.017)−0.022 (0.017)
Body Mass Index
Autoregressive Paths
BMIT1 → BMIT20.857 (0.009) **0.850 (0.010) **0.844 (0.011) **
EFT1 → EFT20.942 (0.011) **0.891 (0.020) **0.888 (0.020) **
EMT1 → EMT20.506 (0.023) **0.410 (0.026) **0.409 (0.026) **
Cross−Lagged Paths
BMIT1 → EFT2−0.002 (0.012)−0.010 (0.012)−0.007 (0.012)
BMIT1 → EMT20.015 (0.019)0.007 (0.018)0.005 (0.019)
EFT1 → BMIT20.021 (0.016)−0.010 (0.022)−0.010 (0.023)
EFT1 → EMT20.206 (0.024) **0.183 (0.033) **0.181 (0.033) **
EMT1 → BMIT20.030 (0.015) *0.012 (0.016)0.011 (0.016)
EMT1 → EFT2−0.003 (0.016)−0.021 (0.017)−0.022 (0.017)
Note: Standardized errors shown in parentheses. EF = executive function, EM = episodic memory. p < 0.10 * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Table 4. Measurement invariance fit indices in waist-to-hip ratio and BMI across two waves with cognitive function as a single latent factor.
Table 4. Measurement invariance fit indices in waist-to-hip ratio and BMI across two waves with cognitive function as a single latent factor.
Modelχ2dfCFITLIRMSEASRMR
Waist-to-Hip Ratio
Configural604.84890.9660.9540.0470.055
Metric 624.14950.9650.9560.0460.057
Scalar1072.841010.9360.9240.0600.072
Partial Scalar642.401000.9640.9570.0450.057
Unadjusted Cross−Lagged Panel 642.401000.9640.9570.0450.057
Cross−Lagged with Demographics and SES Covariates1212.111480.9420.9260.0520.061
Cross−Lagged Panel with Demographics, SES, and Health Covariates1294.092080.9410.9250.0440.050
Body Mass Index
Configural463.25890.9780.9700.0400.038
Metric 482.93950.9770.9710.0390.039
Scalar937.411010.9510.9410.0560.059
Partial Scalar500.781000.9760.9720.0390.040
Cross−Lagged 500.781000.9760.9720.0390.040
Cross−Lagged with Demographics and SES Covariates1216.471480.9450.9300.0520.054
Cross−Lagged Panel with Demographics, SES, and Health Covariates1299.222080.9450.9300.0440.045
Note: df = degree of freedom, CFI = comparative fit index, TLI = Tucker−Lewis index, RMSEA = root square error of approximation, SRMR = standardized root mean square residual.
Table 5. Standardized autoregressive and cross-lagged paths coefficients of obesity (waist-to-hip ratio and BMI) and the latent variables of global cognitive function.
Table 5. Standardized autoregressive and cross-lagged paths coefficients of obesity (waist-to-hip ratio and BMI) and the latent variables of global cognitive function.
Unadjusted Cross-Lagged Panel ModelCross-Lagged Panel with Demographic and SES CovariatesCross-Lagged Panel with Demographic, SES, and Health Covariates
Waist-to-Hip Ratio
Autoregressive Paths
WHRT1 → WHRT20.563 (0.022) *0.420 (0.030) **0.415 (0.030) **
CFT1 → CFT20.933 (0.007) **0.860 (0.016) **0.856 (0.016) **
Cross−Lagged Paths
WHRT1 → CFT2−0.023 (0.012) −0.016 (0.014)−0.012 (0.014)
CFT1 → WHRT2−0.032 (0.025)−0.075 (0.030) *−0.068 (0.030) *
Body Mass Index
Autoregressive Paths
BMIT1 → BMIT20.857 (0.009) **0.849 (0.010) **0.844 (0.011) **
CFT1 → CFT20.935 (0.007) **0.862 (0.016) **0.858 (0.016) **
Cross−Lagged Paths
BMIT1 → CFT20.004 (0.012)−0.005 (0.012)−0.003 (0.012)
CFT1 → BMIT20.039 (0.014) *−0.002 (0.019)−0.002 (0.020)
Note: Standardized errors shown in parentheses. CF = cognitive function. p < 0.10 * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Hartanto, A.; Yong, J.C.; Toh, W.X. Bidirectional Associations between Obesity and Cognitive Function in Midlife Adults: A Longitudinal Study. Nutrients 2019, 11, 2343. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11102343

AMA Style

Hartanto A, Yong JC, Toh WX. Bidirectional Associations between Obesity and Cognitive Function in Midlife Adults: A Longitudinal Study. Nutrients. 2019; 11(10):2343. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11102343

Chicago/Turabian Style

Hartanto, Andree, Jose C. Yong, and Wei Xing Toh. 2019. "Bidirectional Associations between Obesity and Cognitive Function in Midlife Adults: A Longitudinal Study" Nutrients 11, no. 10: 2343. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11102343

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop