Next Article in Journal
The Effect of Incubator Cover on Newborn Vital Signs: The Design of Repeated Measurements in Two Separate Groups with No Control Group
Previous Article in Journal
Diagnosis and Management of Neonatal Hypoglycemia: A Comprehensive Review of Guidelines
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Spoken Expressive Vocabulary in 2-Year-Old Children with Hearing Loss: A Community Study

1
Murdoch Children’s Research Institute, Royal Children’s Hospital, Parkville, VIC 3052, Australia
2
Department of Audiology and Speech Pathology, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC 3010, Australia
3
Department of Paediatrics, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC 3010, Australia
4
School of Psychology and Public Health, La Trobe University, Bundoora, VIC 3086, Australia
5
Menzies Health Institute Queensland, Griffith University, Gold Coast, QLD 4222, Australia
6
Liggins Institute, University of Auckland, Auckland 1023, New Zealand
7
Centre for Community Child Health, Royal Children’s Hospital, Parkville, VIC 3052, Australia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Children 2023, 10(7), 1223; https://doi.org/10.3390/children10071223
Submission received: 31 May 2023 / Revised: 30 June 2023 / Accepted: 10 July 2023 / Published: 14 July 2023

Abstract

:
Through a cross-sectional community study of 2044 children aged 2 years, we (1) examine the impact of hearing loss on early spoken expressive vocabulary outcomes and (2) investigate how early intervention-related factors impact expressive vocabulary outcomes in children with hearing loss predominantly identified through universal newborn hearing screening. We used validated parent/caregiver-reported checklists from two longitudinal cohorts (302 children with unilateral or bilateral hearing loss, 1742 children without hearing loss) representing the same population in Victoria, Australia. The impact of hearing loss and amplification-related factors on vocabulary was estimated using g-computation and multivariable linear regression. Children with versus without hearing loss had poorer expressive vocabulary scores, with mean scores for bilateral loss 0.5 (mild loss) to 0.9 (profound loss) standard deviations lower and for unilateral loss marginally (0.1 to 0.3 standard deviations) lower. For children with hearing loss, early intervention and amplification by 3 months, rather than by 6 months or older, resulted in higher expressive vocabulary scores. Children with hearing loss demonstrated delayed spoken expressive vocabulary despite whole-state systems of early detection and intervention. Our findings align with calls to achieve a 1-2-3 month timeline for early hearing detection and intervention benchmarks for screening, identification, and intervention.

1. Introduction

Universal newborn hearing screening (UNHS) effectively reduces the median age of congenital hearing loss detection [1,2]. This enables earlier diagnosis, hearing amplification, and intervention for deaf and hard-of-hearing (DHH) children, with improved language outcomes reported at school age compared to those detected or amplified later [3,4]. However, these improved language outcomes remain poorer than normative expectations and population means [5]. Therefore, we should continue to seek modifiable factors that may improve early language outcomes for DHH children, over and above well-established early detection, to reduce this gap between children with and without hearing loss to the greatest extent possible.
There are relatively few reports of the spoken language outcomes of young DHH children. Early intervention (EI)-based studies show mixed results, either demonstrating children can achieve age-appropriate speech/language outcomes by three years of age [6] or age-appropriate skills at 12–18 months that fall below expectations by age 3 years [7,8]. A study aggregating 12 United States (US) states’ EI-based data on children with bilateral hearing loss found parent-reported expressive vocabulary scores were more than one standard deviation poorer than expected by age 2 years [9]. Significantly better scores were observed in children who met Early Hearing Detection and Intervention (EHDI) guidelines (screen by 1 month, diagnose by 3 months, enroll in EI by 6 months of age) compared to those who did not. Consistent with previous reports [10], better language was observed for children with milder losses, fewer comorbidities, and higher maternal education. However, this may represent a best-case scenario, as it included outcomes only from children actively engaged in EI services and already amplified, and the sample appeared to be more advantaged and to have fewer disabilities than the general DHH population. Thus, these findings may not reflect the broader population of children with any degree of hearing loss impacting one or two ears.
While some catch-up is possible [11], poorer early vocabulary is an important predictor of later language skills [12,13], often presaging persistent communication and educational difficulties. As EHDI continues to evolve, so do children’s outcomes [14]. To inform changes in early management, we need to understand what influences very early vocabulary outcomes in today’s DHH children. Research needs to include those who are not amplified, are not accessing EI, and/or are accessing different interventions, and include appropriate same-population comparisons.
Comparing two community cohorts of 2-year-old children with and without hearing loss derived from the same Australian state, this study aimed to (1) describe the impact on spoken expressive vocabulary of all degrees of unilateral and bilateral hearing loss and (2) describe intervention-related factors (hearing amplification device use, age at first hearing amplification, and age at first enrollment with EI services) for DHH children with unilateral and bilateral losses and quantify their impact on expressive vocabulary for DHH children with bilateral hearing loss.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design and Participants

This was a cross-sectional study of spoken expressive vocabulary in 2-year-old children from two longitudinal cohorts: DHH children from the Victorian Childhood Hearing Longitudinal Databank (VicCHILD) [15], and children without hearing loss from the Early Language in Victoria Study (ELVS) [16], both born or residing in the state of Victoria (population 6.7 million), Australia. Combining these two cohorts formed a single community sample enriched for hearing loss that represented expressive vocabulary skills across the full range of hearing. The studies were approved by the Ethics Committees of The Royal Children’s Hospital (VicCHILD and ELVS) and La Trobe University (ELVS), with parents/caregivers having provided written informed consent.

2.2. Participants with Permanent Hearing Loss

VicCHILD is a population-level data repository for over 1100 children with any degree or type of permanent hearing loss. All children identified with hearing loss through Victoria’s UNHS program (99% uptake, 1.8% loss to follow-up) are invited to participate, as are children who attend the Royal Children’s Hospital Caring for Hearing In Children Clinic for congenital or late-onset hearing loss. Most VicCHILD participants are under one year of age at enrollment. All participants also have access to government-supported hearing amplification and EI programs. Data are collected longitudinally with repeated measures as the child grows. Further information on VicCHILD’s methodology is detailed elsewhere [15]. This study included all VicCHILD participants born between 2013 and 2019, with hearing, demographic, early language, and service use data from the first two collection points (enrollment and age around 2 years).

2.3. Participants without Known Permanent Hearing Loss

ELVS has documented the speech and language development of a community sample of children without known permanent hearing loss, developmental delays, or serious disabilities when recruited in 2003-4 at ages 8–10 months [17]. Over 1900 children were recruited from six of Victoria’s 31 metropolitan local government areas, selected to represent children from different socio-economic backgrounds, and have been followed in successive waves. Demographic and spoken vocabulary outcomes data from participants at around 2 years of age were used, when the participant retention rate was 91.1% [16]. Further information on ELVS’s methodology is detailed elsewhere [17].

2.4. Outcome

The primary outcome was parent-/caregiver-reported spoken expressive vocabulary at around 2 years, collected in both cohorts using closely related measures. ELVS used the 680-word vocabulary checklist in the MacArthur Bates Communicative Development Inventory (MCDI) Words and Sentences test [18], whereas VicCHILD used the 100-word checklist in the Sure Start Language Measure (SSLM) [19], a validated measure based on the MCDI: UK Short Form [20]. Both the MCDI and SSLM have mean standard scores of 100 (standard deviation of 15) and, when compared, show high reliability and concurrent validity [19]. The study included DHH children aged 18–30 months at assessment whose parent/caregiver completed the SSLM and children without hearing loss aged 23.5–25.5 months whose parent/caregiver completed the MCDI.
The expressive vocabulary outcome was derived from standardized SSLM scores for both cohorts. For the ELVS cohort, the SSLM raw score was calculated for the 100 items in the MCDI common to the SSLM. For any combination of words, such as “sofa/couch” being a single item on the SSLM but two items on the MCDI, a single score was assigned in the SSLM raw score calculation if at least one of the words was selected on the MCDI. Based on this process, MCDI scores were converted to SSLM raw score equivalents, then to standardized scores based on age (in months) and sex.

2.5. Exposures

The exposure for aim 1 was hearing loss, defined as no hearing loss or a combination of the degree of loss (mild (21–40 dB)/moderate (41–60 dB)/severe (61–90 dB)/profound (>91 dB)) and presence in one/both ears (unilateral/bilateral). Degree of loss, calculated using three or four frequency averages, was obtained from UNHS records or parent/caregiver-supplied audiology results, classified using national decibel ranges for the affected ear (unilateral) or better hearing ear (bilateral) [21].
For aim 2, intervention-related factors were considered as separate exposures to explore the impact of intervening on single characteristics individually. Within the bilateral hearing loss cohort, exposures were hearing amplification status at survey completion (amplified vs. unamplified), frequency of hearing device use (never/rarely (no device or use <4 h/day), sometimes/often (4–8 h/day) or always (>8 h/day) derived from parent/caregiver-estimated hours of daily use at time of assessment), and age first enrolled into an EI program (≤3 months, 3.1–6 months, >6 months). Age at first hearing amplification fitting (≤3 months, 3.1–6 months, >6 months) was an exposure for children ever fitted with hearing amplification.

2.6. Potential Confounders

Directed acyclic graphs were developed to model assumptions about the causal structures (see Figure S1). For both aims, we identified possible demographic-related confounders as child sex, parent education level (completed at least undergraduate education: yes/no), primary language at home (English only: yes/no), and social disadvantage (measured using the Australian census-based Socio-Economic Indexes for Area (SEIFA), national mean 1000, SD 100; higher scores represent less disadvantage) [20]. Possible birth-related confounders were Neonatal Intensive Care Unit (NICU) admission and gestational age.

2.7. Statistical Analyses

All analyses were completed in statistical software R version 4.0.2 [22] and conducted for children with complete data for the respective aim only.

2.7.1. Aim 1

For aim 1, the samples of individuals with and without hearing loss were combined. We used a causal modeling approach to estimate the impact of hearing loss on expressive vocabulary at age 2 years, outlined as follows: Linear regression models were fitted to the data, modeling the outcome conditional on exposure, with three different models considered. Model 1 was an unadjusted model. Model 2 included demographic confounders as covariates, with Model 3 additionally including birth-related factors as covariates. The latter two models included interaction terms where appropriate (see Appendix A for further detail). Models 2 and 3 were fitted to consider different confounding adjustment sets and, additionally, the trade-off between potential bias from the exclusion of birth-related confounders (Model 2) and from sparse data in confounder substrata (Model 3). Estimates of the mean difference in standardized SSLM scores between hearing loss groups were obtained by standardizing (i.e., g-computation [23]) over the hearing loss sample to estimate the effect of hearing loss on SSLM score within the hearing loss population. Confidence intervals and standard errors were obtained via non-parametric bootstrapping. Estimates from Model 3 were interpreted as the primary analysis, which adjusted for demographic and birth-related potential confounders aiming to minimize confounding bias in causal effect estimates, with results under all models also provided. Further details on the statistical analysis methods can be found in Appendix A and Supplementary Figure S1.

2.7.2. Aim 2

For aim 2, hearing-related characteristics of individuals with any hearing loss were described. When estimating the impact of intervention-related factors on expressive vocabulary, we hypothesized the impact would be different for individuals with unilateral or bilateral loss. Also, considering the small sample size of individuals with unilateral hearing loss, we focused on the bilateral group when estimating this impact. Intervention-related factors were considered separate exposure variables and analyzed individually. Three regression models were again considered for each exposure, with the same covariates included as were used in aim 1 based on the same assumption around potential confounders and causal structures. Models 2 and 3 were also adjusted for bilateral hearing loss severity, as we considered this to be an influential factor in the expressive vocabulary of individuals. As with aim 1, Model 3 was interpreted as the primary analysis. For categorical variables with more than two levels, estimates of the mean difference in SSLM scores were calculated compared to a baseline level. We selected the baseline to reflect the “best case scenario”, i.e., earlier intervention, earlier age of detection.

3. Results

3.1. Sample Characteristics

The sample included 302 DHH children (75.5% of VicCHILD participants approached) and 1742 children from the ELVS cohort who were considered to not have permanent hearing loss (Figure 1). The mean age at assessment was similar between groups (25.4 months and 24.2 months, respectively), with more children without hearing loss being from an English-speaking-only household (Table 1). Children without hearing loss tended to be less disadvantaged than DHH peers (mean SEIFA index 1037.6 versus 1015.6), although both were above the national SEFIA mean of 1000. Of the DHH children, 209 (69.2%) had bilateral hearing loss, with a relatively equal distribution across degrees of loss.

3.2. Aim 1: Impact of Hearing Loss on Expressive Vocabulary

Estimates from Model 3 are presented in Table 2 as the primary analysis, along with estimates from Models 1 and 2. Adjusted mean expressive vocabulary scores were estimated to be lower for DHH children (of any severity and in either ear) compared to children with no hearing loss (Table 2). For bilateral hearing loss, the impact became more substantial as the degree increased. Compared to no hearing loss, adjusted mean expressive vocabulary scores ranged from 7.3 points (or 0.5 of a standard deviation) lower for mild bilateral loss (95% CI −11.4 to −2.5, p < 0.01) to 13.5 points (0.9 standard deviations) lower for profound bilateral loss (95% CI −18.5 to −8.4, p < 0.01). For unilateral hearing losses, the impact was less substantial, with adjusted mean scores ranging between 1.5 and 4.4 points lower (0.1 and 0.3 of a standard deviation, respectively) (Table 2).

3.3. Aim 2: Impact of Intervention-Related Factors on Expressive Vocabulary in Children with Hearing Loss

Estimates from Model 3 are again presented, with the distribution of hearing loss under each exposure group presented in Table S1. Two hundred and sixteen children (71.5% of 302 children from aim 1) used hearing amplification at survey completion, with 86.6% having bilateral hearing loss (187/216). Of the 22 children with unaided bilateral hearing loss, 81.8% had mild loss (Figure 2). For unilateral hearing loss, 31.2% were fitted with hearing amplification at the time of the survey (29/93) (Table 3). Unlike bilateral losses, there was no obvious relationship between the degree of unilateral loss and the distribution of intervention-related factors. For children with bilateral hearing loss, adjusted mean expressive vocabulary scores were lower if hearing amplification was used at time of assessment compared to scores of children without hearing amplification (adjusted mean difference 5.4 points, 95% CI −2.5 to 13.2 points, p = 0.18) (Table 4).
Reflective of a well-functioning UNHS environment, the median age at diagnosis was 1.0 month for children with any hearing loss. Most children received hearing amplification early (median age 3 months; Table 3), with fitting generally earlier if the degree of bilateral hearing loss was greater (Figure 2), whereas 46% of children with mild bilateral loss were fitted after age 6 months. Hearing amplification at 3 months or younger was associated with higher mean expressive vocabulary scores compared to first amplification when older than 3 months for individuals with bilateral hearing loss (adjusted mean difference of 8.6 points, 95% CI 3.1 to 14.1, p < 0.01 and 4.3 points, 95% CI −1.7 to 10.3, p = 0.16 for ages 3.1 to 6 months and >6 months, respectively; Table 4).
The relationship between hearing amplification use and degree of loss appeared stronger for children with bilateral loss versus unilateral loss; children rated as always wearing their hearing device tended to have greater degrees of bilateral loss compared to other use categories (Figure 2). Higher expressive vocabulary scores were estimated for children with bilateral hearing loss who never/rarely (rated to have <4 h daily average use or no hearing device fitted) wore hearing amplification, followed by those who always wore (>8 h daily average use), with those who sometimes/often (4–8 h daily average use) wore hearing amplification demonstrating the lowest expressive vocabulary scores (Table 4).
Most DHH children (with either unilateral or bilateral hearing loss) were enrolled in EI services early (median age 6 months) and were enrolled with a service at survey completion (Table 3). Children enrolled at older ages tended to have mild or moderate bilateral hearing loss (Figure 2). Mean SSLM scores for children with bilateral hearing loss were higher if enrolment was in the first 3 months of life compared to older ages (adjusted mean difference of 5.4 points, 95% CI −0.1 to 11.0, p = 0.05, and 10.0 points, 95% CI 4.2 to 15.7, p < 0.01 for enrolment at ages 3.1–6 months and >6 months, respectively, Table 4).

4. Discussion

4.1. Principal Findings

Despite Victoria’s sophisticated early identification and intervention systems reaching essentially all children, the spoken expressive vocabulary of 2-year-old DHH children lagged behind those without hearing loss. Spoken expressive vocabulary of children with bilateral losses was increasingly impacted as the degree of hearing loss increased, ranging from 0.5 to 0.9 standard deviations below expected levels after adjustment for potential demographic and birth-related confounders. Children with unilateral losses had an expressive vocabulary closer to (yet, on average, still poorer than) children without hearing loss, without a clear relationship between outcome and hearing loss severity.
Importantly, we demonstrated enrollment in EI by 3 months of age resulted in higher spoken expressive vocabulary scores. A similar pattern towards higher expressive vocabulary scores was also observed in the presence of earlier amplification. This may provide support for the narrower 1-2-3 month alternative timeline to the 1-3-6 EHDI indicators [24]. However, the association between hearing amplification use and language outcomes was not straightforward. We describe a U-shaped relationship where children rated as either “never/rarely” or “always” using hearing amplification showed greater expressive vocabulary scores than those children rated as “sometimes/often” using their hearing device(s).

4.2. Strengths of the Study

Our 96% UNHS-identified community cohort represents the common early diagnosis pathway occurring in countries with well-resourced EHDI systems. By including children with any degree of hearing loss in either ear and those with unamplified hearing losses, this enables a more generalizable estimate of expressive vocabulary performance than previous studies of early intervention cohorts (which excluded unilateral losses [9] or only included children with hearing amplification [9] or who had very low birthweight [25]). Despite sample sizes restricting the precision of estimates, our causal modeling approach is more flexible than other analysis approaches used elsewhere [9] (see Appendix A).

4.3. Limitations

Our statistical approach attempted to minimize potential confounding biases. When interpreting the results, we acknowledge the trade-off between sparse data in confounder substrata (under Model 3) and the potential for additional unmeasured confounding bias (under Model 2). Therefore, results are presented under all models to allow the reader to interpret the estimates while acknowledging these potential limitations. Unmeasured residual confounding remains possible, such as nonverbal IQ and differences between our hearing and hearing loss groups with respect to the inclusion of children with developmental delays. Limited by sample size, we analyzed exposures separately for aim 2. This meant we were unable to consider the potential combined effects of these exposures.
Our study was limited to examining spoken vocabulary as a measure of expressive language in DHH children, acknowledging that sign language is an important form of communication for DHH children. Among our VicCHILD participant children, less than 3% were reported to use Australian sign language (Auslan) as the predominant mode of communication. Moreover, there are no standardized or validated tools to measure sign language outcomes in preschool-aged children.
Children with hearing loss were drawn from our population-level data repository, with 60% of participant parents/caregivers reporting their child had no additional special health need or medical diagnosis. This is in keeping with other studies reporting that around 40% of DHH children have concomitant medical comorbidities; however, our study did not collect adequate information about medical comorbidities to determine whether this could be a factor influencing language outcomes.
For aim 2, we assumed the impact of intervention-related factors on expressive vocabulary would be different between individuals with unilateral or bilateral loss and therefore presented the estimated impact for bilateral losses only. A secondary analysis was conducted to estimate the impact for individuals with any permanent hearing loss—unilateral or bilateral. This secondary analysis indicated a similar relationship to that observed for bilateral hearing loss only, with attenuated effect sizes. However, this analysis assumed a constant causal effect across children with both lateralities of hearing loss, which we believe may not be reflective of the true behavior, and therefore have presented bilateral loss only within the main paper.
Our study design surveyed hearing amplification use at a single timepoint, which may miss capturing the variance in and accumulative effect of hearing amplification use over time [26]. All children with a permanent hearing loss who have Australian citizenship are eligible for hearing devices at no cost. We included children with no hearing device fitted in the never/rarely device use category for the aim 2 analysis. This approach may not be as relevant in populations with less generous and equitable access to hearing devices.
While born into the same geographic population, expressive vocabulary assessment occurred around a decade earlier for participants without known hearing loss compared to our DHH cohort. However, we believe our language performance comparisons remain valid since we do not expect typically developing children’s language outcomes to have changed greatly across time.

4.4. Interpretation in Light of Other Studies

Our findings support and extend findings from other studies by including children with unilateral loss and not limiting eligibility to those enrolled in EI programs. Like other existing studies of older children, we have demonstrated that children with even a mild degree of hearing loss have an expressive vocabulary below what is expected for their peers without hearing loss [27,28]. As also seen elsewhere [9,29], children with bilateral hearing loss demonstrated poorer expressive vocabulary as their degree of hearing loss increased. Slight delays for children with unilateral hearing loss align with reports for similar-age children with minimal hearing losses (unilateral and mild bilateral) [25,30].
We found no clear association between frequency of hearing amplification use and mean expressive vocabulary at age 2 years. The U-shaped performance curve where high vocabulary scores were found both for children reported to wear hearing amplification “never/rarely” and “always” warrants exploration. High hearing amplification use in children with high vocabulary scores may reflect the positive impact earlier detection through UNHS has on language outcomes [31] and the influence of hearing amplification use across time [29]. Alternatively, this group may represent a subgroup of children with hearing loss more likely to score highly in outcomes and adhere to hearing amplification use recommendations due to unmeasured factors. Reverse causation [32] cannot be ruled out for children with high vocabulary scores and low reported hearing amplification use, whereby parents/caregivers cease to enforce hearing aid use for children who are doing well and appear to be hearing without their amplification. This would also explain better expressive vocabulary scores in children with unamplified hearing loss at age 2 years.
There is some evidence that children without hearing loss with larger spoken expressive vocabularies at age 2 will, at age 5, show better performance in academic and behavioral outcomes compared to those with smaller spoken expressive vocabularies [33]. Early identification of spoken expressive vocabulary delays could personalize management, focusing on learning activities and experiences that help optimize skills for children with hearing loss across early childhood. Future research should explore underlying etiological or genetic markers that could predict language trajectories to direct resources to those DHH children in most need of intensive intervention.

5. Conclusions and Implications

This study provides population-level evidence of delayed spoken expressive vocabulary in DHH children at age 2 years, even with early detection through UNHS. We confirmed that the earlier the enrolment in EI programs and access to hearing amplification, the better the spoken expressive vocabulary outcomes. Specifically, our data supports the 1-2-3-month goal to screen, identify, and enter intervention rather than the original 1-3-6 EHDI benchmarks. While the extent of gains that could still be made is unknown, it is clear that interventions are not yet optimized for DHH children, as their early spoken expressive vocabulary outcomes are still poorer than their hearing peers. Moving forward, we must aim to more precisely understand the factors that impact language development in DHH children so that intervention can be targeted at those who need it most.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/children10071223/s1, Figure S1: Statistical approach with directed acyclic graphs; Table S1: Frequencies of hearing loss across exposures used for aim 2.

