Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 03 February 2017
Sec. Addictive Disorders

Psychological Factors and Alcohol Use in Problematic Mobile Phone Use in the Spanish Population

\r\nJos De-Sola*José De-Sola1* \r\nHernn TalledoHernán Talledo2 \r\nGabriel Rubio*Gabriel Rubio3*Fernando Rodríguez de Fonseca,,*\r\nFernando Rodríguez de Fonseca1,4,5*
  • 1Faculty of Psychology, Department of Psychobiology, Complutense University of Madrid (Universidad Complutense de Madrid), Madrid, Spain
  • 2St. Ignatius of Loyola University (Universidad San Ignacio de Loyola), Lima, Peru
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, Hospital Universitario 12 de Octubre, Madrid, Spain
  • 4Mental Health Clinical Management Unit, Institute of Biomedical Research in Malaga, University Regional Hospital of Malaga (Instituto de Investigación Biomédica de Málaga – IBIMA, Hospital Regional Universitario de Málaga), Malaga, Spain
  • 5Hospital Universitario 12 de Octubre, Madrid, Spain

This research aims to study the existing relationships among the factors of state anxiety, depression, impulsivity, and alcohol consumption regarding problematic mobile phone use, as assessed by the Mobile Phone Problem Use Scale. The study was conducted among 1,126 participants recruited among the general Spanish population, aged 16–65 years, by assessing the predictive value of these variables regarding this problematic use. Initially tobacco use was also considered being subsequently refused because of the low internal consistency of the scale used. In general terms, the results show that this problematic use is mainly related to state anxiety and impulsivity, through the dimensions of Positive and Negative Urgency. Considering its predictive value, multiple regression analysis reveals that state anxiety, positive and negative urgency, and alcohol consumption may predict problematic mobile phone use, ruling out the influence of depression.

Introduction

There have been many and various attempts to find the key determinants of mobile phone addiction or problematic use, coupled with discussions on whether it is an addiction, as with substances, or a behavior that, in situations of abuse, may lead to similar issues. It is clear that mobile phones, as with many technology-related behaviors, foster situations of problematic use, especially among young people and adolescents, although such situations are also found in adult populations (1).

There is a logical and known coexistence between substance use and behavioral addictions (26). However, objectively, over and above this debate, there are far-reaching consequences that are associated with problematic mobile phone use, such as insomnia and sleep disorders (711), stiffness and muscular problems, eye problems (12), pain and weakness in thumbs and wrists (13), auditory and tactile illusions (14, 15), anxiety and mood swings (16, 17), high blood pressure (18), and behavioral and social problems such as “sexting” or the impulse to send or receive pictures or videos possessing sexual content (19, 20), mobile phone use in hazardous or prohibited situations, and the widespread interference of mobile phone use in personal, professional, social, or family life (2123).

Against this backdrop, the need to find variables that are associated with or those that determine dependence and problematic mobile phone use has led to research on factors, such as education, occupation, lifestyle, gender, age, personality, and drug use. They are all interrelated and have been analyzed as precursors, mediators, or consequences of abusive behavior, and it is not always easy to find direct relationships that explain their causality.

Most likely one of the most relevant personality traits in behavioral addictions is impulsivity (24), a multifactorial trait that can be defined as a predisposition to quickly and immediately react without premeditation to internal or external stimuli, without considering the damage and negative consequences (25). In other words, it is a behavior that occurs with little or no precaution (26) and that involves significant cognitive distortions (27, 28). In this regard, several studies have found a relationship between impulsivity and addiction, dependence, or problematic mobile phone use (21, 2931).

In general, as with other personality factors, impulsivity is considered to be a precursor or mediator or to coexist with addictions and psychopathological behaviors (3235). Thus, its relationship with alcohol use has been observed (3639), though this relationship has not always been evident without the mediation of intermediate variables such as certain psychopathological traits (40). Nor is it easy to find studies that show a direct relationship between alcohol abuse and problematic behavior due to mobile phones, given that, in many cases, there is the intermediation of factors such as depression (4143).

The relationship between problematic mobile phone use and depression has also been investigated, although historically, research in the context of the Internet has predominated. In principle, there are differences between the psychopathological manifestations of problematic mobile phone use and the Internet because the latter is more characteristic of introverts and loners (44). Thus, depression may more frequently coexist with Internet abuse and anxiety with problematic mobile phone use (45). However, anxiety and depression are often linked, which has led to studies in which both may be predictors, which is a means of coping with personal dysphoria and may also lead to sleep disturbances (46), an aspect shared with the Internet (8).

Similarly, there is a clear relationship between problematic mobile phone use and anxiety (17, 45, 47), in addition to their coexistence with insomnia and depression, as noted above (7, 9). Specifically, Lepp et al. (48) find that mobile phone use is positively related to anxiety and that anxiety has a negative relationship with the level of personal life satisfaction. This may lead to the concept of social anxiety, already noted by Merlo (49), which is related to the need for the device, in addition to impulsivity and urgency in regard to sending and responding to messages (11, 16, 17). In general, social anxiety (50, 51) and environmental dependence (5254) may have close relationships with mobile phone dependence, more frequently among women because of their greater sensitivity to interpersonal relationships, mainly through interactive social applications (55).

Finally, tobacco use and its relationship to problematic mobile phone use has also been investigated, although this coexistence has not always been demonstrated, as in the case of Jenaro et al. (9), who find no significant relationships. By contrast, Sánchez-Martínez and Otero (56), Toda et al. (57), and López-Fernández et al. (58) confirm this relationship. As in other cases, it is important to consider the possibility of intermediate personality variables, such as extraversion or self-esteem (47).

This research is based on the search for relationships among anxiety (expressed as state anxiety), depression, impulsivity, and alcohol use regarding problematic mobile phone use in the Spanish population. However, based on historical outcomes, we hypothesize that only anxiety and impulsiveness may be psychological determinants and predictors of problematic mobile phone use, which is also related to depression and alcohol use that may be circumstantial both as a cause and an effect or simply may coexist with such problematic use.

Initially tobacco use was also considered in this research, being refused because of the low internal consistency of the scale used. However, cannabis, psychostimulants, or other type of illegal drugs were not finally considered because of the lightweight in the context of a general population. A focused study of problematic phone use in patients diagnosed of illegal drug use disorders has to be considered for a future separate study in order to clarify the potential existence of mutually interacting factors in this in specific segment of the population.

The study extends beyond adolescents, covering the entire Spanish population. We believe that mobile phones, as an element of dependence and problematic use, have ended up capturing large segments of the adult population, becoming an addiction due to the similarity of its manifestations with the criteria of substance abuse and pathological gambling.

Materials and Methods

The research project was examined and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Regional Hospital of Málaga-IBIMA institute.

Sample and Participants

The sample includes 1,126 respondents from a survey of 1,600 questionnaires at the national level, both men and women, with an age range of 16–65 years. The survey procedures automatically exclude and erase uncompleted questionnaires, so only full respondents were used. The sampling was performed by using a non-probability procedure by quotas proportionate to the size of the Spanish population in the 17 Autonomous Communities, except Ceuta and Melilla, according to data from the National Statistics Institute (Instituto Nacional de Estadística) in 2014. Slightly more than half of the interviews were conducted in provincial capitals and in cities of over 100,000 inhabitants and the rest in rural areas and small towns (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Distribution of the sample with respect to geographic area, age, gender, main occupation, and level of schooling.

To obtain information comparable to other studies, quotas were also set by age, with the over-representation of segments of between 16 and 25 years and between 26 and 35 years. The sample average is 32.8 years, with a SD of 11.67, with 47.7% being male and 53.3% female. Regarding occupation, more than half of the respondents work, and the rest are unemployed, students, and people who take care of household duties. The level of education is high, with a majority corresponding to higher education or university degrees; almost a third completed secondary school, whereas a minority does not have education beyond basic or elementary school. The educational level of parents mainly corresponds to basic studies, followed by higher education and secondary education, and a small share of parents have no schooling. With respect to tobacco use, and using the Fagerstrom scale, we identified 85 cases with moderate problematic use (score > 4). Regarding alcohol use, 286 cases had a moderate risk of alcohol-induced harm with AUDIT scores > 8 (Table 1). Finally, 5.5% of the sample use illegal drugs, basically cannabis (77 cases) and psychostimulants (13 cases).

Procedure

The research was conducted through an online questionnaire that was sent between January and December 2014. Emailed links allowed each participant to access a platform from which the interview would begin through the survey software SSI Web version 6.8 by Sawtooth Software. It could be stopped to go back to the interview when necessary, and this link was disabled once the questionnaire was completed. All participants had to have their own mobile phone, which was assessed using a first initial filter question.

Approximately 20% of the sample was obtained through emails sent by us. The remaining questionnaires were completed by an online survey and sociological research company that used its database of 151,170 people in Spain, finally ending up with 1,126 answered questionnaires.

Statistical Analysis

The data analysis was performed using SPSS v.23.

That analysis included Pearson correlations, first, between the total scores of the scales of state anxiety (STAI-S), depression (BDI-13), alcohol use (AUDIT), impulsivity in its five dimensions and total (UPPS-P), and problematic mobile phone use [Mobile Phone Problem Use Scale (MPPUS)] for each of its three factors and total. Second, the Pearson correlations of alcohol use and depression were specifically obtained over the other variables except MPPUS.

Subsequently, multiple regression analyses were conducted using the “Intro” system of SPSS, which simultaneously introduces all variables without removal, considering the total score of the MPPUS and the scores of each of its three factors separately as dependent variables, to ascertain the predictive value both globally and for each of the components. As independent variables, we included finally the state anxiety, alcohol use, depression scores, and the scores of the five dimensions of the UPPS-P separately (positive urgency, negative urgency, lack of premeditation, lack of perseverance, and sensation seeking), omitting in this case total impulsivity as a variable to avoid redundancy with the UPPS-P dimensions.

In all cases, the maximum level of significance admitted was 5%.

Instruments

Mobile phone problematic use, state anxiety, depression, impulsivity, and alcohol use were assessed through the MPPUS (47), the UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale (37, 5961), the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT), the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-13) (62), and the State Anxiety Inventory Scale (STAI-S) (63).

Initially, we also considered in the analysis tobacco consumption, using the Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence (FTND) (64, 65), being later discarded for further analysis due to its low internal consistency.

Mobile Phone Problematic Use Scale

In assessing problematic mobile phone use, the MPPUS was used (47), with our adaptation of the MPPUS to the Spanish adult population, in turn based on the work of López-Fernández et al. (58) among adolescents (MPPUSA).

In that research, an exploratory factor analysis provided four factors or components that explain 59.8% of the variance. The first factor, with 25.9% of the variance, called “Abuse and Dependence,” is defined by excessive mobile phone use and recurrent thoughts, mood swings when the phone cannot be used, problems and interference in everyday life, discomfort and personal awareness of abuse, or warnings from the social environment. The second factor, comprising 17.6% of the variance, called “Craving and Loss of Control,” considers problems that arise from the progressive abandonment of activities, incapacity of control, or as a resource to compensate for dysphoric moods. The third factor, which entails 12.3% of the variance, called “Social Environment Dependence,” involves the personal perception of mobile phone dependence in relevant social environments. The last factor, with a single item (“I never have enough time for mobile phones”) and an explained variance of 4%, may define the tendency toward a progression or increase in the use of the device. This factor was not used because of its limited utility and practical statistical significance.

Therefore, this research analyzes problematic use, both from the total score of the MPPUS and separately, by considering these first three factors.

Impulsivity: The UPPS-P Impulsive Behaviour Scale

When measuring impulsivity, we used the UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale (61) in its latest five-dimension version (UPPS-P) (37, 59, 60) and Spanish adaptation by Verdejo-García et al. (66).

It consists of 59 items with Likert-type scales ranging from 1 to 4, depending on the level of agreement. It has five dimensions: negative urgency, which expresses the tendency to experience strong impulses under conditions of negative or dysphoric affective states; positive urgency, or the tendency to act hastily in response to positive emotional states; lack of premeditation, characterized by the lack of reflection or anticipation prior to the consequences of the behaviors themselves; lack of perseverance, or the difficulty in focusing on a task even though it is long, difficult, or boring; and sensation seeking, which may include both the tendency to seek and enjoy exciting activities and openness to new experiences, although in some cases they can be dangerous, thus having positive and negative aspects (67). Some of the items are written in reverse, which is an aspect that was corrected in the statistical analyses.

Depression: The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-13)

For depression, we used the BDI (68) in its 13-item reduced version (BDI-13) (63).

It considers affective, cognitive, motivational, and physiological symptoms of depression. The 13 items, with Likert-type scales, have four answer choices ranging from 0 to 3 points, with a maximum of 39 points.

State Anxiety: The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-S)

State anxiety was assessed using the STAI (69) by considering the Spanish adaptation of the STAI-S (63). State anxiety versus trait anxiety refers to transitional moments or periods characterized by tension, apprehension, and increased activity of the autonomic nervous system, which can vary in time or intensity. We have used state anxiety versus trait anxiety because it refers to the present time, in principle more objectifiable, assuming that state anxiety may also be reflected in trait anxiety.

The STAI-S has 20 items with Likert-type scales ranging from 0 to 3 and a possible range of 0 to 60 points; some items are written in reverse, which was corrected in the final statistical analyses.

Alcohol Use: The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT)

Alcohol use was evaluated using the AUDIT. It consists of 10 closed-ended questions: the first eight have five response options ranging from 0 to 4, whereas the last two have three options, with a possible score of 0, 2, and 4. They measure the frequency and amount of alcohol use, dependence, and problems derived from its use.

Results

Reliability and Internal Consistency of the Instruments

Except for the tobacco use (FTND), finally excluded, and alcohol use (AUDIT), in general terms the instruments used in this research showed adequate internal consistency coefficients through Cronbach’s alpha, in line with those obtained in other studies.

Thus, in this study, the MPPUS presents an alpha of 0.939, with a mean score of 68.95 and an SD of 36.89 (Table 2). This consistency is similar to other studies in which coefficients between 0.86 and 0.97 are observed (47, 58, 7077).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean, median, range of scores, number of cases, and Cronbach’ alpha internal consistency of instruments.

Similarly, with the UPPS-P, a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.935 was obtained, with an average score of 120.65 and an SD of 24.29 when considering the scale as a whole. Regarding its individual dimensions, for negative urgency, the alpha is 0.873 (Mean = 25.77, SD = 7.05); for lack of premeditation, it is 0.868 (Mean = 21.70, SD = 5.73); for lack of perseverance, it is 0.800 (Mean = 20.19, SD = 4.93); for sensation seeking, it is 0.889 (Mean = 27.73, SD = 8.33); and for positive urgency, it is 0.945 (Mean = 25.25, SD = 9, 61) (Table 2). These data are very consistent with studies such as the Spanish adaptation by Verdejo-García et al. (66), who obtain an alpha of 0.94 for the total scale, 0.87 for negative urgency, 0.87 for lack of premeditation, 0.79 for lack of perseverance, 0.89 for sensation seeking, and 0.93 for positive urgency.

Regarding the BDI-13 and the STAI-S, historically, their internal consistency is strong. Thus, in the BDI-13, a range of Cronbach’s alpha coefficients of 0.78–0.97 has been found (78), with 0.877 in our research, a mean score of 3.60, and an SD of 4.62. The same can be said of the STAI-S, with a historical outcome of internal consistency in Spain of between 0.80 and 0.94 (79, 80). In our case, it is 0.923, with a mean score of 16.32 and an SD of 9.76 (Table 2).

In the case of the AUDIT, several studies in Spain have given it an adequate reliability, validity, and sensitivity in hospital and primary health-care clinical populations (8183), with Cronbach’s alpha coefficients of between 0.81 and 0.93. However, in our study, the alpha value is weak, coming to 0.595, a mean score of 6.60 and an SD of 3.79 (Table 2). However, Contel et al. (84) obtain a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of 0.62 in a comparative study among non-alcoholic patients.

Much the same has occurred with the FTND, which is designed to detect heavy smokers at risk of disease. In general, various studies show a low and variable internal consistency for the FTND, with Cronbach’s alpha coefficients of 0.61 (65), 0.83 (85), and 0.66 in its Spanish adaptation (86). In our sample, it is very low, 0.268, with an average score of 2.35 and an SD of 2.41.

Both results may be because they are instruments with few items, especially in the case of the FTND, designed to work with clinical populations versus our general population survey.

Relationships with and Influence in Regard to Problematic Mobile Phone Use

Therefore, we finally considered the overall sum of the scores of the state anxiety scale (STAI-S), depression inventory (BDI-13), alcohol use test (AUDIT), global impulsivity and impulsivity in each of its five dimensions (UPPS-P), and problematic mobile phone use (MPPUS), both in its total score and with its three factors.

Our hypothesis considers that only anxiety and impulsivity may have a predictive power in regard to problematic mobile phone use. For this reason, the analyses consider, on the one hand, a Pearson’s correlation matrix to determine the relationships of these variables with problematic mobile phone use and, on the other hand, a multiple regression analysis to ascertain which actually predicts it.

This aims to determine which variables are predictors and which coexist and are related to problematic mobile phone use that may, in this case, also result from it.

Relationship with Problematic Mobile Phone Use and between Variables

In general, anxiety and impulsivity in its five dimensions, especially positive and negative urgency, have a stronger relationship with problematic mobile phone use. To a lesser extent, alcohol use and depression are also significantly related.

When considering the three factors of the MPPUS, anxiety is significantly related to abuse and dependence and to craving and loss of control. The total impulsivity may significantly correlate with the three factors of the MPPUS but mainly with the first two, in which positive urgency may primarily be related to abuse and dependence whereas negative urgency may be present in both. Lack of premeditation and lack of perseverance also show a higher correlation with abuse and dependence, although to a lesser extent.

Alcohol use correlates with the three factors, with a higher correlation in Abuse and Dependence, whereas depression has the greatest correlation with craving and loss of control.

Outside problematic mobile phone use and when analyzing the relationships between variables, we observe that alcohol use is related to anxiety and total impulsivity in all its dimensions and, to a lesser extent to depression. In turn, depression may maintain important relationships with anxiety and impulsivity, mainly through negative urgency and lack of perseverance (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Pearson correlation coefficients between the total score of the Mobile Phone Problem Use Scale (MPPUS), its three factors, and psychological variables, and alcohol use.

Regression Analysis of Problematic Mobile Phone Use

The predictive value of state anxiety, depression, impulsivity in its five dimensions, and alcohol use was analyzed by considering problematic use in the MPPUS as a dependent variable. Overall, these variables explain 28.7% of total problematic mobile phone use, 21.3% of abuse and dependence (factor I), 10.9% of craving and loss of control (factor II), and 6.5% of social environment dependence (factor III).

Specifically, in total problematic mobile phone use, anxiety and alcohol use may be the variables with the greatest explanatory power, in addition to impulsivity expressed in positive urgency and, to a lesser extent, negative urgency.

At the same time, the abuse and dependence factor, in addition to alcohol use and anxiety, may be determined by positive urgency and lack of premeditation, whereas the craving and loss of control factor, in addition to anxiety, negative urgency, and lack of perseverance, have relevance. Finally, in the social environment dependence factor together with alcohol use, negative urgency is maintained with lack of perseverance, which is interpreted as insistence or positive perseverance when it has a negative value (Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Multiple regression analysis of the total score and factors of the mobile phone problem use scale (MPPUS).

Therefore, despite our initial hypothesis, which only considered anxiety and impulsivity as predictor variables, we observe that, in addition to them, alcohol use also explains problematic mobile phone use. However, and as expected, although it has a connection with depression, it has no explanatory power in this research, it is not a direct cause, and it may be the result or mediator of problematic use.

Discussion

We have analyzed depression, state anxiety, impulsivity, and alcohol use, both for their direct relationships and for their predictive power in regard to problematic mobile phone use at large, and specifically in regard to the three factors of the MPPUS that were considered (Abuse and Dependence, Craving and Loss of Control, and Social Environment Dependence).

Our hypothesis was based on the fact that although there is clear proof in the scientific literature that these variables are related to problematic mobile phone use, only anxiety and impulsivity may actually be predictive of it. In this regard, the rest may be considered effects or mediators.

Initially, based on the interrelationships among all variables, anxiety and impulsivity in its five dimensions, especially positive and negative urgency, are the factors that have stronger relationships with problematic mobile phone use. To a lesser extent, alcohol use and depression also have significant relationships. Regarding the MPPUS factors, anxiety and impulsivity, expressed in terms of positive and negative urgency, maintain the highest relationships with the factors of abuse and dependence and craving and loss of control. The dimensions of lack of perseverance and lack of premeditation may also maintain their closest relationships with abuse and dependence.

Moreover, in considering the joint predictive value of anxiety, depression, impulsivity in its five dimensions, and alcohol use as independent variables, we note that they eventually explain problematic mobile phone use in 28.7% of all cases, with 21.3% for factor I, abuse and dependence, 10.9% for factor II, craving and loss of control, and 6.5% for factor III, social environment dependence.

Specifically, anxiety, alcohol, and impulsivity expressed through positive and negative urgency have a relevant predictive weight. Put differently, the factor analysis may help explain this result. It shows that, in abuse and dependence, positive urgency predominates, motivated by precipitation derived from positive affective states, in addition to lack of premeditation or reflection on the consequences, that is, a determined impulse to use mobile phones due to anxiety, leading to an impulsive behavior resulting from pleasant affective states in which alcohol may have an important presence. Only when we observed craving and loss of control did we find that mobile phones, in this case through negative urgency, also represent a means of escaping from dysphoric states with anxiety and lack of perseverance. Although social environment dependence is a factor that is little explained, it shows that the use of mobile phones as an escape from unpleasant emotional states leads to behavioral persistence, most likely social contact seeking, and that these feelings go together with alcohol use.

However, the assessment of tobacco use was finally unhelpful in this research. This result most likely has more to do with the choice of the measuring instrument than with the variable itself. We have already observed that, historically, the FTND was not an instrument with appropriate coefficients of internal consistency, which is an aspect that was found in this study. Clearly, the FNTD was designed for a rapid clinical assessment, with few items in the detection of smokers with high nicotine dependence. This makes it inadequate for our general population sample, with 23.1% of smokers and mean scores (M = 2.35, SD = 2.41) that are well below the minimum requirements (≥4 points) of dependence in this test. However, not all studies have found consistent results with problematic mobile phone use. In our case, we cannot confirm that it coexists or is a predictor, but we genuinely believe the existence of some type of relation.

Regarding depression, according to our initial hypothesis, it has a relationship with problematic mobile phone use but with no final predictive power. Similarly, it has been historically investigated in relation to different types of behavioral addictions, also coexisting with alcohol and other drugs (41). However, in the case of mobile phones, Ghasempour and Mahmoodi-Aghdam (87) find that depression was able to predict addiction by using mediator variables such as feelings of inferiority and low self-esteem, which may lead to seeking secure relationships through messages and the use of social networks. Babadi-Akashe et al. (88) also find a relationship among depression, obsessive-compulsive disorders, and interpersonal sensitivity among users with mobile phone dependence. Augner and Hacker (89) show significant relationships among mobile phone abuse, chronic stress, emotional stability, and depression among young women. Tavakolizadeh et al. (90) observe a coexistence between the tendency toward somatization, anxiety, and depression and excessive mobile phone use. Giota and Kleftaras (91) find that the problematic use of social networks was related to, among other aspects, neuroticism and depression, especially among women. Chen (92), Toda and Ezoe (93), and Kim et al. (94) confirm the relationship between depression and mobile phone addiction, which is a means of relieving or balancing negative affective states. However, opposite results have also been reported, as in the case of Whiteside and Lynam (61), who do not find such a relationship.

Depression is also related to impulsivity; however, only the latter may have predictive power in regard to problematic use, as was observed above. In this sense, Smetaniuk (1) finds that age, depression, extraversion, and low impulse control are significant predictors of problematic mobile phone use. In the case of impulsivity, as in our research, various studies have shown its relationship with mobile phone addiction and dependence (29), particularly due to the dimensions of attentional impulsivity (31), lack of perseverance, and negative urgency (21, 95). Mottram and Fleming (96) find that impulsivity and particularly lack of perseverance may be predictors of compensatory social behaviors, such as online interactive activities or social networks. Roberts and Pirog (97) also find that materialism, or the tendency to want and have expensive products or the most prestigious brands, and impulsivity predict mobile phone addiction, especially through text messages. Walther et al. (6) also indicate that impulsivity lies at the root of behavioral addictions, specifically gamblers and problematic video gamers, and in these cases, there is a coexistence with alcohol, tobacco, and cannabis use.

We have also observed that alcohol use has a significant predictive weight. Kuss and Griffiths (98) have already indicated its coexistence with social networking abuse; in turn, however, alcohol use has a significant relationship with impulsivity. This is also found, both in this research and in other studies, in which it is observed that lack of control or response inhibition (39), positive urgency (37), and negative urgency linked to lack of perseverance (36, 38) may be involved. Simultaneously, impulsivity may be a direct predictor of alcohol dependence, especially from the perspective of negative urgency, lack of perseverance, and lack of premeditation as a means of alleviating negative affective states (99). Impulsive behavior is also linked to tobacco use, which we could not observe in this research, specifically from negative urgency, which is a predictor of craving (95). In the same vein with adolescents, Gunnarsson et al. (100) note the relationship between impulsivity and antagonism with the environment and tobacco and alcohol use. Malmberg et al. (101) also find a coexistence between personality factors and substance use in which this type of consumption can also determine certain personality traits, such as impulsivity in general and sensation seeking in particular, which are factors of greater interaction with tobacco and alcohol use.

Therefore, as observed in this research, the weight and significance of anxiety in the context of addictions is known, for example, in the case of social anxiety that leads to social environment dependence. Social anxiety predicts mobile phone use based on an increased use of text messages (61, 102) in which variables such as the perception of self-efficacy and self-worth seem to intervene (103). One of its expressions would be “textiety” or the anxiety over receiving and immediately responding to text messages (104), with social networks as a means of seeking support and safety (105); here, imitating others and low self-esteem become important (106).

Additionally, very much in line with our results, Lee et al. (107) find that the compulsive use of smartphones may be related to social anxiety. Mobile phones would minimize the perceived risks and personal insecurity in relation to the environment. Similarly, Bian and Leung (108) show that social anxiety, shyness, and loneliness may consequently increase the likelihood of smartphone addiction. Hong et al. (109) also find that social extraversion and anxiety are related to mobile phone addiction, in which it can be a means of reducing or balancing that anxiety.

In short, and as stated above, our initial hypothesis is based on the fact that, although there are obvious and proven relationships among the variables that we consider, only anxiety and impulsivity may be able to predict problematic mobile phone use. However, the results show that, in addition to these, alcohol use may also have predictive power and, in this case, depression may remain a mediator variable, resulting from or coexisting with this problematic use.

It must also be noted that the use of the MPPUS factors, in addition to the overall result, has also made it possible to differentiate the components of problematic use, providing a further specification of the variables with the predictive power for each and, therefore, obtaining a better profile of mobile phone dependence. Despite the use of three of these factors, we note that the best predictive values are concentrated in the first two. Additionally, a cautious approach should be taken in regard to the social environment dependence factor because it is the worst explained by the regression model.

However, it is clear that the variables that were considered as independent variables may also play a mediating role or be dependent on problematic mobile phone use. As shown by other studies, there is not a unique direction, and methodological approaches can be diverse, with a large environment of interrelations among the variables.

Finally, is important to emphasize that the cell phone use is clearly not an extension of computer or Internet use. Both, Internet and cell phone show some differences: cell phone abuse responds to a pattern of greater lack of impulse control (30), while depression appears to be more consubstantial with problematic Internet use. In the same way, anxiety seems to be more consubstantial with problematic cell phone use, specifically in the context of social environment via text messaging (45). In any case, further research is necessary to clearly establish the contribution of impulsivity to problematic cell phone use, by using more adequate neuropsychological tests. If confirmed, impulsive patients might need specific advisory interventions for diminishing the risk of developing a problematic mobile phone use.

Conclusion

Our results on problematic mobile phone use in Spain are in line with other studies concerning various types of addictive behaviors, with substances such as alcohol or pathological gambling. This similarity and comparability with recognized addictions show that, behind problematic mobile phone use, there is an almost identical structure of the variables and interrelationships that, comparatively, may speak in favor of considering mobile phone use to be a behavioral addiction. Therefore, and given the demonstrated relationship between impulsivity not only with drugs use but also with other type of behavioral addictions as the mobile phone use, it could be important to prevent the problematic phone use considering the impulsive personality trait. These preventive actions could essentially affect to adolescents being important also to consider a broader ranges of population.

Limitations of This Research

Sociodemographic and drug use differences among the problematic phone users have not been considered in this study. At the same time, the assessment of impulsivity and other variables with subjective methods as questionnaires present important limitations. Next steps and future research will provided specific analysis in this sense as well as incorporate computerized tasks in the impulsive behavior research.

Ethics Statement

The present project was approved by the Ethics Committee of i+12 Institute in Madrid. The study is a web platform-based survey with includes an inform consent at the beginning of the questionnaire where the participants are informed of the purpose of the study. All subjects gave their free acceptance in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All the procedures guarantee the generation of completely anonymized datasets.

Author Contributions

FF and GR designed the study with the help of JD-S. JD-S did the survey, collected the data, and prepared statistical files. HT did the statistically analysis. First draft of manuscript was written by JD-S. The draft was finally reviewed in depth by all authors. Financial support was obtained by FF.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Funding

This work was financed by Red de Trastornos Adictivos, the Carlos III Health Institute (Instituto de Salud Carlos III), and EU-ERDF (Sub-programme RD16/0017/0001). Additional support comes from Proyectos de Excelencia, Consejería de Economía, Innovación y Ciencia, Junta de Andalucía, UE/ERDF (CTS-8221) to FR.

References

1. Smetaniuk P. A preliminary investigation into the prevalence and prediction of problematic cell phone use. J Behav Addict (2014) 3:41–53. doi:10.1556/JBA.3.2014.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Alavi SS, Ferdosi M, Jannatifard F, Eslami M, Alaghemandan H, Setare M. Behavioral addiction versus substance addiction: correspondence of psychiatric and psychological views. Int J Prev Med (2012) 3:290–4.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

3. Caplan SE. Relations among loneliness, social anxiety, and problematic internet use. Cyberpsychol Behav (2007) 10:234–42. doi:10.1089/cpb.2006.9963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Potenza MN. Should addictive disorders include non-substance-related conditions? Addiction (2006) 101:142–51. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01591.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Puente M, Balmori A. Addiction to cell phones: are there neurophysiological mechanisms involved. Project (2007) 61:8–12.

Google Scholar

6. Walther B, Morgenstern M, Hanewinkel R. Co-occurrence of addictive behaviours: personality factors related to substance use, gambling and computer gaming. Eur Addict Res (2012) 18:167–74. doi:10.1159/000335662

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Beranuy M, Oberst U, Carbonell X, Chamarro A. Problematic internet and mobile phone use and clinical symptoms in college students: the role of emotional intelligence. Comput Human Behav (2009) 25:1182–7. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2009.03.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Cheung LM, Wong WS. The effects of insomnia and internet addiction on depression in Hong Kong Chinese adolescents: an exploratory cross-sectional analysis. J Sleep Res (2011) 20:311–7. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2010.00883.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Jenaro C, Flores N, Gómez-Vela M, González-Gil F, Caballo C. Problematic internet and cell-phone use: psychological, behavioral, and health correlates. Addict Res Theory (2007) 15:309–20. doi:10.1080/16066350701350247

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Sahın S, Ozdemir K, Unsal A, Temiz N. Evaluation of mobile phone addiction level and sleep quality in university students. Pak J Med Sci (2013) 29:913–8.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

11. Sansone RA, Sansone LA. Cell phones: the psychosocial risks. Innov Clin Neurosci (2013) 10:33–7.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

12. Aggarwal K. Twenty-six percent doctors suffer from severe mobile phone-induced anxiety: excessive use of mobile phone can be injurious to your health. Indian J Clin Pract (2013) 24:7–9.

Google Scholar

13. Ali M, Asim M, Danish SH, Ahmad F, Iqbal A, Hasan SD. Frequency of De Quervain’s tenosynovitis and its association with SMS texting. Muscles Ligaments Tendons J (2014) 4:74–8.

Google Scholar

14. Lin Y-H, Lin S-H, Li P, Huang W-L, Chen C-Y. Prevalent hallucinations during medical internships: phantom vibration and ringing syndromes. PLoS One (2013) 8:e65152. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0065152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Verma R, Rajiah K, Cheang A, Barua A. Textaphrenia: an emerging silent pandemic. J Psychiatry (2014) 17:510–1.

Google Scholar

16. Gascón JF, Alcalde M, Bernal P, del Arriaga JL, Zamora MJ. El impacto de la mensajería instantánea en los estudiantes en forma de estrés y ansiedad para el aprendizaje [Impact of instant messaging on students in the form of stress and anxiety as regards learning]. Didáctica, Innovación y Multimedia (2014) 30:1–15.

Google Scholar

17. Ha JH, Chin B, Park D-H, Ryu S-H, Yu J. Characteristics of excessive cellular phone use in Korean adolescents. Cyberpsychol Behav (2008) 11:783–4. doi:10.1089/cpb.2008.0096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Suresh S, Sabanayagam C, Kalidindi S, Shankar A. Cell-phone use and self-reported hypertension: national health interview survey 2008. Int J Hypertens (2011) 2011:1–7. doi:10.4061/2011/360415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Delevi R, Weisskirch RS. Personality factors as predictors of sexting. Comput Human Behav (2013) 29:2589–94. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2013.06.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Strassberg DS, McKinnon RK, Sustaíta MA, Rullo J. Sexting by high school students: an exploratory and descriptive study. Arch Sex Behav (2013) 42:15–21. doi:10.1007/s10508-012-9969-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Billieux J, van der Linden M, Rochat L. The role of impulsivity in actual and problematic use of the mobile phone. Appl Cogn Psychol (2008) 22:1195–210. doi:10.1002/acp.1429

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Rosell M, Sanchez-Carbonell X, Jordana CY, Fargues M. El adolescente ante las tecnologías de la información: internet, móvil y videojuegos [Adolescents faced with information technology: internet, mobile phone, video games]. Papeles del Psicólogo (2007) 28:196–204.

Google Scholar

23. Davey S, Davey A. Assessment of smartphone addiction in Indian adolescents: a mixed method study by systematic-review and meta-analysis approach. Int J Prev Med (2014) 5:1500–11.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

24. De-Sola J, Rubio G, Rodríguez de Fonseca F. La impulsividad: ¿Antesala de las adicciones comportamentales? [Impulsiveness: on the verge of behavioral addictions?]. Health Addict (2013) 13:145–55.

Google Scholar

25. Moeller FG, Barratt ES, Dougherty DM, Schmitz JM, Swann AC. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. Am J Psychiatry (2001) 158:1783–93. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Evenden JL. Varieties of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology (1999) 146:348–61. doi:10.1007/PL00005481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Gagnon J, Daelman S, McDuff P, Kocka A. UPPS dimensions of impulsivity. J Individ Differ (2013) 34:48–55. doi:10.1027/1614-0001/a000099

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Michalczuk R, Bowden-Jones H, Verdejo-Garcia A, Clark L. Impulsivity and cognitive distortions in pathological gamblers attending the UK National problem gambling clinic: a preliminary report. Psychol Med (2011) 41:2625–35. doi:10.1017/S003329171100095X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Andreassen CS, Griffiths MD, Gjertsen SR, Krossbakken E, Kvam S, Pallesen S. The relationships between behavioral addictions and the five-factor model of personality. J Behav Addict (2013) 2:90–9. doi:10.1556/JBA.2.2013.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Billieux J, Van der Linden M, d’Acremont M, Ceschi G, Zermatten A. Does impulsivity relate to perceived dependence on and actual use of the mobile phone? Appl Cogn Psychol (2007) 21:527–37. doi:10.1002/acp.1289

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Roberts JA, Pullig C, Manolis C. I need my smartphone: a hierarchical model of personality and cell-phone addiction. Pers Individ Dif (2015) 79:13–9. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2015.01.049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Jeffrey W, Barry J, Trevor W. Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron (2011) 69:680–94. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Hogarth L. The role of impulsivity in the aetiology of drug dependence: reward sensitivity versus automaticity. Psychopharmacology (2011) 215:567–80. doi:10.1007/s00213-011-2172-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Miller J, Flory K, Lynam D, Leukefeld C. A test of the four-factor model of impulsivity-related traits. Pers Individ Dif (2003) 34:1403–18. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00122-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Whiteside SP, Lynam DR, Miller JD, Reynolds SK. Validation of the UPPS impulsive behaviour scale: a four-factor model of impulsivity. Eur J Pers (2005) 19:559–74. doi:10.1002/per.556

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Anestis MD, Selby EA, Joiner TE. The role of urgency in maladaptive behaviors. Behav Res Ther (2007) 45:3018–29. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Cyders MA, Smith GT, Spillane NS, Fischer S, Annus AM, Peterson C. Integration of impulsivity and positive mood to predict risky behavior: development and validation of a measure of positive urgency. Psychol Assess (2007) 19:107–18. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.19.1.107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Magid V, Colder CR. The UPPS impulsive behavior scale: factor structure and associations with college drinking. Pers Individ Dif (2007) 43:1927–37. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.06.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Papachristou H, Nederkoorn C, Havermans R, Van der Horst M, Jansen A. Can’t stop the craving: the effect of impulsivity on cue-elicited craving for alcohol in heavy and light social drinkers. Psychopharmacology (2012) 219:511–8. doi:10.1007/s00213-011-2240-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Whiteside SP, Lynam DR. Understanding the role of impulsivity and externalizing psychopathology in alcohol abuse: application of the UPPS impulsive behavior scale. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol (2003) 11:210–7. doi:10.1037/1064-1297.11.3.210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Evren C, Dalbudak E, Evren B, Demirci AC. High risk of internet addiction and its relationship with lifetime substance use, psychological and behavioral problems among 10th grade adolescents. Psychiatr Danub (2014) 26:330–9.

Google Scholar

42. Thomée S, Härenstam A, Hagberg M. Mobile phone use and stress, sleep disturbances, and symptoms of depression among young adults – a prospective cohort study. BMC Public Health (2011) 11:66. doi:10.1186/1471-2458-11-66

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Young KS, Rogers RC. The relationship between depression and internet addiction. Cyberpsychol Behav (1998) 1:25–8. doi:10.1089/cpb.1998.1.25

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Pérez EJ, Monje MT, de León JM. Adicción o abuso del teléfono móvil. Revisión de la Literatura. Adicciones (2012) 24:139–52. doi:10.20882/adicciones.107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Lu X, Watanabe J, Liu Q, Uji M, Shono M, Kitamura T. Internet and mobile phone text-messaging dependency: factor structure and correlation with dysphoric mood among Japanese adults. Comput Human Behav (2011) 27:1702–9. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2011.02.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Demirci K, Akgönül M, Akpinar A. Relationship of smartphone use severity with sleep quality, depression, and anxiety in university students. J Behav Addict (2015) 4:85–92. doi:10.1556/2006.4.2015.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Bianchi A, Phillips JG. Psychological predictors of problem mobile phone use. Cyberpsychol Behav (2005) 8:39–51. doi:10.1089/cpb.2005.8.39

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Lepp A, Barkley JE, Karpinski AC. The relationship between cell phone use, academic performance, anxiety, and satisfaction with life in college students. Comput Human Behav (2014) 31:343–50. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2013.10.049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Merlo L. Increased cell phone use may heighten symptoms of anxiety. Prim psychiatry (2008) 15:27–8.

Google Scholar

50. Pierce T. Social anxiety and technology: face-to-face communication versus technological communication among teens. Comput Human Behav (2009) 25:1367–72. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2009.06.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Sultan AJ. Addiction to mobile text messaging applications is nothing to “lol” about. Soc Sci J (2014) 51:57–69. doi:10.1016/j.soscij.2013.09.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Igarashi T, Motoyoshi T, Takai J, Yoshida T. No mobile, no life: self-perception and text-message dependency among Japanese high school students. Comput Human Behav (2008) 24:2311–24. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2007.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Walsh S, White KM, Young RMD. Needing to connect: the effect of self and others on young people’s involvement with their mobile phones. Aust J Psychol (2010) 62:194–203. doi:10.1080/00049530903567229

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Walsh SP, White KM, Cox S, Young RM. Keeping in constant touch: the predictors of young Australians’ mobile phone involvement. Comput Human Behav (2011) 27:333–42. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2010.08.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Valkenburg PM, Peter J. Preadolescents’ and adolescents’ online communication and their closeness to friends. Dev Psychol (2007) 43:267–77. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.2.267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Sánchez-Martínez M, Otero A. Factors associated with cell phone use in adolescents in the community of Madrid (Spain). Cyberpsychol Behav (2009) 12:131–7. doi:10.1089/cpb.2008.0164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Toda M, Monden K, Kubo K, Morimoto K. Mobile phone dependence and health-related lifestyle of university students. Soc Behav Pers (2006) 34:1277–84. doi:10.2224/sbp.2006.34.10.1277

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. López-Fernández O, Honrubia-Serrano ML, Freixa-Blanxart M. Adaptación española del “Mobile Phone Problem Use Scale” para población adolescente. Adicciones (2012) 24:123–30. doi:10.20882/adicciones.104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Cyders MA, Smith GT. Emotion-based dispositions to rash action: positive and negative urgency. Psychol Bull (2008) 134:807–28. doi:10.1037/a0013341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Lynam DR, Smith GT, Whiteside SP, Cyders MA. The UPPS-P: Assessing Five Personality Pathways to Impulsive Behavior. West Lafayette, IN: Purdue University (2006).

Google Scholar

61. Whiteside SP, Lynam DR. The five factor model and impulsivity: using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Pers Individ Dif (2001) 30:669–89. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00064-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Beck AT, Beck RW. Screening depressed patients in family practice. Postgrad Med (1972) 52:81–5. doi:10.1080/00325481.1972.11713319

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch R, Lushene R. Manual del cuestionario de Ansiedad Estado-Rasgo (STAI) [Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)]. Madrid, Spain: TEA Ediciones (1982).

Google Scholar

64. Fagerström KO. Measuring degree of physical dependence to tobacco smoking with reference to individualization of treatment. Addict Behav (1978) 3:235–41. doi:10.1016/0306-4603(78)90024-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Heatherton TF, Kozlowski LT, Frecker RC, Fagerstrom K-O. The Fagerstromtest for nicotine dependence: a revision of the fagerstrom tolerance questionnaire. Br J Addict (1991) 86:1119–27. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01879.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Verdejo-García A, Lozano Ó, Moya M, Alcázar MÁ, Pérez-García M. Psychometric properties of a Spanish version of the UPPS-P impulsive behavior scale: reliability, validity and association with trait and cognitive impulsivity. J Pers Assess (2010) 92:70–7. doi:10.1080/00223890903382369

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. d’Acremont M, Linden MV. Adolescent impulsivity: findings from a community sample. J Youth Adolesc (2005) 34:427–35. doi:10.1007/s10964-005-7260-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1961) 4:561–71. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R. STAI Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (“Self-Evaluation Questionnaire”). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press (1970).

Google Scholar

70. Foerster M, Roser K, Schoeni A, Röösli M. Problematic mobile phone use in adolescents: derivation of a short scale MPPUS-10. Int J Public Health (2015) 60:277–86. doi:10.1007/s00038-015-0660-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Kalhori SM, Mohammadi MR, Alavi SS, Jannatifard F, Sepahbodi G, Reisi MB, et al. Validation and psychometric properties of mobile phone problematic use scale (MPPUS) in University students of Tehran. Iran J Psychiatry (2015) 10:25–31.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

72. López-Fernandez O, Honrubia-Serrano L, Freixa-Blanxart M, Gibson W. Prevalence of problematic mobile phone use in British adolescents. Cyberpsychol Behav (2013) 17:91–8. doi:10.1089/cyber.2012.0260

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Montag C, Błaszkiewicz K, Lachmann B, Sariyska R, Andone I, Trendafilov B, et al. Recorded behavior as a valuable resource for diagnostics in mobile phone addiction: evidence from psychoinformatics. Behav Sci (2015) 5:434–42. doi:10.3390/bs5040434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Park N, Lee H. Nature of youth smartphone addiction in Korea. J Commun Res (2014) 51:100–32. doi:10.22174/jcr.2014.51.1.100

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Sar AH, Işiklar A. Adaptation of problem mobile phone use scale to Turkish. J Hum Sci (2012) 9:264–75.

Google Scholar

76. Takao M. Problematic mobile phone use and big-five personality domains. Indian J Community Med (2014) 39:111–3. doi:10.4103/0970-0218.132736

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Takao M, Takahashi S, Kitamura M. Addictive personality and problematic mobile phone use. CyberpsycholBehav (2009) 12:1–9.

Google Scholar

78. Dadfar M, Kalibatseva Z. Psychometric properties of the persian version of the short beck depression inventory with Iranian psychiatric outpatients. Scientifica (Cairo) (2016) 2016:1–6. doi:10.1155/2016/8196463

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Fonseca Pedrero E, Paíno Piñeiro MM, Sierra Baigrie SB, Lemos Giráldez S, Muñiz Fernández J. Propiedades psicométricas del ‘Cuestionario de Ansiedad Estado-Rasgo’ (STAI) enuniversitarios [Psychometric properties of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) in college students]. Behav Psychol (2012) 20:547–61.

Google Scholar

80. Guillén-Riquelme A, Buela-Casal G. Actualización psicométrica y funcionamiento diferencial de los items en el State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) [Psychometric update and differential functioning of items of the State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)]. Psicothema (2011) 23:510–5.

Google Scholar

81. Arnáiz A, Bautista J, Montesdeoca J, Martel A, León P. Utilidad diagnóstica del cuestionario Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) para detectar el consumo de alcohol de riesgo en atención primaria. Med Clin (2001) 116:121–4. doi:10.1016/S0025-7753(01)71745-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Ángel Pérula-de Torres L, Ángel Fernández-García J, Arias-Vega R, Muriel-Palomino M, Márquez-Rebollo E, Ruiz-Moral R. Validez del cuestionario AUDIT para la detección de problemas relacionados con el consumo de alcohol en mujeres. Med Clin (2005) 125:727–30. doi:10.1016/S0025-7753(05)72170-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Rubio G, Bermejo J, Caballero MC, Santo Domingo J. [Validation of the Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test (AUDIT)]. Rev Clin Esp (1998) 198(1):11–4.

Google Scholar

84. Contel MC, Gual AS, Farran J. Test para la identificación de trastornos por uso de alcohol (AUDIT): traducción y validación del AUDIT al catalán y castellano. Adicciones (1999) 11:337–47. doi:10.20882/adicciones.613

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. De Meneses-Gaya C, Zuardi AW, de Marques JM, Souza RM, Loureiro SR, Crippa JAS. Psychometric qualities of the Brazilian versions of the Fagerstrom test for nicotine dependence and the heaviness of smoking index. Nicotine Tob Res (2009) 11:1160–5. doi:10.1093/ntr/ntp114

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Becoña E, Vazquez FL. The Fagerstrom test for nicotine dependence in a Spanish sample. Psychol Rep (1998) 83:1455–8. doi:10.2466/PR0.83.7.1455-1458

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Ghasempour A, Mahmoodi-Aghdam M. The role of depression and attachment styles in predicting students’ addiction to cell phones. Addict Health (2015) 7:192–7.

Google Scholar

88. Babadi-Akashe Z, Zamani BE, Abedini Y, Akbari H, Hedayati N. The relationship between mental health and addiction to mobile phones among University students of Shahrekord, Iran. Addict Health (2014) 6:93–9.

Google Scholar

89. Augner C, Hacker GW. Associations between problematic mobile phone use and psychological parameters in young adults. Int J Public Health (2012) 57:437–41. doi:10.1007/s00038-011-0234-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Tavakolizadeh J, Atarodi A, Ahmadpour S, Pourgheisar A. The prevalence of excessive mobile phone use and its relation with mental health status and demographic factors among the students of Gonabad university of medical sciences in 2011-2012. Razavi Int J Med (2014) 2:e15527. doi:10.5812/rijm.15527

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Giota KG, Kleftaras G. The role of personality and depression in problematic use of social networking sites in Greece. Cyberpsychology (2013) 7. doi:10.5817/CP2013-3-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Chen YF. The relationship of mobile phone use to addiction and depression amongst American college students. Mob Commun Soc Change (2004) 10:344–52.

Google Scholar

93. Toda M, Ezoe S. Multifactorial study of mobile phone dependence in medical students: relationship to health-related lifestyle, Type A behavior, and depressive state. Open J Prev Med (2013) 03:99–103. doi:10.4236/ojpm.2013.31012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Kim JH, Seo M, David P. Alleviating depression only to become problematic mobile phone users: can face-to-face communication be the antidote? Comput Human Behav (2015) 51:440–7. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2015.05.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Billieux J, Van der Linden M, Ceschi G. Which dimensions of impulsivity are related to cigarette craving? Addict Behav (2007) 32:1189–99. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Mottram AJ, Fleming MJ. Extraversion, impulsivity, and online group membership as predictors of problematic internet use. Cyberpsychol Behav (2009) 12:319–21. doi:10.1089/cpb.2007.0170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Roberts JA, Pirog SF. A preliminary investigation of materialism and impulsiveness as predictors of technological addictions among young adults. J Behav Addict (2013) 2:56–62. doi:10.1556/JBA.1.2012.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD. Online social networking and addiction – a review of the psychological literature. Int J Environ Res Public Health (2011) 8:3528–52. doi:10.3390/ijerph8093528

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Verdejo-García A, Bechara A, Recknor EC, Pérez-García M. Negative emotion-driven impulsivity predicts substance dependence problems. Drug Alcohol Depend (2007) 91:213–9. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.05.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Gunnarsson M, Petter Gustavsson J, Tengström A, Franck J, Fahlke C. Personality traits and their associations with substance use among adolescents. Pers Individ Dif (2008) 45:356–60.

Google Scholar

101. Malmberg M, Kleinjan M, Overbeek G, Vermulst AA, Lammers J, Engels RCME. Are there reciprocal relationships between substance use risk personality profiles and alcohol or tobacco use in early adolescence? Addict Behav (2013) 38:2851–9. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Reid DJ, Reid FJM. Text or talk? Social anxiety, loneliness, and divergent preferences for cell phone use. Cyberpsychol Behav (2007) 10:424–35. doi:10.1089/cpb.2006.9936

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Chiu SI. The relationship between life stress and smartphone addiction on taiwanese university student: a mediation model of learning self-Efficacy and social self-Efficacy. Comput Human Behav (2014) 34:49–57. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2014.01.024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Taneja C. The psychology of excessive cellular phone use. Delhi Psychiatr J (2014) 17:448–51.

Google Scholar

105. Correa T, Hinsley AW, de Zúñiga HG. Who interacts on the Web?: the intersection of users’ personality and social media use. Comput Human Behav (2010) 26:247–53. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2009.09.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Park N, Hwang Y, Huh E. Exploring problematic mobile phone use: relationships between adolescents’ characteristics and mobile phone addiction. Trabajopresentadoen el Annual Meeting of the International Communication Association. Suntec City: Suntec Singapore International Convention & Exhibition Centre (2010). p. 21–6.

Google Scholar

107. Lee YK, Chang CT, Lin Y, Cheng ZH. The dark side of smartphone usage: psychological traits, compulsive behavior and technostress. Comput Human Behav (2014) 31:373–83. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2013.10.047

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Bian M, Leung L. Linking loneliness, shyness, smartphone addiction symptoms, and patterns of smartphone use to social capital. Soc Sci Comput Rev (2014) 33:61–79. doi:10.1177/0894439314528779

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Hong FY, Chiu SI, Huang DH. A model of the relationship between psychological characteristics, mobile phone addiction and use of mobile phones by Taiwanese university female students. Comput Human Behav (2012) 28:2152–9. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2012.06.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mobile phone addiction, problematic mobile phone use, mobile phone overuse, factors in mobile phone addiction, alcohol, tobacco, mobile phone use

Citation: De-Sola J, Talledo H, Rubio G and de Fonseca FR (2017) Psychological Factors and Alcohol Use in Problematic Mobile Phone Use in the Spanish Population. Front. Psychiatry 8:11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00011

Received: 30 November 2016; Accepted: 17 January 2017;
Published: 03 February 2017

Edited by:

Alain Dervaux, Centre hospitalier Sainte-Anne, France

Reviewed by:

Aviv M. Weinstein, Ariel University, Israel
Carlos Roncero, Autonomous University of Barcelona, Spain

Copyright: © 2017 De-Sola, Talledo, Rubio and de Fonseca. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: José De-Sola, jsola@ccee.ucm.es;
Gabriel Rubio, gabrielrubio@med.ucm.es;
Fernando Rodríguez de Fonseca, Fernando.rodriguez@ibima.eu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.