Skip to main content

STUDY PROTOCOL article

Front. Psychiatry, 08 June 2021
Sec. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
This article is part of the Research Topic Longitudinal Data Analysis in Child and Adolescent Mental Health View all 10 articles

Study Protocol: Longitudinal Attention and Temperament Study

  • 1Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, United States
  • 2Department of Psychology, Rutgers University, Newark, NJ, United States
  • 3School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, United Kingdom

Background: Attention processes may play a central role in shaping trajectories of socioemotional development. Individuals who are clinically anxious or have high levels of trait anxiety sometimes show attention biases to threat. There is emerging evidence that young children also demonstrate a link between attention bias to salient stimuli and broad socioemotional profiles. However, we do not have a systematic and comprehensive assessment of how attention biases, and associated neural and behavioral correlates, emerge and change from infancy through toddlerhood. This paper describes the Longitudinal Attention and Temperament study (LAnTs), which is designed to target these open questions.

Method: The current study examines core components of attention across the first 2 years of life, as well as measures of temperament, parental psychosocial functioning, and biological markers of emotion regulation and anxiety risk. The demographically diverse sample (N = 357) was recruited from the area surrounding State College, PA, Harrisburg, PA, and Newark, NJ. Infants and parents are assessed at 4, 8, 12, 18, and 24 months. Assessments include repeated measures of attention bias (via eye-tracking) in both infants and parents, and measures of temperament (reactivity, negative affect), parental traits (e.g., anxiety and depression), biological markers (electrophysiology, EEG, and respiratory sinus arrythmia, RSA), and the environment (geocoding, neighborhood characteristics, perceived stress). Outcomes include temperamental behavioral inhibition, social behavior, early symptom profiles, and cellular aging (e.g., telomere length).

Discussion: This multi-method study aims to identify biomarkers and behavioral indicators of attentional and socioemotional trajectories. The current study brought together innovative measurement techniques to capture the earliest mechanisms that may be causally linked to a pervasive set of problem behaviors. The analyses the emerge from the study will address important questions of socioemotional development and help shape future research. Analyses systematically assessing attention bias patterns, as well as socioemotional profiles, will allow us to delineate the time course of any emerging interrelations. Finally, this study is the first to directly assess competing models of the role attention may play in socioemotional development in the first years of life.

Background

The centrality of attention in development grows out of its role as a specific brain-based mechanism whose core function is to influence the operation of other mechanisms—by choosing the focus of attention for further processing, by maintaining this focus as needed, and by disengaging from the focus of attention when it no longer serves current goals (1). The earliest forms of self-regulation are rooted in the ability to disengage, shift gaze, and re-orient on a new focus of attention (2). In this way, attention mechanisms may play a pivotal role in shaping the individual's experienced environment from the first days of life (3). An emerging literature points to a potential causal association between attention (particularly attention bias to threat) and the presence of clinical and trait anxious behaviors in adults and children (4, 5). Attention bias refers to selective attention processes that preferentially select for and process specific categories of salient stimuli (6). There is some evidence that systematic biases toward and away from threat may play a causal or sustaining role in the emergence of disorder (7). In the anxiety literature, threat is often conveyed with the use of negative faces (e.g., angry or sad), particularly when examining social anxiety (7).

If this view is correct, individual differences in attention, first emerging in infancy, should be associated with diverging trajectories of socioemotional development. These trajectories may be potentiated among children at temperamental risk for anxiety (8) or children exposed to anxiogenic environments (9). In particular, the evident link between early temperamental negative affect and the later emergence of anxiety may be potentiated by the added presence of an attention bias to threat (10, 11). Although we are unable to follow the full emergence of anxiety in the first 2 years, we can capture the processes that may lay an initial developmental foundation. Understanding these early relations could thus provide avenues for (1) understanding mechanisms that lead to the emergence of social withdrawal and anxiety and (2) identifying individuals at risk for socioemotional difficulties. Taken together, this knowledge would help the field focus on specific windows of intervention, targeting causal mechanisms while the system is still plastic and malleable.

However, the literature to date cannot directly provide the needed data because it has focused on older children and adults when examining the relation between attention, affect, and socioemotional functioning. In addition, the data generated are from predominantly single-session, cross-sectional designs focusing on individuals already presenting with clear signs of clinical anxiety or trait-level distress (12, 13). Much of the developmentally-informed research on anxiety has focused on the classification and treatment of disorder (14). Although there is increasingly more data available with child samples, we have scant knowledge of normative or maladaptive developmental trajectories in infancy (15, 16). As such, it is not clear how attention patterns come to be associated with affect and how these constructs, together, underlie the emergence of anxiety.

Field and Lester (17) suggested three potential developmental models of attention bias (Figure 1 illustrates the models using temperament as the potential developmental moderator). The integral bias model (18) suggests that the magnitude of any information processing is determined by individual factors (e.g., anxiety, temperament) and should be evident and fairly stable across the lifespan, assuming that it is measured using a developmentally appropriate task (Figure 1A). As such, infants with early signs of negative affect would already show a more pronounced bias to threat relative to infants without this temperamental profile. Much of the current clinical literature makes this implicit assumption. The moderation model (17) suggests that development moderates the expression of an existing bias to threat (Figure 1B), such that under certain circumstances (e.g., in children at temperamental risk for anxiety) the initial normative bias may be linked to the later emergence of elevated fear and social withdrawal (6, 19, 20). In contrast, normative biases will decrease over time for children with low temperamental risk. Finally, the acquisition model (Figure 1C) suggests that developmental experiences shape the acquisition of an attention bias gradually over time (17), either in tandem or subsequent to the emergence of fear and anxiety.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic illustration of the three models for the emergence of attention bias in the first years of life based on Field and Lester (17). The integral bias, moderation, and acquisition models differ in the timing, stability, and sensitivity to outside factors for attention processes.

Testing these models, and examining the broader assumptions regarding attention-emotion relations, requires systematic studies that examine individual differences (21) across multiple levels of analysis (22) over time. Our prior work has examined associated questions in cross-sectional samples (2327). The Longitudinal Attention and Temperament study (LAnTs; Figure 2) was designed to extend this work by bringing together three developmentally-appropriate tasks (dot-probe, overlap, vigilance) that can be used across the first 2 years of life (26). In addition, we assess early temperament using both observed behavior and parental reports. To identify endogenous factors that may modulate developmental risk, we assess resting electroencephalogram (EEG) to capture measures associated with emotion regulation and socioemotional risk, including frontal EEG alpha asymmetry (28), delta-beta coupling (29), and neural noise (30). We also capture respiratory sinus arrythmia (RSA) at rest and during our temperament battery (31) to examine peripheral markers of regulation (32). Given the central role parents play in shaping the daily experiences of children, contextual measures of parental attention bias, symptomatology, and psychosocial stress are assessed at every time point. Finally, we incorporate both objective (e.g., geocoding) and subjective (e.g., perceived violence and support) measures of the child's broader environment (33). Across levels of analysis the protocol generates a multidimensional profile of the individual and nested layers of development from the micro- through the mesosystem over time (34). LAnTs was designed to examine two core aims:

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Schematic of the LAnTs cohort. Families are recruited from areas surrounding three locations, State College, PA, Harrisburg, PA, and Newark, NJ. The sample was recruited primarily at 4 months of age, and then assessed again at 8, 12, 18, and 24 months.

First, we will test the integral bias, moderation, and acquisition models outlined by Field and Lester (17). The first step will be to quantify the developmental trajectory (i.e., growth curve) of attention to threat. Each developmental model makes a unique prediction regarding how individual, biological, and environmental moderators will affect the size and direction of the developmental trajectory of attentional bias over time. Therefore, we will quantify the extent to which individual differences in negative affect moderate attention trajectories. We will then do the same analysis incorporating individual biomarkers (EEG and RSA). Finally, we will turn our focus on contextual factors (parental attention bias, symptomatology, psychosocial stress).

Second, we will examine the extent to which the gradient of individual attention growth curves predicts behavioral inhibition at age two. We will also capture potential behavioral, biological and contextual moderators of these individual gradients, particularly if the acquisition or moderation models are supported. As part of these outcome assessments, we will also examine early measures of psychopathology (35) and biological measures of chronic stress [e.g., telomere length (36)]. Greater detail regarding the larger analytic approach is provided in the Supplementary Materials.

The purpose of this paper is to provide a detailed description of the LAnTs protocol, measures, and sample. This will help place future analyses within the context of the full protocol. In addition, interested researchers may determine that the sample provides data needed for ancillary analyses.