Author Contributions

P.C. conceptualized and designed the study, contributed to data interpretation, and drafted and revised the manuscript; he takes overall responsibility for all aspects of the study; D.A.S. conceptualized and designed the study, was responsible for the analysis and interpretation of the data, drafted and revised the manuscript; L.S. was responsible for acquisition of the data and reviewed and revised the manuscript; T.H. provided guidance regarding data collection instruments and reviewed and revised the manuscript; M.L. assisted with designing the study, provided guidance regarding data collection instruments, and reviewed and revised the manuscript; E.L.B. and S.R. established ELVS, contributed to data analysis and reviewed and revised the manuscript; M.W. established VicCHILD and ELVS, conceptualized and designed the study, and reviewed and revised the manuscript; V.S. conceptualized and designed the study, provided guidance regarding the data collection instruments and reviewed and revised the manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

D.A.S. was supported by The Lorenzo and Pamela Galli Medical Research Trust. V.S. was supported by the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) Early Career Fellowship 1125687, a Melbourne Children’s Clinician Scientist Fellowship 2021, and a L’Oréal-UNESCO Australian and New Zealand For Women in Science Fellowship 2019. M.W. was supported by the NHMRC Principal Research Fellowship 1160906, and S.R. was supported by the NHMRC Practitioner Fellowship 491210. Research at the Murdoch Children’s Research Institute is supported by the Victorian Government’s Operational Infrastructure Support Program. The funding organizations are independent of all researchers. The VicCHILD project received funding from the Royal Children’s Hospital Foundation [2018–999] and [2014–430]; the Murdoch Children’s Research Institute; the Phyllis Connor Memorial Trust; the Deafness Foundation; the National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) Centre of Research Excellence Grant [1023493]; the Kyle Patrick Lamsam Convery Foundation; the Nelson Alexander Charitable Foundation; and a Royal Australasian College of Physicians Cottrell Research Establishment Fellowship.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. VicCHILD was approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) of The Royal Children’s Hospital, Melbourne, Australia (HREC number: 31081, approved 17 August 2011, most recently approved 18 January 2023), and ELVS was approved by the Ethics Committees of The Royal Children’s Hospital, Melbourne, Australia (HREC number: 27078/33195, approved 10 September 2021, most recently approved 4 April 2023) and the La Trobe University (03-32), approved 25 February 2003.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study. Written informed consent has been obtained from the participants to publish this paper.

Data Availability Statement

All available data supporting the reported results can be found within this publication and its Supplementary Files. Data are not publicly available because not all VicCHILD and ELVS participants have provided consent for data sharing, and data sharing is limited to ethically approved research.

Acknowledgments

We thank all the families and children for participating in VicCHILD and ELVS, the VicCHILD research team past and present members (Christina Barber, Nicole Stefanac, Rachele Aiello, Jane Bridie, Georgie Columbus, Alisha Gulenc, and Anne Hampton) for their help in collecting data, and the ELVS investigator team for allowing access to de-identified data from their cohort.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Supplementary Methods: Statistical Analyses

For aims 1 and 2, three models were considered. Model 2 adjusted for a reduced confounder set, while model 3 adjusted for an additional two possible confounder variables. When interpreting the results, we acknowledge the trade-off between sparse data in confounder substrata under model 3 and the potential for additional unmeasured confounding bias under model 2. Therefore, results are presented under both models to allow the reader to interpret the estimates while acknowledging these potential limitations.
Within the combined cohort used for aim 1, the majority of the sample pertained to individuals without hearing loss, which therefore drove the fitted linear regression model. Ideally, additional interaction terms would have been included in the fitted model (i.e., including all exposure-confounder interactions) to better address the differing confounder distribution between those with hearing loss and those without. However, we limited this to two confounders only due to potential issues with sparse data within some of the exposure groups. In the fitted regression model for aim 1, interaction terms were included between exposure (hearing loss) and two confounders (sex and parent education), determined a priori based on content knowledge. G-computation was used to estimate the causal effect of interest, relaxing the strict assumption of a constant causal effect across confounder substrata under the traditional regression approach (used in previous studies). For aim 2, no interaction terms were included in the model due to the limited sample size under a complete case analysis.

References

  1. Sininger, Y.S.; Martinez, A.; Eisenberg, L.; Christensen, E.; Grimes, A.; Hu, J. Newborn hearing screening speeds diagnosis and access to intervention by 20–25 months. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 2009, 20, 49–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Wood, S.A.; Sutton, G.J.; Davis, A.C. Performance and characteristics of the Newborn Hearing Screening Programme in England: The first seven years. Int. J. Audiol. 2015, 54, 353–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Kennedy, C.R.; McCann, D.C.; Campbell, M.J.; Law, C.M.; Mullee, M.; Petrou, S.; Watkin, P.; Worsfold, S.; Yuen, H.M.; Stevenson, J. Language ability after early detection of permanent childhood hearing impairment. N. Engl. J. Med. 2006, 354, 2131–2141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Ching, T.Y.C.; Dillon, H.; Leigh, G.; Cupples, L. Learning from the Longitudinal Outcomes of Children with Hearing Impairment (LOCHI) study: Summary of 5-year findings and implications. Int. J. Audiol. 2018, 57, S105–S111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Wake, M.; Ching, T.; Wirth, K.; Poulakis, Z.; Mensah, F.K.; Gold, L.; King, A.; Bryson, H.; Reilly, S.; Rickards, F. Population Outcomes of Three Approaches to Detection of Congenital Hearing Loss. Pediatrics 2016, 137, e20151722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Fulcher, A.; Purcell, A.A.; Baker, E.; Munro, N. Listen up: Children with early identified hearing loss achieve age-appropriate speech/language outcomes by 3 years-of-age. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 2012, 76, 1785–1794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Stika, C.J.; Eisenberg, L.S.; Johnson, K.C.; Henning, S.C.; Colson, B.G.; Ganguly, D.H.; DesJardin, J.L. Developmental outcomes of early-identified children who are hard of hearing at 12 to 18 months of age. Early Hum. Dev. 2015, 91, 47–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Stika, C.J.; Eisenberg, L.S.; Carter, A.S.; Johnson, K.C.; Ganguly, D.M.H.; Henning, S.C.; DesJardin, J.L. Developmental Outcomes in Early-Identified Children Who Are Hard of Hearing at 2 to 3 Years of Age. Ear. Hear 2021. publish ahead of print. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Yoshinaga-Itano, C.; Sedey, A.L.; Wiggin, M.; Chung, W. Early Hearing Detection and Vocabulary of Children with Hearing Loss. Pediatrics 2017, 140, e20162964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Ching, T.Y.C. Is Early Intervention Effective in Improving Spoken Language Outcomes of Children with Congenital Hearing Loss? Am. J. Audiol. 2015, 24, 345–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Whitehouse, A.J.O.; Robinson, M.; Zubrick, S.R. Late Talking and the Risk for Psychosocial Problems during Childhood and Adolescence. Pediatrics 2011, 128, e324–e332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Lee, J. Size matters: Early vocabulary as a predictor of language and literacy competence. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2011, 32, 69–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Can, D.D.; Ginsburg-Block, M.; Golinkoff, R.M.; Hirsh-Pasek, K. A long-term predictive validity study: Can the CDI Short Form be used to predict language and early literacy skills four years later? J. Child Lang. 2013, 40, 821–835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Carew, P.; Mensah, F.K.; Rance, G.; Flynn, T.; Poulakis, Z.; Wake, M. Mild-moderate congenital hearing loss: Secular trends in outcomes across four systems of detection. Child Care Health Dev. 2017, 44, 71–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Sung, V.; Smith, L.; Poulakis, Z.; A Burt, R.; Carew, P.; Tobin, S.; Wake, M. Data Resource Profile: The Victorian Childhood Hearing Impairment Longitudinal Databank (VicCHILD). Int. J. Epidemiol. 2019, 48, 1409–1410h. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Reilly, S.; Bavin, E.L.; Bretherton, L.; Conway, L.; Eadie, P.; Cini, E.; Prior, M.; Ukoumunne, O.C.; Wake, M. The Early Language in Victoria Study (ELVS): A prospective, longitudinal study of communication skills and expressive vocabulary development at 8, 12 and 24 months. Int. J. Speech Lang Pathol. 2009, 11, 344–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Reilly, S.; Cook, F.; Bavin, E.L.; Bretherton, L.; Cahir, P.; Eadie, P.; Gold, L.; Mensah, F.; Papadopoullos, S.; Wake, M. Cohort Profile: The Early Language in Victoria Study (ELVS). Int. J. Epidemiol. 2017, 47, 11–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Fenson, L.; Marchman, V.A.; Thal, D.J.; Dale, P.S.; Reznick, J.S.; Bates, E. The MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories User’s Guide and Technical Manual, 2nd ed.; Brookes Publishing: Baltimore, MA, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  19. Roy, P.; Kersley, H.; Law, J. The Sure Start Language Measure Standardisation Study; City University: Nottingham, UK, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  20. Dale, P.S.; Dionne, G.; Eley, T.C.; Plomin, R. Lexical and grammatical development: A behavioural genetic perspective. J. Child Lang. 2000, 27, 619–642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Rose, E.; Sung, V. Otolaryngology. In Paediatric Handbook, 10th ed.; Harding, K., Mason, D.S., Efron, D., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2022; Available online: http://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/unimelb/detail.action?docID=6375231 (accessed on 3 August 2022).
  22. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. 2017. Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 9 July 2023).
  23. Hernán, M.; Robins, J. Causal Inference: What If; Chapman & Hall/CRC: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  24. The Joint Committee on Infant Hearing. Year 2019 Position Statement: Principles and Guidelines for Early Hearing Detection and Intervention Programs. J. Early Hear. Detect. Interv. 2019, 4, 1–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Vohr, B.; Jodoin-Krauzyk, J.; Tucker, R.; Topol, D.; Johnson, M.J.; Ahlgren, M.; Pierre, L. Expressive vocabulary of children with hearing loss in the first 2 years of life: Impact of early intervention. J. Perinatol. 2011, 31, 274–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. McCreery, R.W.; Walker, E.A.; Spratford, M. Understanding Limited Use of Amplification in Infants and Children Who Are Hard of Hearing. Perspect Hear Hear Disord Child. 2015, 25, 15–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Halliday, L.F.; Tuomainen, O.; Rosen, S. Language Development and Impairment in Children with Mild to Moderate Sensorineural Hearing Loss. J. Speech Lang Hear. Res. 2017, 60, 1551–1567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Edquist, G.; Flynn, T.; Jennische, M. Expressive vocabulary of school-age children with mild to moderately severe hearing loss. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 2022, 162, 111281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Tomblin, J.B.; Harrison, M.; Ambrose, S.E.; Walker, E.A.; Oleson, J.J.; Moeller, M.P. Language Outcomes in Young Children with Mild to Severe Hearing Loss. Ear Hear. 2015, 36, 76S–91S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  30. Fitzpatrick, E.M.; Durieux-Smith, A.; Gaboury, I.; Coyle, D.; Whittingham, J. Communication Development in Early- Identified Children with Mild Bilateral and Unilateral Hearing Loss. Am. J. Audiol. 2015, 24, 349–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Yoshinaga-Itano, C.; Manchaiah, V.; Hunnicutt, C. Outcomes of Universal Newborn Screening Programs: Systematic Review. J. Clin. Med. 2021, 10, 2784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Martin, R.M. Epidemiological study designs for health care research and evaluation. In Handbook of Health Research Methods: Investigation, Measurement and Analysis, 1st ed.; Bowling, A., Ebrahim, S., Eds.; Open University Press: London, UK, 2005; pp. 98–163. [Google Scholar]
  33. Morgan, P.; Farkas, G.; Hillemeier, M.; Scheffner Hammer, C.; Maczuga, S. 24-Month-Old Children with Larger Oral Vocabularies Display Greater Academic and Behavioral Functioning at Kindergarten Entry. Child Dev. 2015, 86, 1351–1370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Participant flowchart.
Figure 1. Participant flowchart.
Children 10 01223 g001
Figure 2. Hearing amplification and intervention characteristics of 2-year-old children broken down by unilateral and bilateral hearing loss. Percentages are calculated as the proportion of the degree of loss (mild to profound) within each x-axis group.
Figure 2. Hearing amplification and intervention characteristics of 2-year-old children broken down by unilateral and bilateral hearing loss. Percentages are calculated as the proportion of the degree of loss (mild to profound) within each x-axis group.
Children 10 01223 g002
Table 1. Characteristics of the hearing loss and without hearing loss groups for aim 1.
Table 1. Characteristics of the hearing loss and without hearing loss groups for aim 1.
Hearing Loss Group
(n = 302)
No Hearing Loss Group
(n = 1742)
Missing a
n (%)
Missing a
n (%)
Demographicinformation
Age at assessment, months—mean (SD)0 (0)25.4 (1.7)0 (0)24.2 (0.4)
Sex: male—n (%) 0 (0)170 (56.3)0 (0)887 (50.9)
Parent’s education—n (%)11 (3.6) 23 (1.3)
Year 10 or less 21 (7.2) 150 (8.7)
Year 11 13 (4.5) 234 (13.6)
Year 12 83 (28.5) 692 (40.3)
Tertiary or Postgraduate 174 (59.8) 643 (37.4)
Primary language at home—n (%)12 (4.0) 8 (0.5)
English only 166 (57.2) 1644 (94.8)
Bilingual (English and other language) 89 (30.7) 18 (1.0)
Other language only 35 (12.1) 72 (4.2)
Disadvantage Index, SEIFA—mean (SD) 1 (0.3)1015.6 (61.1)1 (0.1)1037.6 (59.6)
Birth-related factors
NICU admission: yes—n (%)7 (2.3)50 (17.0)0 (0)284 (16.3)
Gestational age, weeks—mean (SD)1 (0.3)38.3 (2.6)152 (8.7)39.3 (1.8)
Hearing status—n (%)0 (0) 0 (0)
Normal hearing 1742 (100.0)
Unilateral hearing loss 93 (30.8)
Mild 12 (4.0)
Moderate 22 (7.3)
Severe 23 (7.6)
Profound 36 (11.9)
Bilateral hearing loss 209 (69.2)
Mild 57 (18.9)
Moderate 73 (24.2)
Severe 38 (12.6)
Profound 41 (13.6)
Outcome measures
SSLM score, standardized—mean (SD)0 (0)88.5 (16.8)0 (0)98.7 (13.4)
Combining words yet—n (%)24 (8.0) 30 (1.7)
Not yet 81 (29.1) 290 (16.9)
Sometimes 82 (29.5) 573 (33.5)
Always 115 (41.4) 849 (49.6)
a In the presence of missing data, the presented statistics are relative to records with available information for characteristics of interest.
Table 2. Estimates of the mean difference in standardized SSLM score within the hearing loss sample if all children had no hearing loss compared to if all children had hearing loss (broken down by severity of loss) under the three different models fitted.
Table 2. Estimates of the mean difference in standardized SSLM score within the hearing loss sample if all children had no hearing loss compared to if all children had hearing loss (broken down by severity of loss) under the three different models fitted.
UnadjustedAdjusted aAdjusted b
Mean Difference95% CIp-ValueAdj. Mean Difference95% CIp-ValueAdj. Mean Difference95% CIp-ValueAdj. Mean b
Without hearing loss (ref)96.1
Hearing loss: Unilateral
Mild−3.19(−11.08, 4.69)0.43−3.43(−24.27, 12.92)0.73−1.46(−21.66, 13.46)0.8894.7
Moderate−8.19(−14.56, −8.06)<0.01−4.12(−11.75, 3.29)0.28−3.93(−11.63, 3.47)0.3192.2
Severe−4.35(−10.06, 1.37)0.14−2.17(−9.57, 5.51)0.57−2.22(−9.63, 5.24)0.5693.9
Profound−5.97(−10.56, −1.39)0.01−4.50(−10.42, 0.62)0.11−4.37(−10.26, 0.71)0.1391.8
Hearing loss: Bilateral
Mild−9.01(−12.68, −5.34)<0.01−7.65(−11.76, −2.90)<0.01−7.31(−11.38, −2.48)<0.0188.8
Moderate−11.31(−14.56, −8.06)<0.01−9.23(−13.25, −4.76)<0.01−8.36(−12.39, −4.20)<0.0187.8
Severe−15.98(−20.45, −11.52)<0.01−13.51(−17.79, −9.32)<0.01−13.35(−17.78, −9.13)<0.0182.8
Profound−14.77(−19.07, −10.46)<0.01−13.91(−18.86, −8.80)<0.01−13.54(−18.53, −8.37)<0.0182.6
a Adjusted for the child’s sex, primary language at home, parent education, and SEIFA; b Further adjusted for NICU admission and gestational age.
Table 3. Hearing-related characteristics of children with hearing loss.
Table 3. Hearing-related characteristics of children with hearing loss.
Missing Information a
n (%)
Hearing Loss Group
(N = 302)
Unilateral Loss
(N = 93)
Bilateral Loss
(N = 209)
Age at diagnosis (months)61 (30.2)
median (IQR) 1.0 (0.6, 1.6)1.0 (0.7, 1.6)0.8 (0.6, 1.4)
UNHS detected—n (%)0 (0)290 (96.0)91 (97.8)199 (95.2)
Any hearing device used ever—n (%)0 (0)231 (76.5)41 (44.1)190 (90.9)
Any hearing device used at survey—n (%)0 (0)216 (71.5)29 (31.2)187 (89.5)
Hearing aid only 152 (50.3)27 (29.0)125 (59.8)
Cochlear implant only 41 (13.6)2 (2.2)39 (18.7)
Hearing aid and cochlear implant 23 (7.6)0 (0)23 (11.0)
None 86 (28.5)64 (68.8)22 (10.5)
Age first hearing device fitted, continuous (months)75 (24.8)
median (IQR) 3 (2, 6.5)6 (4, 12)3 (2, 6)
Age first hearing device fitted, categorical—n (%)75 (24.8)
≤3 months 114 (50.2)36 (45.0)78 (53.6)
3.1–6 months 56 (24.7)23 (28.8)33 (22.5)
>6 months 57 (25.1)21 (26.3)36 (24.5)
Average hearing device use: week day—n (%)8 (2.6)
Never/rarely, <4 h 115 (39.1)71 (15.4)44 (21.9)
Sometimes/often, 4–8 h 62 (21.1)14 (78.0)48 (23.9)
Always, >8 h 117 (39.8)6 (6.6)111 (55.2)
EI service used ever—n (%)2 (0.7)225 (75.0)39 (41.9)186 (89.9)
EI service used at survey—n (%)78 (25.8)209 (93.3)29 (78.4)180 (96.3)
Age EI program enrolment (months)94 (31.1)
median (IQR) 6 (4, 10)8 (5, 12)6 (3, 9)
Age EI program enrolment—n (%)94 (31.1)
≤3 months 51 (24.5)4 (13.8)47 (26.3)
3.1–6 months 74 (35.6)9 (31.0)65 (36.3)
>6 months 83 (39.9)16 (55.2)67 (37.4)
a In the presence of missing data, presented values are relative to records with available information for characteristics of interest.
Table 4. Estimates of the mean difference in standardized SSLM score (for individuals with bilateral hearing loss) between intervention-related exposures: (a) hearing device used at survey; (b) average hearing device use; (c) average age of hearing device fitted; and (d) age at EI nrolment. Estimates were obtained under three different models.
Table 4. Estimates of the mean difference in standardized SSLM score (for individuals with bilateral hearing loss) between intervention-related exposures: (a) hearing device used at survey; (b) average hearing device use; (c) average age of hearing device fitted; and (d) age at EI nrolment. Estimates were obtained under three different models.
Unadjusted Adjusted aAdjusted b
Mean Difference95% CIp-ValueAdj. Mean Difference95% CIp-ValueAdj. Mean Difference95% CIp-Valuen *Adj. Mean b
Hearing device used at survey 199
No (ref) 90.6
Yes−2.81(−10.16, 4.54)0.45−4.23(−12.16, 3.71)0.30−5.36(−13.21, 2.50)0.18 85.2
Age hearing device fitted 177
<3 months (ref) 88.0
3.1–6 months−10.49(−16.11, −4.87)<0.01−9.82(−15.16, −4.47)<0.01−8.59(−14.10, −3.08)<0.01 79.4
>6 months−3.79(−9.52, 1.93)0.19−5.66(−11.39, 0.08)0.05−4.31(−10.30, 1.69)0.16 83.6
Average hearing device use 193
Always (ref) 86.1
Sometimes/often−5.75(−11.32, −0.18)0.04−5.06(−10.31, 0.20)0.06−4.62(−9.80, 0.57)0.08 81.4
Never/rarely2.55(−3.20, 8.30)0.383.27(−3.07, 9.61)0.313.39(−2.86, 9.63)0.29 89.4
Age EI program enrolment 171
<3 months (ref) 91.3
3.1–6 months−2.99(−8.88, 2.90)0.32−5.60(−11.11, −0.10)0.05−5.44(−10.95, 0.07)0.05 85.8
>6 months−8.92(−14.78, −3.06)<0.01−10.59(−16.28, −4.91)<0.01−9.95(−15.68, −4.22)<0.01 81.3
a Adjusted for the child’s sex, primary language at home, parent education, SEIFA, severity of hearing loss; b Further adjusted for NICU admission and gestational age; * Number of records with complete information used for complete case analysis.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Carew, P.; Shepherd, D.A.; Smith, L.; Howell, T.; Lin, M.; Bavin, E.L.; Reilly, S.; Wake, M.; Sung, V. Spoken Expressive Vocabulary in 2-Year-Old Children with Hearing Loss: A Community Study. Children 2023, 10, 1223. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10071223

AMA Style

Carew P, Shepherd DA, Smith L, Howell T, Lin M, Bavin EL, Reilly S, Wake M, Sung V. Spoken Expressive Vocabulary in 2-Year-Old Children with Hearing Loss: A Community Study. Children. 2023; 10(7):1223. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10071223

Chicago/Turabian Style

Carew, Peter, Daisy A. Shepherd, Libby Smith, Tegan Howell, Michelle Lin, Edith L. Bavin, Sheena Reilly, Melissa Wake, and Valerie Sung. 2023. "Spoken Expressive Vocabulary in 2-Year-Old Children with Hearing Loss: A Community Study" Children 10, no. 7: 1223. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10071223

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop