Next Article in Journal
Short-Term Variability and Predictors of Urinary Pentachlorophenol Levels in Ohio Preschool Children
Previous Article in Journal
A Method for Screening Climate Change-Sensitive Infectious Diseases
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Neighborhood Environment Perceptions and the Likelihood of Smoking and Alcohol Use

by
Nattinee Jitnarin
1,*,
Katie M. Heinrich
2,
Christopher K. Haddock
1,
Joseph Hughey
3,
LaVerne Berkel
4 and
Walker S.C. Poston
1
1
Institute for Biobehavioral Health Research, National Development and Research Institutes, Inc., 1920 143rd Street, Suite 120, Leawood, KS 66224, USA
2
Department of Kinesiology, Kansas State University, Manhattan, KS 66506, USA
3
Departments of Psychology and Architecture and Urban Planning, University of Missouri-Kansas City, Kansas City, MO 64110, USA
4
School of Education, University of Missouri-Kansas City, 615 E. 52nd Street, Kansas City, MO 64110, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12(1), 784-799; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph120100784
Submission received: 31 October 2014 / Accepted: 9 January 2015 / Published: 14 January 2015

Abstract

:
Neighborhood characteristics are important correlates for a variety of health outcomes. Among several health risk behaviors, smoking and alcohol use have significant consequences. Perceptions of neighborhood problems are associated with depressive symptoms, lower physical activity, and lower quality of life. However, it is unclear which perceived aspects of neighborhoods might be related to smoking and drinking. We examined whether perceived neighborhood characteristics were associated with smoking and drinking patterns using data from US metropolitan Midwestern area adults. Participants completed surveys including sociodemographic characteristics, neighborhood perceptions, behavioral and psychological health. For men, negative perceptions of neighborhood infrastructures were significant predictors for smoking and binge drinking. Among women, no perceived environmental factors were associated with smoking or drinking. However, education was a significant negative predictor for smoking. As age increased, the likelihood of using cigarettes, heavy and binge drinking in women decreased significantly. Depression was a positive predictor for smoking and heavy drinking in men and women, respectively. These findings indicate that the perceived neighborhood infrastructure was predictive of health behaviors among men, even after adjusting for key confounders. Closer attention may need to be paid to the role of neighborhood environmental characteristics along with individual-level characteristics in influencing unhealthy behaviors.

1. Introduction

Neighborhood characteristics are important correlates for a variety of health outcomes and health inequalities [1,2,3]. For example, neighborhood socioeconomic status (SES) often has been associated with residents’ health behaviors and outcomes including physical activity, obesity, and mental health [3,4,5,6,7,8]. Although it is not clear how neighborhood SES influences health status, one proposed mechanism is that low SES affects the environments of individuals by creating settings that promote unhealthy behaviors [4,9].
Smoking and alcohol use are important health behaviors that have significant consequences. According to the recent reports from the World Health Organization [10,11], alcohol and tobacco use are the third and sixth leading global risk factors for disease and disability, respectively. Although numerous studies indicate that individual characteristics such as education, employment and/or occupational status are associated with smoking and drinking [12,13], there is growing evidence demonstrating associations between neighborhood characteristics and smoking and drinking behaviors. [5,6,7,8,14]. For example, people living in areas of high neighborhood deprivation were more likely to smoke and use alcohol compared with those living in a neighborhood with moderate deprivation [14].
Several factors may explain why neighborhood characteristics influence smoking and drinking prevalence. Substandard or distressed physical features of neighborhoods (e.g., poor quality housing, litter, damage, and other incivilities) and inadequate basic resources including stores, public transportation, hospitals, and recreational facilities, are considered as important determinants of smoking and drinking behaviors [15]. In addition, social norms, psychosocial distress, exposure to tobacco and alcohol advertising and their outlets have been proposed as factors at the neighborhood level that may influence tobacco and alcohol use [14,16,17,18]. However, few studies have explored the physical environments of the neighborhoods (e.g., quality, facilities, problems, and walkability) or residents’ perceptions of the physical environment as influences on smoking and alcohol use [4,19].
Despite increasing interest in the perceived physical environment of neighborhoods, most studies tend to focus on its relationships with mental health indicators [4,6,20,21] and physical activity [5,8,22,23]. It has been demonstrated that perceptions of neighborhood problems (e.g., poor quality of facilities, less safety) are associated with lower quality of life, greater depressive symptoms and lower physical activity [24]. However, it is unclear which perceived aspects of neighborhoods might be related to smoking and drinking behaviors. The current study examined the relationships between neighborhood environment perceptions and the likelihood of tobacco or alcohol use and binge drinking using individual data on neighborhood perceptions collected from a large sample of adults residing in randomly selected Census block groups in a Midwestern metropolitan area.

2. Methods

2.1. Sample and Setting

The current study was part of the KC BEST study, a cross-sectional population-based survey on the built environment and health that was conducted in a large Midwestern metropolitan area [25]. The primary aim of KCBEST was to examine how neighborhood environments influenced obesity risk.
In the KCBEST study, 21 census block-groups were randomly selected within strata based on median yearly family household income and the proportion of minorities in the block groups according to the 2010 U.S. Census. Family household income within a census block group was divided into tertiles (from the population distribution of the household income variable); low, middle, and high income to ensure maximum income differences for the census block groups. Income tertiles were defined as $4999.00 to $23,386.33 for low income; $23,386.34 to $35,569.00 for middle income; and $35,569.01 to $150,001.00 for high income. Seven census blocks were randomly selected from each group. We sampled census blocks with low and high minority representation with a minimum of 19% ethnic minority residents to ensure adequate diversity (i.e., so that no areas that were composed of only one ethnic/racial group were selected).
To collect individual-level data, households within the selected block groups were solicited until the target number of interviews were reached. Door-to door household interviews were completed with at least 25 household per block group, for a total of 586 participants (30.4% of 1928 adults contacted and eligible). Individuals were eligible to participate if they were between 18 and 74 years of age, had lived in the area at least 12 months, were able to read and understand surveys in English, and were primarily responsible for making food decisions for the household. Pregnant women and individuals who currently had any chronic health conditions or a disability that prevented them from participating in physical activity were excluded from participation. Prior to the interview, eligible participants were informed by a letter that outlined the purpose of the study and requested cooperation. Letters in advance of a household interview have been found to significantly decrease interview refusals [26].
Trained interviewers collected data during 60-min face-to-face interviews. Interviewers were instructed to speak to the adult responsible for food decisions for the household. The reason for asking for the person who made food decisions for the household was because the main study (KC BEST) focused on food preparation and selection. The study was approved by the relevant institutional IRBs and informed consent was obtained from all eligible participants.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Primary Variables of Interest

Perceived Neighborhood Characteristics. The Environmental Module of the International Physical Activity Prevalence Study questionnaires (IPAQ E-module) [27,28] was used to assess perceived neighborhood environments. The IPAQ E-module has 17 environmental items designed for assessing whether neighborhoods are perceived as conducive to physical activity. In this study, we used 10 items (seven core, two recommended, and one optional item) [28] which included questions asking about: (1) residential density; (2) access to destinations; (3) neighborhood infrastructure; (4) aesthetic qualities; and (5) neighborhood safety. These items have been shown to have good reliability and validity [22,27,29,30].
Smoking and Alcohol Use Patterns. Cigarette smoking and alcohol use were determined using validated questions from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System [31]. The smoking questions included: (1) Have you ever smoked at least 100 cigarettes or the equivalent amount of tobacco in your lifetime? (2) Have you ever smoked daily? (3) Do you now smoke daily, occasionally, or not at all? and (4) On the average, how many cigarettes do you now smoke a day? Those who answered daily or occasionally were considered to be smokers and those who answered not at all were considered to be non-smokers (including former smokers). Heavy drinking was defined as consuming more than one and two drinks of any alcohol per day for women and men respectively (http://www.cdc.gov/alcohol/faqs.htm). In addition, binge drinking was determined by whether they consumed five or more drinks of alcohol on an occasion during the past 30 days [32].

2.2.2. Secondary Variables

Health Characteristics. Body Mass Index (BMI) was calculated as weight (kg) divided by height squared (m2). Physical activity level was determined using the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ; Guidelines for data processing and analysis of the IPAQ—Short and Long forms. www.ipaq.ki.se/scoring.pdf, November 2005), which asked participants to report their activities spent in physical activity for at least 10 min during the last 7 days. IPAQ scores were estimated by weighting time spent in each activity intensity with its estimated metabolic equivalent (MET) energy expenditure. Participants’ physical activity levels were categorized as low (<600 MET-min/week), moderate (600–3000 MET-min/week), and high (>3000 MET-min/week). Participants also were asked to provide the number of poor physical health days experienced during the last 30 days [33]. This question has established validity and reliability, is predictive of longitudinal health outcomes, and is used as part of an overall health rating system for the U.S. [33,34].
Sociodemographic Characteristics. Participants’ sociodemographic characteristics included age, gender, marital status, race/ethnicity, education, employment status, and annual household income
Psychosocial Characteristics. Depressive symptoms were measured with the 10-item version of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression scale (CES-D 10) [35]. It was found to have comparable reliability estimates to those reported for the original CES-D and had strong internal consistency (Chronbach’s α = 0.9) and test-retest reliability (r = 0.8) [35,36]. In addition, the 4-item version of the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) [37] was employed to assess the degree to which participants often felt or thought the way described by the items in the past month. The PSS has been found to be highly reliable in the general U.S. population [37,38]. For both scales, higher scores indicated higher levels of psychological distress.
Neighborhood SES Indicators. Neighborhood demographic variables collected included median yearly family household income and percent of the minority population within a census block group. As explained previously, the selected block groups were divided into three categories based on their median yearly family household income; lowest, middle, and highest tertiles; and were median-split into 2 categories based on their minority population; lowest and highest percentage of minority.

2.3. Analytic Plan

Factor Analysis. The factorability of the perceived environment covariance matrix was examined using Dziuban and Shirkey’s test [39]. The overall Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy was 0.71, which places it in the “average” range. The Bartlett sphericity test was significant (p < 0.001) and the determinant of the matrix was in the acceptable range. Thus, the perceived environment covariance matrix appeared appropriate for factor analysis. In order to condense the perceived environment variables from the IPAQ-E module, principal component analysis with varimax rotation using SAS PROC FACTOR was used to identify groups of related perceived neighborhood attributes. Component loadings ≥0.4 were considered significant [40]. The analysis identified three components accounting for 59.1% of the variance. All three dimensions demonstrated acceptable reliability: (1) Neighborhood’s Infrastructures (4 items); (2) Neighborhood Safety (2 items); and (3) Neighborhood Accessibility (2 items). Therefore, these dimensions were used for further analysis.
The Neighborhood’s Infrastructures dimension included: (1) “The sidewalks in my neighborhood are well maintained and not obstructed”; (2) “There are sidewalks on most of the streets in my neighborhood”; (3) “There are facilities to bicycle in or near my neighborhood” and (4) “My neighborhood has several free or low cost recreation facilities”. The Neighborhood Safety dimension included: (1) “The crime rate in my neighborhood makes it unsafe to go on walks at night”; and (2) “There is so much traffic on the streets that it makes it difficult or unpleasant to walk in my neighborhood”. The Neighborhood Accessibility dimension included: (1) “Many shops, stores, markets or other places to buy things I need are within easy walking distance of my home”; (2) It is within a 10–15 min walk to a transit stop from my home; and (3) “There are many interesting things to look at while walking in my neighborhood”.
Internal consistency for each of the dimensions was demonstrated to be adequate using Cronbach’s Alpha; 0.70, 0.60, and 0.60 for Neighborhood’s Infrastructures, Safety, and Accessibility dimensions, respectively. A score for each dimension was constructed by summing responses to each item included; higher scores indicated better ratings of the perceived neighborhood environments. For the bivariate analyses, each category was dichotomized on the basis of a median split, dividing the index into negative (i.e., unattractive or unsafe neighborhood) and positive (i.e., attractive or safe neighborhood) overall perception.
Data Analysis. We hypothesized that how people perceived their neighborhood environment (positive/negative) would be related to tobacco use and alcohol consumption. Thus, our models focused on the relationships between neighborhood perception variables and tobacco and alcohol use. Potential confounding variables that have been identified in the literature or have been thought to be conceptually related to alcohol and tobacco use or neighborhood perceptions also were included in all models (e.g., age in 5-year intervals, other demographic factors, physical activity level, BMI, and self-reported health). Independent samples t-tests were used to compare gender differences on continuous variables, while Chi-Square analysis was used to compare gender differences on categorical variables. The literature suggests that neighborhood characteristics are differently perceived and observed between men and women, so the data were analyzed separately. To examine the likelihood of being a current smoker, heavy drinker, and/or a binge drinker, mixed models were created within the SAS PROC GLIMMIX (SAS 9.2, SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA) with neighborhood as a random effect in each model to adjust for the sampling approach used in the study. Statistical models produced odds ratios (ORs) which represent the odds of having the outcome of interest (being a smoker, binge and heavy drinker).