Methods

General Procedure

We collected data from infants (N = 357) longitudinally at 4, 8, 12, 18, and 24 months, using a multi-method approach (see Table 1). At all five time-points the infant protocol included 3 eye-tracking tasks and a behavioral temperament battery [reactivity (37) at 4 months and the Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery (Lab-TAB) (38) at 8–24 months]. At these visits, parents also completed two eye-tracking tasks and questionnaires assessing infant temperament, their own psychological state and traits, and the sociodemographic features of their environment. Geocoding was used as an additional measure of the familial environment.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. List of LAnTS measures by time point of data collection.

At the latter four time-points participants engaged in a structured parent-infant interaction. Infants also provided resting EEG and RSA. RSA data were also collected during the behavioral temperament battery and parent-child dyads. At 24 months, infants completed a behavioral inhibition (BI) protocol and engaged in an additional social dyad with an unfamiliar same-age peer. At this final visit, buccal swabs were collected from parents and infants for telomere length assays.

Data collection was generally completed in two, 2-h visits to the lab for the first four timepoints, although some families completed all tasks in a single visit, and a subset of families required three visits. During Visit 1, the infant and the primary caregiver typically completed the eye tracking tasks, with the infant first, followed by the caregiver. At 4 months, the eye-tracking and behavioral measures were usually all collected in a single day. For the 8- through 24-month time points, resting EEG was collected during Visit 2, followed by free play, and the Lab TAB episodes. RSA was collected throughout the behavioral tasks. The majority of visits followed this structure, but task orders sometimes varied based on the infant's needs. Most caregivers completed the online questionnaires at home prior to the visit, but in some cases, they were completed in the lab or over the phone. If questionnaires had to be completed in the lab, primary caregivers would do so while the infant was completing the eye tracking tasks or after data collection was completed. The social dyad was completed on a separate day, in a final visit to the lab at 24 months.

A detailed description of each measure (see also Table 1) can be found in the Supplemental Materials.

Sample Characteristics

Here we highlight core metrics that describe and characterize the sample at the time of enrollment. Data collection is still ongoing through Fall 2021.

Sample

Participants were recruited through local baby registries (40% families) and university-sponsored participant databases (13% families). In addition, we used a variety of community-level recruitment strategies, such as visiting local lactation/parenting classes, communicating with families at local community events, and talking to parents at local hospitals, health care centers, and Women's and Infant Centers (WIC). Community recruiting identified 38% of our families. The remaining 10% of families were recruited by word-of-mouth. Prospective families were contacted by letter, email, or phone explaining the motivations and methods of the study. The Institutional Review Boards at the Pennsylvania State University and Rutgers University approved all procedures and parents provided written consent and were compensated for their participation.

Infants and their caregivers were enrolled when the infants were 4 months of age (N = 298; 151 males, 147 females; Mage = 4.80 months; SDage = 0.80, Rangeage = 3.27–7.60 months), with an additional 46 participants enrolled at 8 months (N = 46; 19 males, 27 females; Mage = 8.83 months; SDage = 0.73, Rangeage = 7.53–10.20 months), and 13 participants at 12 months (N = 13; six males, seven females; Mage = 12.73 months; SDage = 1.12, Rangeage = 10.63–14.90 months), for a total enrollment of 357 infants in the full sample (176 males, 181 females). Participants were recruited from areas surrounding three sites: State College, PA (N = 167), Harrisburg, PA (N = 81), and Newark, NJ (N = 109).

Race and Ethnicity

Caregivers identified 58 of the infants (16%) as African American/Black, 9 (3%) as Asian, 78 (22%) as Latinx, 180 (50%) as white, and 27 (8%) as mixed race. Five (1%) additional caregivers declined to provide this information (see Figure 3, left).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. LAnTs sample demographics, including racial/ethnic background (left) and income (right).

Annual Household Income

Across the sample, 49 families (14%) reported a household income of $15,000 or less, 20 (6%) reported $16,000–20,000, 22 (6%) reported $21,000–30,000, 16 (5%) reported $31,000–40,000, 22 (6%) reported $41,000–50,000, 29 (8%) reported $51,000–60,000, and 140 (39%) reported an income above $60,000. Fifty-nine (17%) additional caregivers declined to provide this information (see Figure 3, right).

Parental Education

For mother's education, 11 (3%) completed grade school only, 17 (5%) had some high school, 36 (10%) graduated from high school, 57 (16%) had some college or trade/technical degree, 73 (20%) were college graduates, 58 (16%) had graduate training, and 66 (19%) had a graduate degree; 39 (11%) additional caregivers declined to provide this information. For fathers, 11 (3%) completed grade school only, 15 (4%) had some high school, 50 (14%) graduated from high school, 60 (17%) had some college or trade/technical degree, 70 (20%) were college graduates, 42 (12%) had graduate training, and 56 (16%) had a graduate degree; 53 (15%) additional caregivers declined to provide this information.

Infant Temperament

Of our enrolled families, 312 parents completed the Infant Behavior Questionnaire (39) (one parent did not provide data for the negative affect subscale). For high-order factors, infants were rated on negative affect (M = 3.01, SD = 0.66, Min = 1.00, Max = 5.08), surgency (M = 4.50, SD = 7.84, Min = 2.37, Max = 6.53), and orienting/regulation (M = 5.08, SD = 6.09, Min = 2.28, Max = 7.00; see Figure 4) at time of enrollment.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Histograms noting the distribution of core measures of infant temperament for the higher-order scales of negative affect, regulation, and surgency from the Infant Behavior Questionnaire (left column) and maternal symptoms of anxiety and depression (right column) from the Beck Anxiety and Beck Depression Inventories, respectively, at the point of enrollment.

Parent Psychopathology

Parents completed the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) (40) and the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) (41) as measures of parental psychopathology (see Figure 5). Values were prorated to account for missing values, such that the denominator of the sum score was adjusted for each item a parent did not complete on the questionnaire. Of our enrolled families, 272 parents completed the BAI at the time of enrollment (M = 6.66, SD = 7.55, Min = 0.00, Max = 53.00). The BDI was completed by 277 parents at the time of enrollment (M = 5.80, SD = 6.49, Min = 0.00, Max = 48.00).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Histograms noting the distribution of core measures of parental perception of the environment at the point of enrollment. The first two rows present scores from of Parenting Daily Hassles with the frequency, hassles intensity, challenging behaviors, and parenting subscales. The third row presents the distribution of scores from the Confusion, Hubbub, and Order Scale.

Home Life and Parenting

As an assessment of environmental confusion in the home, 265 parents completed the Confusion, Hubbub, and Order Scale (CHAOS) (42) (M = 27.18, SD = 7.25, Min = 15.00, Max = 50.00; Figure 5). Parents also completed the Parent Daily Hassles Survey (PDHS-R), an assessment of the frequency and intensity of daily hassles (43). At enrollment, 263 parents completed the frequency of hassles scale (M = 37.09, SD = 13.76, Min = 20.00, Max = 100.00) and 235 parents completed the intensity of hassles scale (M = 37.09, SD = 13.76, Min = 20.00, Max = 100.00). The PDHS-R further provides a challenging behavior and parenting task intensity score. The challenging behavior total score is obtained by summing seven items from the intensity scale scores and the parenting tasks scale is obtained by summing eight items from the intensity scale. At enrollment, 234 completed the challenging behavior subscale (M = 13.99, SD = 5.89, Min =7, Max = 35.00) and 233 parents completed the parenting task intensity subscale (M = 14.39, SD = 5.17, Min = 8.00, Max = 32.00).

Discussion

The LAnT study's multi-method approach aims to (1) test the three models proposed by Field and Lester (17) and (2) examine the association between early patterns of attention to threat and BI at age 2 (3). This work fills evident gaps in the literature since the attention-affect research (1) has focused on adult clinically-defined populations, (2) often does not systematically assess constructs across multiple tasks and contexts, and (3) rarely takes a developmental view that examines core mechanisms as they emerge in infancy in hopes of differentiating between normative patterns and patterns associated with specific risk trajectories. This line of research reflects calls from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) to implement the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) across processes and across time (44). Here, we integrate multilevel mechanisms by examining response to potential threat (negative valence systems), attention patterns (cognitive systems) and early patterns of affect across varying socioemotional contexts (negative valence systems and social processes). We also go to the heart of NIMH's Objective 2, by characterizing trajectories of neural and behavioral development in order to identify clinically useful indicators of change across illness trajectories. This approach also parallels emerging studies (45) that examine selective attention and responsiveness to emotional expression as a means of scaffolding the development of empathy and social cognition. The available data also suggest that attention patterns, and their associations with socioemotional functioning, may change over time (4648). Thus, it will be important to continue longitudinal assessments beyond toddlerhood and into early childhood.

By capturing the earliest mechanisms that may be causally linked to a pervasive set of problem behaviors, the study applies innovative measurement techniques to central questions of socioemotional development and may shape future research. The systematic assessment of attention bias patterns, socioemotional profiles, and environmental characteristic will allow us to delineate the time course of any emerging interrelations. Finally, as outlined in the current paper, the measures generated through the protocol can serve as the foundation for numerous other questions of interest to the scientific community.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets for this study will be shared in the National Institute of Mental Health Data Archive (NDA) and Databrary (49) as data are collected, processed, and curated. Inquiries regarding data sharing and the status of the data can be addressed to the study PIs.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Boards at Pennsylvania State University and Rutgers University, Newark. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

KP-E, VL, KB, AF, and LAnTs team drafted the manuscript. KP-E, VL, KB, and AF conceptualized the study and wrote the grant funding the research. The LAnTs team designed the tasks, collected data, and wrote the protocol descriptions. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

The LAnTs Team

Lori Reider2, Jessica Burris2, Denise Oleas2, Anna Zhou1, Centia Thomas1, Samantha Leigh1, Brendan Ostlund1, Berenice Anaya1, Kelley Gunther1, Alicia Vallorani1, Elizabeth Youatt1, Caitlin Smith1, Norbert Promagan1, Kayla Brown1, Laura Bierstedt2, Claudia Pinzon2, Kali Revilla2, Michell Sarquez2, Piumi Rajasekera1, Elveena Fareedi1, Annika Kershner1, Meghan McDoniel1, Xiaoxue Fu1, Santiago Morales1, Leigha MacNeill1, Eran Auday1, Briana Ermanni1, Dara Tucker1, Kelly Metcalf1.

1 Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, United States

2 Department of Psychology, Rutgers University, Newark, NJ, United States

Funding

This study described in this manuscript was funded by the National Institute of Mental Health (R01MH109692; MPIs KP-E, VL, and KB). The funding agencies had no role in the design of the study or the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data or in writing the manuscript, apart from their financial contribution.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all of the families who have contributed to this study. We would also like to thank the Social Science Research Institute at Penn State for the initial seed funding that allowed for the initial design and testing of the attention tasks.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.656958/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Posner MI, Rothbart MK. Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. Ann Rev Psychol. (2007) 58:1–23. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085516

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Rothbart MK, Posner MI, Rosicky J. Orienting in normal and pathological development. Dev Psychopathol. (1994) 6:635–52. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400004715

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Pérez-Edgar K. Attention mechanisms in behavioral inhibition: exploring and exploiting the environment. In: Pérez-Edgar K, Fox NA, editors. Behavioral Inhibition. Cham: Springer (2018). p. 237–61.

Google Scholar

4. Fox NA, Hane AA, Pine DS. Plasticity for affective neurocircuitry: how the environment affects gene expression. Curr Direct Psychol Sci. (2007) 16:1–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00464.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Bar-Haim Y. Research review: attention bias modification (ABM): a novel treatment for anxiety disorders. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2010) 51:859–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02251.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

6. Todd RM, Cunningham WA, Anderson AK, Thompson E. Affect-biased attention as emotion regulation. Trends Cogn Sci. (2012) 16:365–72. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Van Bockstaele B, Verschuere B, Tibboel H, De Houwer J, Crombez G, Koster EHW. A review of current evidence for the causal impact of attentional bias on fear and anxiety. Psychol Bull. (2014) 140:682–721. doi: 10.1037/a0034834

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Pérez-Edgar K, Guyer AE. Behavioral inhibition: temperament or prodrome? Curr Behav Neurosci Rep. (2014) 1:182–90. doi: 10.1007/s40473-014-0019-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Siqueland L, Kendall PC, Steinberg L. Anxiety in children: perceived family environments and observed family interaction. J Clin Child Psychol. (1996) 25:225–37. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2502_12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Pérez-Edgar K, Reeb-Sutherland BC, McDermott JM, White LK, Henderson HA, Degnan KA, et al. Attention biases to threat link behavioral inhibition to social withdrawal over time in very young children. J Abn Child Psychol. (2011) 39:885–95. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9495-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Morales S, Taber-Thomas BC, Pérez-Edgar K. Patterns of attention to threat across tasks in behaviorally inhibited children at risk for anxiety. Dev Sci. (2017) 20:e12391. doi: 10.1111/desc.12391

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Bar-Haim Y, Lamy D, Pergamin L, Bakermans-Kranenburg M, van IJzendoorn M. Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Bull. (2007) 133:1–24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Pergamin-Hight L, Naim R, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH, Bar-Haim Y. Content specificity of attention bias to threat in anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. (2015) 35:10–8. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2014.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Chorpita BF, Barlow DH. The development of anxiety: the role of control in the early environment. Psychol Bull. (1998) 124:3–21. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.124.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Burris JL, Oleas D, Reider L, Buss KA, Pérez-Edgar K, LoBue V. Biased attention to threat: answering old questions with young infants. Curr Direct Psychol Sci. (2019) 28:534–9. doi: 10.1177/0963721419861415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Burris JL, Buss K, LoBue V, Pérez-Edgar K, Field AP. Biased attention to threat and anxiety: on taking a developmental approach. J Exp Psychopathol. (2019) 10:2043808719860717. doi: 10.1177/2043808719860717

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Field AP, Lester KJ. Is there room for ‘development’ in developmental models of information processing biases to threat in children and adolescents? Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. (2010) 13:315–32. doi: 10.1007/s10567-010-0078-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Field AP, Lester KJ. Learning of information processing biases in anxious children and adolescents. In: Hadwin JA, Field AP, editors. Information Processing Biases in Child and Adolescent Anxiety. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons (2010). p. 253–78.

Google Scholar

19. LoBue V. What are we so afraid of? How early attention shapes our most common fears. Child Dev Perspect. (2013) 7:38–42. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. LoBue V, DeLoache JS. Detecting the snake in the grass: attention to fear-relevant stimuli by adults and young children. Psychol Sci. (2008) 19:284–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02081.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Pérez-Edgar K, Vallorani A, Buss KA, LoBue V. Individual differences in infancy research: letting the baby stand out from the crowd. Infancy. (2020) 25:438–57. doi: 10.1111/infa.12338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. LoBue V, Reider L, Kim E, Burris JL, Oleas D, Buss KA, et al. The importance of using multiple outcome measures in infant research. Infancy. (2020) 25:420–37. doi: 10.1111/infa.12339

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Fu X, Morales S, LoBue V, Buss KA, Pérez-Edgar K. Temperament moderates developmental changes in vigilance to emotional faces in infants: evidence from an eye-tracking study. Dev Psychobiol. (2020) 62:339–52. doi: 10.1002/dev.21920

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Morales S, Brown KM, Taber-Thomas BC, LoBue V, Buss KA, Pérez-Edgar K. Maternal anxiety predicts attentional bias towards threat in infancy. Emotion. (2017) 17:874–83. doi: 10.1037/emo0000275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Pérez-Edgar K, Morales S, LoBue V, Taber-Thomas BC, Allen EK, Brown KM, et al. The impact of negative affect on attention patterns to threat across the first two years of life. Dev Psychol. (2017) 53:2219–32. doi: 10.1037/dev0000408

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Vallorani A, Fu X, Morales S, LoBue V, Buss KA, Pérez-Edgar K. Variable- and person-centered approaches to affect-biased attention in infancy reveal unique relations with infant negative affect and maternal anxiety. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:1719. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-81119-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. LoBue V, Buss KA, Taber-Thomas BC, Pérez-Edgar K. Developmental differences in infants' attention to social and non-social threats. Infancy. (2017) 22:403–15. doi: 10.1111/infa.12167

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Hane AA, Fox NA, Henderson HA, Marshall PJ. Behavioral reactivity and approach-withdrawal bias in infancy. Dev Psychol. (2008) 44:1491–6. doi: 10.1037/a0012855

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Anaya B, Vallorani AM, Pérez-Edgar K. Individual dynamics of delta96beta coupling: using a multilevel framework to examine inter- and intraindividual differences in relation to social anxiety and behavioral inhibition. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatr. (2021). doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13319. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Voytek B, Kramer MA, Case J, Lepage KQ, Tempesta ZR, Knight RT, et al. Age-related changes in 1/f neural electrophysiological noise. J Neurosci. (2015) 35:13257–65. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2332-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Buss KA, Goldsmith HH. Manual and Normative Data for the Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery - Toddler Version, Psychology Department Technical Report. Madison: University of Wisconsin (2000).

32. Buss KA, Davis EL, Ram N, Coccia M. Dysregulated fear, social inhibition, and respiratory sinus arrhythmia: a replication and extension. Child Dev. (2018) 89:e214–28. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Leventhal T, Dupéré V. Neighborhood effects on children's development in experimental and nonexperimental research. Ann Rev Dev Psychol. (2019) 1:149–76. doi: 10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121318-085221

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Bronfenbrenner U, Morris PA. The ecology of developmental processes. In: Damon W, Lerner RM, editors. Handbook of Child Psychology: Theoretical Models of Human Development. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons Inc. (1998). p. 993–1028.

Google Scholar

35. Carter AS, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Jones SM, Little TD. The infant–toddler social and emotional assessment (ITSEA): factor structure, reliability, and validity. J Abn Child Psychol. (2003) 31:495–514. doi: 10.1023/A:1025449031360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Nelson BW, Allen NB, Laurent H. Infant HPA axis as a potential mechanism linking maternal mental health and infant telomere length. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2018) 88:38–46. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Fox NA, Snidman N, Haas SA, Degnan KA, Kagan J. The relations between reactivity at 4 months and behavioral inhibition in the second year: replication across three independent samples. Infancy. (2015) 20:98–114. doi: 10.1111/infa.12063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Goldsmith HH, Rothbart MK. The Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery (LAB-TAB). Madison: University of Wisconsin Press (1993).

PubMed Abstract

39. Putnam SP, Helbig AL, Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK, Leerkes E. Development and assessment of short and very short forms of the infant behavior questionnaire-Revised. J Person Assess. (2014) 96:445–58. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2013.841171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Beck A, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer R. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol. (1988) 56:893–7. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.56.6.893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Beck A, Steer R, Garbin M. Psychometric properties of the beck depression inventory: twenty-five years of evaluation. Clin Psychol Rev. (1988) 8:77–100. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Matheny AP, Washs TD, Ludwig JL, Philips K. Bringing order out of chaos: psychometric characteristics of the confusion, hubbub, order scale. J Appl Dev Psychol. (1995) 16:429–44. doi: 10.1016/0193-3973(95)90028-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Crnic KA, Booth CL. Mothers' and fathers' perceptions of daily hassles of parenting across early childhood. J Marr Fam. (1991) 53:1043–50. doi: 10.2307/353007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Casey BJ, Oliveri ME, Insel T. A neurodevelopmental perspective on the research domain criteria (RDoC) framework. Biol Psychiatry. (2014) 76:350–3. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Doyle FL, Mendoza Diaz A, Eapen V, Frick PJ, Kimonis ER, Hawes DJ, et al. Mapping the specific pathways to early-onset mental health disorders: the “watch me grow for REAL” study protocol. Front Psychiatry. (2020) 11:553. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Peltola MJ, Yrttiaho S, Leppänen JM. Infants' attention bias to faces as an early marker of social development. Dev Sci. (2018) 21:e12687. doi: 10.1111/desc.12687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Leppänen JM, Cataldo JK, Bosquet Enlow M, Nelson CA. Early development of attention to threat-related facial expressions. PLoS ONE. (2018) 13:e0197424. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0197424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Nakagawa A, Sukigara M. Individual differences in disengagement of fixation and temperament: longitudinal research on toddlers. Infant Behav Dev. (2013) 36:728–35. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Pérez-Edgar K, LoBue V, Buss KA. LANTS: Longitudinal Attention and Temperament Study, Databrary. (2017). Available online at: https://nyu.databrary.org/volume/485 (accessed April 20, 2021).

Keywords: attention, temperament, anxiety, eye-tracking, EEG, longitudinal, infancy

Citation: Pérez-Edgar K, LoBue V, Buss KA, Field AP and The LAnTs Team (2021) Study Protocol: Longitudinal Attention and Temperament Study. Front. Psychiatry 12:656958. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.656958

Received: 21 January 2021; Accepted: 15 April 2021;
Published: 08 June 2021.

Edited by:

Soo H. Rhee, University of Colorado Boulder, United States

Reviewed by:

Mikko Peltola, Tampere University, Finland
Eeva-Leena Kataja, University of Turku, Finland

Copyright © 2021 Pérez-Edgar, LoBue, Buss, Field and the LAnTs Team. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Koraly Pérez-Edgar, kxp24@psu.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.