3. Results

Table 1 provides the sociodemographic, neighborhood, and psychosocial characteristics and health behaviors of the study population.
Table 1. Participants sociodemographic, neighborhood, psychosocial characteristics and health behaviors (n = 586) a.
Table 1. Participants sociodemographic, neighborhood, psychosocial characteristics and health behaviors (n = 586) a.
CharacteristicsAllWomen (n = 409)Men (n = 177)
Sociodemographic Characteristics
Age (years)45.0 ± 14.745.0 ± 14.345.1 ± 15.7
Marital Status, %
  • Single
42.940.947.4
  • Married
57.159.152.6
Race, %
  • White, non-Hispanic
63.162.664.4
  • Racial/Ethnic Minority
36.937.435.5
Education
  • ≤12 years
25.825.6926.01
  • >12 years
74.274.3173.99
Employment Status, %
  • Unemployed
40.142.534.7
  • Employed
59.957.565.3
Annual HH income37,346.5 ± 55,708.633,825.4 ± 48,733.945,482.8 ± 68,661.2
Perceived Neighborhood Characteristics
  • Infrastructures
10.7 ± 3.510.5 ± 3.610.9 ± 3.4
  • Safety
6.1 ± 1.76.0 ± 1.86.5 ± 1.5
  • Accessibility
7.8 ± 2.47.6 ± 2.48.1 ± 2.4
Neighborhood SES Indicators
Block Group Median Income, %
  • Lowest tertile
35.433.1740.57
  • Middle tertile
30.430.4730.29
  • Highest tertile
34.236.3629.14
Block Group Minority Population, %
  • Lowest
52.249.8857.71
  • Highest
47.850.1242.29
Psychosocial Characteristics
Depression2.2 ± 2.22.3 ± 2.21.9 ± 1.9
Perceived Stress 3.9 ± 3.03.9 ± 3.13.9 ± 2.8
Health Characteristics
Smoker, %25.423.5629.48
Heavy drinkers, %59.261.1254.80
Binge drinkers, %12.78.2621.09
Physical Activity, % low14.514.215.3
Number of self-reported physical health days 2.7 ± 5.52.7 ± 5.72.5 ± 4.9
BMI, kg/m228.4 ± 7.628.3 ± 8.028.6 ± 6.6
a mean ± SD unless noted otherwise; Bolded values indicate statistically significant differences between gender based on t-test and Chi-Square values, p < 0.05.
Overall, the mean age of the sample population was 45 years and 69.8% were female. The majority of the sample were non-Hispanic Whites who were employed and completed high school. There were no significant sociodemographic differences between men and women except for annual household income. With respect to psychosocial and health characteristics, female participants were more likely to report having depressive symptoms, but less likely to be binge drinkers in the past 30 days compared to male respondents. Male participants had significantly higher scores on the measures of perceived neighborhood safety and neighborhood accessibility than their female counterparts (p < 0.05).
Table 2 presents ORs and 95% confidence intervals for the associations between individual and perceived neighborhood characteristics and the likelihood of being current smokers, or heavy and binge drinkers. Mixed model analyses were used to examine variables that influenced smoking or problem drinking patterns. Statistically significant associations between neighborhood perceptions and the likelihood of smoking or binge drinking were only found among male participants. Men who had positive perceptions of neighborhood infrastructure were almost five times less likely to smoke (OR = 4.8; p < 0.05), and eight time less likely to binge drink (OR = 7.7; p < 0.05). However, male participants who had positive perceptions on neighborhood accessibility were almost seven time more likely to smoke (OR = 6.7; p < 0.05). The only health characteristic that was significantly associated with the smoking was physical activity level. Men who had medium or high level of physical activity were five times more likely to smoke compared to those who had low level of physical activity.
Table 2. Logistic regression models of smoking, heavy drinking and binge drinking prevalence.
Table 2. Logistic regression models of smoking, heavy drinking and binge drinking prevalence.
CharacteristicsSmokingHeavy DrinkingBinge Drinking
Women OR (95% CI)Men OR (95% CI)Women OR (95% CI)Men OR (95% CI)Women OR (95% CI)Men OR (95% CI)
Perceived Neighborhood Characteristics
Infrastructures
  • Negative Perceptions (ref)
1.01.01.01.01.01.0
  • Positive Perceptions
1.2 (0.7−2.2)0.2 (0.1−0.8)1.2 (0.7−2.1)0.8 (0.3−1.9)1.9 (0.6−6.7)0.1 (0.1−0.7)
Safety
  • Negative Perceptions (ref)
1.01.01.01.01.01.0
  • Positive Perceptions
0.9 (0.5−1.6)3.3 (0.7−15.8)1.4 (0.8−2.4)0.8 (0.3−2.5)2.2 (0.6−8.8)1.4 (0.2−8.4)
Accessibility
  • Negative Perceptions (ref)
1.01.01.01.01.01.0
  • Positive Perceptions
1.3 (0.7−2.4)6.7 (1.5−30.6)1.4 (0.9−2.4)1.2 (0.5−3.0)1.2 (0.4−4.4)1.2 (0.2−6.1)
Health characteristics
BMI (kg/m2)1.0 (0.9−1.0)0.9 (0.8−1.0)0.9 (0.9−1.2)0.9 (0.9−1.0)1.0 (0.9−1.1)0.9 (0.8−1.1)
Physical Activity
  • Low (ref)
1.001.01.01.01.01.0
  • Medium/High
1.3 (0.8−2.1)5.2 (1.5−18.4)1.0 (0.9−1.4)1.4 (0.7−2.7)1.7 (0.6−4.8)3.0 (0.5−17.7)
Self-reported Health1.0 (0.9−1.1)1.2 (0.9−1.4)1.0 (0.9−1.0)1.0 (0.9−1.1)1.0 (0.9−1.1)1.2 (0.9−1.5)
Sociodemographic Characteristics
Age (10-year interval)0.8 (0.6−0.9)0.7 (0.4−1.2)0.7 (0.6−0.9)0.9 (0.6−1.2)0.8 (0.5−1.4)0.7 (0.4−1.4)
Marital Status
  • Single (ref)
1.01.01.01.01.01.0
  • Married
0.7 (0.4−1.2)0.6 (0.2−2.5)0.6 (0.3−1.1)0.8 (0.3−2.2)0.3 (0.1−1.2)0.8 (0.2−4.3)
Race
  • White, non-Hispanic (ref)
1.01.01.01.01.01.0
  • Minorities
0.7 (0.3−1.4)0.2 (0.04−1.1)0.5 (0.3−0.9)0.6 (0.2−1.7)2.2 (0.6−7.9)1.1 (0.12−9.30)
Education
  • ≤12 years (ref)
1.01.001.01.01.01.0
  • >12 years
0.7 (0.4−1.3)1.00 (0.2−5.9)1.0 (0.5−1.8)0.5 (0.2−1.7)1.1 (0.3−4.8)0.4 (0.1−2.6)
Employment Status, %
  • Unemployed (ref)
1.01.01.01.01.01.0
  • Employed
1.0 (0.6−1.8)0.6 (0.2−2.7)1.5 (0.9−2.6)2.4 (0.9−6.5)0.5 (0.1−1.7)1.4 (0.2−9.7)
Psychosocial Characteristics
Depression1.1 (0.9−1.2)1.8 (1.2−2.7)0.9 (0.8−1.1)0.8 (0.6−1.1)1.0 (0.7−1.4)1.3 (0.7−2.1)
Perceived Stress 1.0 (0.9−1.2)0.8 (0.6−1.0)1.0 (0.9−1.2)1.1 (0.9−1.3)1.1 (0.8−1.3)0.9 (0.6−1.4)
Bolded values indicate statistically significant differences. CI: Confident interval. Results are adjusted for neighborhood SES and percentage of minority in block group using a multilevel modeling approach.
Depression also showed a significant relationship with smoking in male respondents (OR = 1.8; p < 0.01). Among female participants, being younger was associated with smoking (OR = 1.3; p < 0.05) and heavy drinking (OR = 1.4; p < 0.05), while heavy drinkers were more likely to be White, non-Hispanics (OR = 2.0; p < 0.05).

4. Discussion

We have explored the relationship between perceived neighborhood environments and individual sociodemographic and psychosocial characteristics on the likelihood of being a current smoker, heavy and/or binge drinker. We found that perceived neighborhood problems were strongly associated with the likelihood of current smoking and binge drinking in men. Male respondents who had negative perceptions of neighborhood infrastructures were more than four times more likely to be current smokers and eight times more likely to be binge drinkers, which is consistent with results from a previous study assessing perceived neighborhood problems and smoking and drinking [41]. In contrast, men who had negative perceptions of neighborhood accessibility were less likely to smoke. This finding is in line with several studies reporting that convenient accessibility to shops or stores has been liked with smoking [16,42]. Chuang [16] suggested that greater access to stores may accentuate greater access to tobacco products and more pro-tobacco influences. In addition, Pearce et al. [42] hypothesized that, where shops, supermarkets, and stores are more accessible, the opportunities of tobacco consumption are increasing. However, similar relationships between those health behaviors and neighborhood perceptions were not found among female participants. These results might be because men and women perceive neighborhood environment characteristics differently, which could influence on how they respond to the neighborhood situation. For example, several studies have found that females who had lower perceptions of neighborhood infrastructure or safety were more likely to constrain their health-risk activity compared to their male counterparts [43,44,45]. One possible explanation for this difference might be related to gender roles in that women tend to be the main caregivers and role model for children which may lead them to make healthier choices [46]. Unlike previous work demonstrating and association between perceived neighborhood safety and smoking and alcohol consumption [41,47], this study failed to replicate such relationships.
Depressive symptoms increased the likelihood of smoking among men, suggesting that those who experience distress might use tobacco as a coping strategy or distress reliever [48,49]. In addition, negative neighborhood perceptions have been found to be associated with mental health problems through the effects of psychological stress [50,51] and those who were in those conditions might use cigarettes and alcohol to reduce stress, distress and to escape problems [14,52]. Moreover, aspects and physical features of neighborhoods (i.e., poor-quality housing, absence of basic resources, lack of health care or recreational facilities, lack of access to public transportation), and density of buildings may function as stressors that could influence smoking and alcohol consumption [15,53].
Several sociodemographic variables were found to be associated with health behaviors. In accordance with previous research, younger women were more likely to smoke, and use alcohol excessively (i.e., be heavy drinkers) when compared to their older counterparts [54,55]. In addition, female participants who identified as non-Hispanic White were more likely to be heavy drinkers than were women of color [56]. O’Hare [57] implied that this drinking pattern could be mediated by cultural factors and social learning, which has been supported by several other studies [56,58,59]. Thus ethnic/race drinking differences should be further explored.
Physical activity level was found to have a positive association with being a current smoker. Men who had medium and high physical level tended to smoke more than those who were physically inactive, which is in opposition to findings from studies that reported a negative association [60]. However, our findings are in agreement with those of others who reported significant associations between smoking and physical activity [61,62,63]. The possible explanation might be due to a compensation effect [63,64], in which smokers tended to exercise more in order to compensate for the negative health effect of smoking.
Several limitations about this study should be acknowledged. Due to the cross-sectional nature of this study, the associations between neighborhood perceptions and health behaviors could be influenced by other factors such as personality, beliefs or attitudes which may cause individuals to choose to live or behave in a particular way. For example, people tend to choose their neighborhoods based on similar lifestyle profile of neighbors [65] or people who always engage in risky behaviors would express similar attitudes regardless of their neighborhoods [66]. Longitudinal studies examining these relationships are needed and would allow us to better understand the direction of the relationships between exposures and outcomes. In addition, some of the associations demonstrated wide 95% confidence intervals, indicating that the effect of these variables are uncertain and might be due to the small sample size and further information is needed. Despite these limitations, our findings suggest that some neighborhood perception variables are associated with tobacco and alcohol use among men in our sample and that this association could be influenced by stress and depression presented in the environments [24]. Therefore, it is significant to identify the mechanism that may explain these relationships.

5. Conclusions

Our findings indicate that the perceived neighborhood environment, especially neighborhood infrastructures, was predictive of health behaviors among participants in this sample, even after adjusting for key confounders. Interestingly, other individual characteristics such as demographic variables and psychosocial variables also demonstrated strong relationship with those health outcomes. Thus, closer attention may need to be paid to the role of neighborhood environment characteristics along with individual-level characteristics in influencing unhealthy behaviors.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by a grant from the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases awarded to Poston (R01DK064284) and a NIH Minority Supplement to Berkel (DK064284-S).

Author Contributions

Nattinee Jitnarin contributed to the analysis and interpretation of the data, and led the drafting of the manuscript. Christopher K. Haddock and Walker S.C. Poston contributed to the interpretation of the findings and critically revised the manuscript. Katie M. Heinrich, Joseph Hughey, and LaVerne Berkel critically reviewed the manuscript. All authors contributed to and approved the final manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. De Hollander, A.E.M.; Staatsen, B.A.M. Health, environment and quality of life: An epidemiological perspective on urban development. Landscape Urban Plan. 2013, 65, 53–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Ellaway, A.; Macintyre, S.; Kearns, A. Perceptions of place and health in socially contrasting neighborhoods. Urban Stud. 2001, 38, 2299–2316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Pickett, K.E.; Pearl, M. Multilevel analyses of neighborhood socioeconomic context and health outcomes: A critical review. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2001, 55, 111–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Echeverria, S.; Diez Roux, A.V.; Shea, S.; Borrell, L.N.; Jackson, S. Associations of neighborhood problems and neighborhood social cohesion with mental health and health behaviors: The Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis. Health Place 2008, 14, 853–865. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Gidlow, C.; Cochrane, T.; Davey, R.C.; Smith, G.; Fairburn, J. Relative importance of physical and social aspects of perceived neighborhood environment for self-reported health. Prev. Med. 2010, 51, 157–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Mair, C.; Diez Roux, A.V.; Galea, S. Are neighborhood characteristics associated with depressive symptoms? A review of evidence. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2008, 62, 940–946. [Google Scholar]
  7. Santos, R.; Silva, P.; Santos, P.; Ribeiro, J.C.; Mota, J. Physical activity and perceived environmental attributes in a sample of Portuguese adults; Results from the Azorean Physical Activity and Health Study. Prev. Med. 2008, 47, 83–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Santos, R.; Vale, S.; Miranda, L.; Mota, J. Socio-demographic and perceived environmental correlates of walking in Portuguese adults—A multilevel analysis. Health Place 2009, 15, 1094–1099. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Pampel, F.C.; Krueger, P.M.; Denney, J.T. Socioeconomic disparities in health behaviors. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2010, 36, 349–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Report on the Global Tobacco Epidemic: Implementing Smoke-Free Environments; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2009.
  11. Global Status Report on Alcohol and Health 2014; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2014.
  12. Huisman, M.; Kunst, A.E.; Mackenbach, J.P. Inequalities in the prevalence of smoking in the European Union: Comparing education and income. Prev. Med. 2005, 40, 756–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Siahpush, M.; Heller, G.; Singh, G.K. Lower levels of occupation, income and education are strongly associated with a longer smoking duration: Multivariate results from the 2001 Australian National Drug Strategy Survey. Public Health 2005, 119, 1105–1110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Hill, T.D.; Angel, R. Neighborhood disorder, psychological distress, and heavy drinking. Soc. Sci. Med. 2005, 61, 965–975. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Cutrona, C.E.; Wallace, G.; Wesner, K.A. Neighborhood characteristics and depression: An examination of stress processes. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 15, 188–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Chuang, Y.C.; Cubbin, C.; Ahn, D.; Winkleby, M.A. Effects of neighborhood socioeconomic status and convenience store concentration on individual level smoking. J. Epidemiol. Community Health. 2005, 59, 568–573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Reid, R.J.; Peterson, N.A.; Lowe, J.B.; Hughey, J. Tobacco outlet density and smoking prevalence: Does racial concentration matter? Drug Edu. Prev. Polic. 2005, 12, 233–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Schneider, J.E.; Reid, R.J.; Peterson, N.A.; Lowe, J.B.; Hughey, J. Tobacco outlet density and demographics at the tract level of analysis in Iowa: Implications for environmentally based prevention initiatives. Prev. Sci. 2005, 6, 319–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Gary, T.L.; Safford, M.M.; Gerzoff, R.B.; Ettner, S.L.; Karter, A.J.; Beckles, G.L.; Brown, A.F. Perception of neighborhood problems, health behaviors, and diabetes outcomes among adults with diabetes in managed care: The Translating Research into Action for Diabetes (TRIAD) study. Diabetes Care 2008, 31, 273–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Kim, D. Blues from the neighborhood? Neighborhood characteristics and depression. Epidemiol. Rev. 2008, 30, 101–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Schaefer-McDaniel, N. Neighborhood stressors, perceived neighborhood quality and child mental health in New York City. Health Place 2009, 15, 148–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. De Boudeaudhuij, I.; Sallis, J.F.; Saelen, B.E. Environmental correlates of physical activity in a sample of Belgium adults. Amer. J. Health Promot. 2003, 18, 83–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Sallis, J.F.; Bowles, H.R.; Bauman, A.; Ainsworth, B.E.; Bull, F.C.; Craig, C.L.; Sjostrom, M.; de Bourdeaudhuij, I.; Lefevre, J.; Matsudo, S.; et al. Neighborhood environments and physical activity among adults in 11 countries. Amer. J. Prev. Med. 2009, 36, 484–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Diez Roux, A.V. Investigating neighborhood and area effects on health. Amer. J. Public Health 2001, 91, 1783–1789. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Heinrich, K.M.; Hughey, J.; Randles, A.; Wall, D.; Peterson, N.A.; Jitnarin, N.; Berkel, L.; Eaton, P.; Bowles, D.; Haddock, C.K.; et al. The Census of Social Institutions (CSI): A public health direct observation measure of local land use. J. Urban Health 2010, 87, 410–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Fowler, J.J. Survey Research Methods, 3rd ed.; SAGE Publications: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  27. Alexander, A.; Bergman, P.; Hagstromer, M.; Sjostrom, M. IPAQ environmental module; Reliability testing. J. Public Health 2006, 14, 76–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. International Prevalence Study. International Physical Activity Prevalence Study Environmental Survey Module. Available online: http://www.drjamessallis.sdsu.edu/IPAQIPS.pdf (accessed on 6 September 2014).
  29. Oyeyemi, A.L.; Adegoke, B.O.; Oyeyemi, A.Y.; Fatudimu, B.M. Test-retest reliability of IPAQ environmental-module in an African population. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act 2008, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Saelen, B.E.; Sallis, J.F.; Black, J.B.; Chen, D. Neighborhood based differences in physical activity: An environmental scale evaluation. Amer. J. Public Health 2003, 93, 1552–1558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System Survey Questionnaire. Available online: http://www.cdc.gov/brfss/questionnaires.htm (accessed on 1 September 2014).
  32. Pletcher, M.J.; Varosy, P.; Kiefe, C.I.; Lewis, C.E.; Sidney, S.; Hulley, S.B. Alcohol consumption, binge drinking, and early Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) study. Amer. J. Epidemiol. 2005, 161, 423–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Moriaty, D.G.; Zack, M.M.; Kobau, R. The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention’s Healthy Days Measures—Population tracking of perceived physical and mental health over time. Health Qual. Life Outcomes 2003, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Dominick, K.L.; Ahern, F.M.; Gold, C.H.; Heller, D.A. Relationship of health-related quality of life to health care utilization and mortality among older adults. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 2002, 14, 499–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Irwin, M.; Artin, K.H.; Oxman, M.N. Screening for depression in the older adult: Criterion validity of the 10-item Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D). Arch. Int. Med. 1999, 159, 1701–1704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Radloff, L.S. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 1977, 1, 385–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Cohen, S.; Kamarck, T.; Mermelstein, R. A global measure of perceived stress. J. Health Soc. Behav. 1983, 24, 385–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Cohen, S.; Williamson, G. Perceived stress in a probability sample of the U.S. In The Social Psychology of Health: Claremont Symposium on Applied Social Psychology; Spacapam, S., Oskamp, S., Eds.; Sage Publications: Newbury Park, CA, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  39. Dziuban, C.D.; Shirkey, E.C. When is a correlation matrix appropriate for factor analysis? Some decision rules. Psychol. Bull. 1974, 81, 358–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Meulman, J.J.; van der Kooij, A.J.; Heiser, W.J. Principal components analysis with nonlinear optimal scaling transformations for ordinal and nominal data. In Handbook of Quantitative Methods in the Social Sciences; Kaplan, D., Ed.; Sage Publications: Newbury Park, CA, USA, 2004; pp. 49–70. [Google Scholar]
  41. Ellaway, A.; Macintyre, S. Are perceived neighborhood problems associated with the likelihood of smoking? J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2009, 63, 78–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Pearce, J.; Hiscock, R.; Moon, G.; Barnett, R. The neighborhood effects of geographical access to tobacco retailers on individual smoking behavior. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2009, 63, 69–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Garcia, B.E.; Spence, J.C.; McGannon, K.R. Gender differences in perceived environmental correlates of physical activity. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2005, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Kim, D.; Diez Roux, A.V.; Kiefe, C.I.; Kawachi, I.; Liu, K. Do neighborhood socioeconomic deprivation and low social cohesion predict coronary calcification? The CARDIA Study. Amer. J. Epidemiol. 2010, 172, 288–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Mohai, P. Gender differences in the perception of most important environmental problems. Race Gender Class 1997, 5, 153–169. [Google Scholar]
  46. Ellaway, A.L.; Macintyre, S. Women in their place: Gender and perceptions of neighborhoods and health in the West of Scotland. In Geographics of Women’s Health; Dyck, I., Davis Lewis, N., McLafferty, S., Eds.; Routledge: London, UK, 2001; pp. 265–281. [Google Scholar]
  47. Shareck, M.; Ellaway, A. Neighborhood crime and smoking: The role of objective and perceived crime measures. BMC Public Health 2011, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Huurre, T.; Lintonen, T.; Kaprio, J.; Pelkonen, M.; Marttunen, M.; Aro, H. Adolescent risk factors for excessive alcohol use at age 32 years. A 16-year propective follow-up study. Soc. Psychiat. Psychiat. Epidem. 2010, 45, 125–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Munafo, M.R.; Araya, R. Cigarette smoking and depression: A question of causation. Brit. J. Psychiatr. 2010, 196, 425–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Booth, J.; Ayers, S.L.; Marsiglia, F.F. Perceived neighborhood safety and psychological distress: Exploring protective factors. J. Soc. Soc. Welfare 2012, 39, 137–156. [Google Scholar]
  51. Schulz, A.J.; Zenk, S.N.; Israel, B.A.; Mentz, G.; Stokes, C.; Galea, S. Do neighborhood economic characteristics, racial composition, and residential stability predict perceptions of stress associated with the physical and social environment? Findings from a multilevel analysis in Detroit. J. Urban Health 2008, 85, 642–661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Stockdale, S.E.; Wells, K.B.; Tang, L.; Belin, T.R.; Zhang, L.; Sherbourne, C.D. The importance of social context: Neighborhood stressors, stress-buffering mechanisms, and alcohol, drug, and mental health disorders. Soc. Sci. Med. 2007, 65, 1867–1881. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Evans, G.W. The built environment and mental health. J. Urban. Health 2003, 80, 536–555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Centers of Disease Control and Prevention. Alcohol use among pregnant and nonprenant women of childbearing age—United States, 1991–2005. MMWR (Morb. Mortal. Wkly. Rep.) 2009, 58, 529–532. [Google Scholar]
  55. Centers of Disease Control and Prevention. Vital signs: Current cigarette smoking among adults aged ≥18 years—United States, 2009. MMWR 2010, 59, 1135–1140. [Google Scholar]
  56. Galvan, F.H.; Caetano, R. Alcohol use and related problems among ethnic minorities in the United States. Alcohol. Res. Health 2003, 27, 87–94. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  57. O’Hare, T. Differences in Asian and White drinking: Consumption level, drinking contexts, and expectancies. Addict. Behav. 1995, 20, 261–266. [Google Scholar]
  58. Caetano, R.; Clark, C.L. Trends in situ ational norms and attitudes toward drinking among Whites, Blacks, and Hispanics: 1984–1995. Drug Alcohol. Depend. 1999, 54, 45–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Bersamin, M.; Paschall, M.J.; Flewelling, R.L. Ethnic differences in relationships between risk factors and adolescent binge drinking: A national study. Prev. Sci. 2005, 6, 127–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Kaczynski, A.T.; Maske, S. Smoking and physical activity: A systematic review. Amer. J. Health Behav. 2008, 32, 93–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Verkooijen, K.T.; Neilsen, G.A.; Kermers, S.J. The association between leisure time physical activity and smoking in adolescences: An examination of potential mediating and moderating factors. Int. J. Behav. Med. 2008, 15, 157–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Audrain-McGovern, J.; Rodriguez, D.; Moss, H.B. Smoking progression and physical activity. Cancer Epidemiol. Biomarkers Prev. 2003, 12, 1121–1129. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  63. Conway, T.L.; Cronan, T.A. Smoking, exercise, and physical fitness. Prev. Med. 1992, 21, 723–734. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. DeRuiter, W.; Faulkner, G. Tobacco harm reduction strategies: The case for physical activity. Nicotine. Tob. Res. 2006, 8, 157–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Kawachi, I.; Berkman, L.F. Neighborhoods and Health; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  66. Ross, C.E. Walking, exercising, and smoking: Does neighborhood matter? Soc. Sci. Med. 2000, 51, 265–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Jitnarin, N.; Heinrich, K.M.; Haddock, C.K.; Hughey, J.; Berkel, L.; Poston, W.S.C. Neighborhood Environment Perceptions and the Likelihood of Smoking and Alcohol Use. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 784-799. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph120100784

AMA Style

Jitnarin N, Heinrich KM, Haddock CK, Hughey J, Berkel L, Poston WSC. Neighborhood Environment Perceptions and the Likelihood of Smoking and Alcohol Use. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2015; 12(1):784-799. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph120100784

Chicago/Turabian Style

Jitnarin, Nattinee, Katie M. Heinrich, Christopher K. Haddock, Joseph Hughey, LaVerne Berkel, and Walker S.C. Poston. 2015. "Neighborhood Environment Perceptions and the Likelihood of Smoking and Alcohol Use" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 12, no. 1: 784-799. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph120100784

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop