Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 23 March 2021
Sec. Health Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Psychological Dimensions in Human Sexual Health and Behavior View all 11 articles

Desire, Familiarity, and Engagement in Polyamory: Results From a National Sample of Single Adults in the United States

  • 1Department of Psychology, Chapman University, Orange, CA, United States
  • 2Kinsey Institute, Indiana University Bloomington, Bloomington, IN, United States

Coupledom and notions of intimacy and family formation with one committed partner are hallmarks of family and relationship science. Recent national surveys in the United States and Canada have found that consensually non-monogamous relationships are common, though prevalence of specific types of consensual non-monogamy are unknown. The present research draws on a United States Census based quota sample of single adults (N = 3,438) to estimate the prevalence of desire for, familiarity with, and engagement in polyamory—a distinct type of consensually non-monogamous relationship where people typically engage in romantic love and sexual intimacy with multiple partners. Results show that 1 out of 6 people (16.8%) desire to engage in polyamory, and 1 out of 9 people (10.7%) have engaged in polyamory at some point during their life. Approximately 1 out of 15 people (6.5%) reported that they knew someone who has been or is currently engaged in polyamory. Among participants who were not personally interested in polyamory, 1 out of 7 (14.2%) indicated that they respect people who engage in polyamory. Few sociodemographic correlates emerged; no differences in prevalence were found based on political affiliation, income, religion, geographic region, or race/ethnicity. Sexual minorities, men, and younger adults reported greater desire to engage in polyamory (compared to heterosexuals, women, and older adults, respectively). Men and people with lower education backgrounds were more likely to have previously engaged in polyamory (compared to women and people with higher education backgrounds, respectively). Given that emotional and sexual intimacy is an important part of most people’s lives, understanding the varied ways in which people navigate their intimate lives is critical for the fields of relationship, sexuality, and family science.

Introduction

Coupledom and notions of intimacy and family formation with one committed partner are hallmarks of family and relationship science. Investigations of diverse intimate relationships—long-term cohabitation, blended families, and even affairs—abound within family and relationship science literature (e.g., Bumpass et al., 1991; Treas and Giesen, 2000; Schmitt, 2005; Carr and Springer, 2010). However, these inquiries almost exclusively focus on monogamous relationships, including predictors of longevity or divorce and instances of serial monogamy. In Western science, we tend to conceptualize romantic love as limited to only one person (a zero-sum allotment of love), yet we appear to view familial and platonic love as endless (Burleigh et al., 2017; Moors et al., 2019). Although there are a variety of ways in which people navigate their intimate lives, theories of human development and intimacy often implicitly assume that a preference for monogamy is universal (Seltzer, 2000; Conley et al., 2017; Moors et al., 2017). This rose-tinted scientific lens for monogamy raises the questions: How common is desire for and engagement in polyamory—a relationship structure in which partners engage in multiple loving and sexual relationships? How common is it to personally know someone who is/has engaged in polyamory? Do people respect polyamory as a relationship option?

While many people around the globe engage in serial monogamy, remarkable transformations in relationship and family demography have occurred over the past several decades (Glick, 1988; Fisher, 1989; Finkel et al., 2014; Foster, 2016). At the nexus of this change is the new reality of contemporary intimacy that has received limited attention by researchers, clinicians, and policymakers: A sizable portion of adults in the United States and Canada have been or are currently involved in consensually non-monogamous relationships (e.g., swinging, open, and polyamorous relationships; Haupert et al., 2017a; Fairbrother et al., 2019). Approximately 1 out of 22 people (4–5%) who are currently in a romantic relationship identify as part of a consensually non-monogamous relationship (4–5%; Fairbrother et al., 2019; Levine et al., 2018; Rubin et al., 2014). When asked about an ideal relationship type, approximately 1 out of 9 people (11.9%) indicate consensual non-monogamy (Fairbrother et al., 2019). Looking at lifetime prevalence, 1 out of 5 people (19.6–21.9%) have engaged in some form of consensual non-monogamy (Haupert et al., 2017a, b; Fairbrother et al., 2019). To put this into perspective, previous engagement in consensual non-monogamy is as common as owning a cat or speaking a language other than English at home in the United States (Newport et al., 2006; DeNavas-Walt et al., 2017).

Within the past couple of years, scholars have begun to shed light on national-level interest and engagement in consensual non-monogamy. What remains unknown is desire for and engagement in specific types of consensually non-monogamous relationships, such as polyamory. One study suggests that the general public’s interest in polyamory appears to be on the rise. Through an analysis of hundreds of thousands of people’s Internet searches, Moors (2017) found that seeking out information about polyamory has markedly increased over the past 10 years in the United States. Coinciding with the general public’s interest, media representation of polyamory has emerged over the past several years—from TV and film representation (e.g., Showtime’s Polyamory: Married and Dating; Netflix’s Insecure; Professor Marston and the Wonder Women) to mainstream news coverage (e.g., New York Times, BBC, Buzzfeed). However, understanding prevalence for desire, previous engagement, and familiarity remains unknown.

The goal of the present study is to establish prevalence estimates and understand sociodemographic correlates for (1) lifetime engagement in polyamory, including challenging and positive experiences, (2) willingness to engage in polyamory, (3) frequency of personally knowing someone who is/has engaged in polyamory, and (4) positive affect toward polyamory among people who have/would not personally engage in it. To our knowledge, this is the first study to focus on people’s desire for, engagement in, and familiarity with polyamory using a demographically representative sample of adults in the United States Examining prevalence of polyamory will advance our understanding of how Americans are transforming their intimate and family lives. In the next section, we provide an overview of typical relationship agreements for each of the common sub-types of consensual non-monogamy.

Consensual Non-Monogamy: Boundaries and Definitions

Intimate relationships can be thought of as boundaries that people mutually agreed upon. Some people agree to be romantically exclusive (social monogamy) and sexually exclusive (sexual monogamy), whereas others may agree on varying levels of romantic and/or sexual openness (consensual non-monogamy; Conley et al., 2013; Gray and Garcia, 2013). At the core of consensually non-monogamous relationships are the consenting agreements to engage in varying degrees of romance and sex with more than one partner. That is, all partners involved make agreements to engage (or not) in concurrent romantic and/or sexual relationships (Conley et al., 2013; Moors et al., 2017). These relationships differ from infidelity or “cheating;” for this reason consensual non-monogamy is often referred to as ethical non-monogamy. Common forms of consensual non-monogamy include open relationships, swinging relationships, and polyamorous relationships. Swinging and open relationships tend to have boundaries that encourage sex with multiple concurrent partners but limit emotional intimacy or romantic love with these partners. Typically, people who engage in swinging partake in sexual activity with their partner (e.g., group sex, swapping partners; Buunk and van Driel, 1989; Matsick et al., 2014). People who engage in open relationships typically engage in sexual activity independently from their partner (Kurdek and Schmitt, 1986). Although, threesomes with one’s partner may be common among people in various types of consensually non-monogamous relationships, particularly for open relationships (Lehmiller, 2018; Scoats et al., 2018). While romantic love is typically “off-limits” for people engaged in swinging and open relationships, friendships appear to be common (Kimberly and Hans, 2017; Wood et al., 2018). Thus, it would be inappropriate to categorize these types of consensually non-monogamous relationships as solely “no strings attached” sexual relationships.

Distinct from swinging and open relationships, agreements in polyamorous relationships typically encourage romantic love and sexual activity with multiple concurrent partners (Barker, 2005; Moors et al., 2017). In the context of polyamory, romantic love and emotional closeness is often viewed as endless rather than limited to only one person (Moors et al., 2019). Polyamorous relationships are structured in a variety of ways, including one or two “primary” partners (often the focal or longest relationship partner) and additional “secondary” partner(s) (often referred to as hierarchical polyamory; Barker, 2005; Sheff and Tesene, 2015; Balzarini et al., 2019a). Polyamorous relationships may also take the form of triads (three person relationships), quads (four person relationships), or “V”s (Munson and Stelboum, 1999; Barker, 2005). Moreover, some polyamorous relationships are not open for everyone, per se, as “polyfidelity” refers to remaining sexually and romantically exclusive to a specific multi-person relationship. In addition, some people practice “mono-poly” relationships where one partner identifies as monogamous and the other partner has romantic/sexual relationships with multiple people (Sheff, 2016). Although the prevalence of people who identify as asexual and engage in polyamory is unknown, romantic/emotional intimacy without sexual intimacy can also exist within polyamory (Klesse, 2006; Scherrer, 2010).

Interest and Engagement in Consensual Non-Monogamy

As mentioned above, previous engagement in consensual non-monogamy among people in the United States and Canada is common (Haupert et al., 2017a; Fairbrother et al., 2019). Moreover, consensual non-monogamy is practiced by a wide range of people. In a study of two large demographically representative samples of single United States adults (N = 3,905 and N = 4,813), Haupert et al. (2017a) found few sociodemographic predictors of lifetime engagement in consensual non-monogamy. Specifically, past engagement in consensual non-monogamy did not vary significantly by age, education level, income, religion, political affiliation, geographic region, or race/ethnicity. Only two sociodemographic differences emerged. Men, compared to women, were more likely to have previously engaged in consensual non-monogamy (OR 1.66–1.83 times more likely). Moreover, lesbian, gay, and bisexual individuals had previously engaged in consensual non-monogamy at a higher frequency than heterosexual individuals (OR 1.59–2.02 times more likely). This pattern of results for gender as well as non-significant findings for age, income, and race/ethnicity were also found in a nationally representative sample of Canadian adults (other sociodemographic factors were not assessed; Fairbrother et al., 2019).

To provide a more nuanced understanding of previous engagement in polyamory, we also asked people about their experiences in these relationships, including reasons why people may have found polyamory challenging or if they would engage in polyamory again in the future. Qualitative research has documented two commonly mentioned challenges among people engaged in consensually non-monogamous relationships: managing jealousy and navigating multiple emotional bonds (Ritchie and Barker, 2006; Aguilar, 2013; Sheff, 2015; Rubinsky, 2018). For instance, people engaged in polyamory mention that they experience jealousy about their partner’s partners, however, they often describe jealousy in mild terms (developed new words such as “shaky” to describe this feeling; Ritchie and Barker, 2006). Similarly, some people describe difficulty having to unlearn traditional dating scripts (e.g., exclusivity, possessiveness) and, instead, engage in transparent and honest emotional relationships (Aguilar, 2013). Thus, in the present study, we investigated whether people experienced possessiveness and difficulty with emotions in the context of polyamory. To our knowledge, the present study is the first to capture whether people who have engaged in polyamory (or any type of consensually non-monogamous relationship) would do so again in the future.

While research on lifetime prevalence of consensual non-monogamy has provided much needed insight into diverse expressions of intimacy, research has yet to examine future intentions. In addition to understanding lifetime prevalence of polyamory in the present study, we probed whether people desire to engage in polyamory. Recent research using convenience samples has examined willingness or desire to engage in consensual non-monogamy (e.g., Moors et al., 2015; Cardoso et al., 2020). Moors et al. (2015) found that among heterosexually identified people who had never engaged in consensual non-monogamy, men expressed greater willingness than women to engage in all three popular consensual non-monogamy sub-types. In this study, people rated their willingness to engage in various consensual non-monogamous arrangements (e.g., may have sex and romantic relationships with whomever, but there must be no secrets) on a 7-point Likert scale. Looking at people who reported willingness ratings of 4 and above (somewhat to extremely), up to 31% of men and 16% of women expressed moderate-to-high levels of willingness (Moors et al., 2015). In another study, Moors et al. (2014) found that desire to engage in consensual non-monogamy was high among sexual minorities. Specifically, differences did not emerge based among bisexual men and women or gay/lesbian men and women. Looking at willingness to engage in different sexual practices, a nationally representative study of United States adults found sizable proportions of people (ranging from 11.6 to 22.1%) indicated that engaging in group sex, threesomes, and swingers parties were somewhat or very appealing (Herbenick et al., 2017). Moreover, men were more likely than women to express greater interest to engage in these three sexual practices. Although interest in sexual practices is not a direct assessment of willingness, these estimates help provide a proxy to interest in some types of consensual non-monogamy (e.g., swinging). Indeed, recent research has documented that positive attitudes toward consensually non-monogamous relationships predict greater willingness to engage in these relationships (Cardoso et al., 2020).

Positive Affect Toward Consensual Non-Monogamy and Personal Networks

In the present study, we also examined whether people knew someone who is or has been in a polyamorous relationship as well as positive attitudes toward polyamory. Stigma toward people engaged in consensually non-monogamous relationships is well documented. Compared to monogamous relationships, consensually non-monogamous relationships are perceived as low in relationship quality, less committed, immoral, and harmful to children (Moors et al., 2013; Grunt-Mejer and Campbell, 2016; Rodrigues et al., 2018). Less understood, however, is if people who are not personally interested in polyamory hold positive attitudes toward polyamory. This type of positive affect could be considered a proxy to being an ally. In the present study, we assessed whether people who were not personally interested in polyamory respected this type of relationship option. In addition, we also examined whether people knew someone in their personal network who had or is currently engaged in a polyamorous relationship. To our knowledge the present study is the first to assess prevalence of personally knowing someone who practices polyamory.

Present Study

The purpose of the present study was to establish prevalence estimates and investigate preferences for polyamory based on a United States Census-based national quota sample of single adults (unmarried or not in a current long-term relationship). While many married Americans may have engaged in polyamory, our focus on single adults allows for widely applicable results, as most United States adults are single for a substantial duration of time (U.S. Census Bureau, 2020). We sought to investigate how sociodemographic factors (e.g., gender, age, religion) were related to each study aim: (1) to establish lifetime prevalence of polyamorous relationships, including challenging and positive experiences related to polyamory and desire to engage in polyamory again in the future, (2) to investigate willingness to engage in polyamory, (3) to identify the frequency of personally knowing someone who is/has engaged in polyamory, and (4) to understand positive affect toward polyamory among people who have/would not personally engage in it.

Given that approximately one-fifth of adults in the United States have engaged in consensual non-monogamy during their lifetime (Haupert et al., 2017a), we anticipate that engagement in polyamory, a sub-type of consensual non-monogamy, will be also be relatively common (though, a smaller proportion than one-fifth). We also anticipate that estimates of willingness to engage in polyamory will be greater than lifetime prevalence. Additionally, as previous research has consistently shown that gender and sexual orientation are related to previous and current engagement in consensually non-monogamous relationships (Haupert et al., 2017a, b; Levine et al., 2018; Fairbrother et al., 2019), we expect that men (compared to women) and sexual minorities (compared to heterosexual individuals) will have engaged in polyamory at some point during their life at a higher frequency, and will express greater interest to engage in polyamory.

Further, we explore the frequency of personally knowing someone who has practiced polyamory, as well as various challenges (e.g., possessiveness) and positive experiences (e.g., future relationships will only be polyamorous) reported by people who have engaged in polyamory. Because these are both exploratory investigations, we do not have specific predictions.

Last, we do not have theoretical reasons to anticipate differences based on age, education level, household income, religious affiliation, race/ethnicity, or geographic region. Similar to previous United States and Canada national-level research on consensual non-monogamy (e.g., Haupert et al., 2017a; Fairbrother et al., 2019), we anticipate that few sociodemographic differences in lifetime prevalence and willingness to engage in polyamory will emerge.

Method

Data Collection

Data were collected as part of the annual Singles in America (SIA) study1. Inclusion criteria for the study included: being at least 18 years old, fluent in English, and single relationship status (i.e., unmarried and single, defined as not seeing anyone or dating casually). SIA is sponsored by the relationship company Match; however, participants were not recruited or in any way drawn from the Match population or subsidiary sites. Participants were recruited exclusively by ResearchNow (Dallas, TX, United States), using independent opt-in Internet research panels for population-based cross-sectional survey. Panelists were initially drawn from a diverse pool of established participants who have been continuously recruited over several years from variety of venues, including paper and electronic mailings, referrals, corporate partnerships, and internet recruitment. Participants were recruited from these opt-in research panels, with recruitment targeting based on demographic distributions (i.e., age, gender, ethnicity, region, income) reflected in the most recent Current Population Survey conducted by the United States Bureau of the Census. The current study also included augmented oversampling of certain demographic categories, specifically gay men and lesbian women.

Research panelists within the sample frame received a recruitment message inviting them to participate for financial remuneration. Remuneration was determined by ResearchNow; the average compensation standard per time spent on the survey was approximately $5.00 USD. Participants received slightly different compensation depending on how needed their particular demographic subgroup was for the sample, which was monitored and balanced in real-time. To screen for inclusion criteria and ensure data quality, research panelists were required to verify their identity through a certification process, which employs validation technologies in real-time to identify and screen out false, duplicate, unengaged, and unqualified respondents that may attempt to take a survey. Additionally, panelists were screened to ensure survey engagement, with those straight-lining responses or moving too quickly through panels removed. Participants completed the full survey, meaning there is no missing data. All data were collected over the Internet. Data access and analysis procedures were approved by [redacted for blind review] University’s Institutional Review Board.

Questionnaires

The Singles in America survey included a wide variety of questions related to participants’ attitudes and behaviors around dating and relationships, in addition to information about demographic characteristics. For the purposes of the present study, the following subset of demographic items were examined: gender, age, sexual orientation, education level, household income, religious affiliation, political affiliation, race/ethnicity, and region. To assess desire, previous behavior, and attitudes toward polyamory, all participants were asked, “When it comes to polyamorous relationships (in a committed, sexual and romantic relationship with multiple people at the same time), which of the following are true for you?” Next, they were presented with the following 11 statements (see below). Participants responded by checking boxes next to all applicable statements. Checked boxes were coded as 1, unchecked as 0.

(1) I have been in a polyamorous relationship and I would be in another.

(2) I have been in a polyamorous relationship and I would not be in another.

(3) I have tried polyamory and found that I was too possessive to cope with it.

(4) I have been polyamorous but found that all of the emotional side effects were too difficult to navigate.

(5) I have never been in a polyamorous relationship but I would consider one.

(6) I have never been in a polyamorous relationship but I want to try it.

(7) I will only consider polyamorous relationships.

(8) I have fantasized about being in a polyamorous relationship.

(9) I would consider a polyamorous relationship if it was more socially acceptable.

(10) I know someone who has had/is in a polyamorous relationship.

(11) I respect polyamorous people but I could not do it myself.

To examine previous engagement in polyamory, we combined statements 1–4 to create an index of people who have tried polyamorous relationships (i.e., if they checked any of those 4 boxes, they were given a ‘1’ whereas if they did not check any, they were coded as ‘0’). Statements 5–9 were combined in the same way to create an index of people who indicated some desire to try or be in polyamorous relationships. Statement 10 was assessed individually to understand who has been exposed to polyamorous relationships in their personal social networks. Last, statement 11 was assessed individually to understand positive attitudes with people in polyamorous relationships, particularly for individuals who do not hold interest in personally engaging in these relationships. Note that 52 participants (0.9%) selected options that were contradictory at face-value (e.g., people who reported that they would not be in another polyamorous relationship, but also reported that they would consider only polyamorous relationships in the future). Because this was such a small subset of our sample, and because people’s feelings toward entering into polyamorous relationships in the future may be more complex than a yes or no, we chose to retain these individuals in the analyses.

Participants

Extensive demographic information for participants (N = 3,438) is presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample demographics.

Results

Below we describe our statistical approach to examine links between sociodemographic factors and each study aim: (1) previous engagement in polyamory, (2) willingness to engage in polyamory, (3) personally knowing someone who is/has engaged in polyamory, and (4) positive affect toward polyamory among people who have/would not personally engage in it. Next, we report national level frequencies of attitudes, desire, and behaviors related to polyamory followed by sociodemographic correlate analyses.

Statistical Approach

To examine sociodemographic correlates of prior engagement in polyamory, willingness to engage in polyamory, knowing someone who has had or is currently in a polyamorous relationship, and positive affect toward polyamory among who are not personally interested in polyamory, we conducted four binary logistic regressions similar to the models conducted in Haupert et al. (2017a). Predictor variables included gender (coded as −0.5 = women, 0.5 = men), age (mean-centered), sexual orientation (coded as −0.67 = heterosexual, 0.33 = bisexual/gay/lesbian for LGB vs. heterosexual contrast; coded as 0 = heterosexual, −0.5 = gay/lesbian, 0.5 = bisexual for bisexual vs. gay/lesbian contrast), education level (mean-centered), household income (log-transformed for positive skewness), religious affiliation (5 codes, one for each affiliation; e.g., 0.5 = atheist, −0.5 = all others), political affiliation (Republican = −0.5, Democrat = 0.5), race/ethnicity (4 codes, one for each ethnicity; e.g., 0.5 = White, −0.5 = all others), and region (4 codes, one for each region; e.g., 0.5 = Midwest, −0.5 = all others). Note that because of cells equaling less than 20 participants, we did not include people who identified as South Asian or Middle Eastern, as Buddhist/Taoist, Hindu, or Muslim, or those who selected ‘other’ for their ethnicity or religious affiliation. Because we conducted a large number of comparisons on a large sample, we set the significant criterion for our tests to p ≤ 0.001 to protect against Type I error (see Cohen, 1992).

Frequencies: Desire, Previous Engagement, Familiarity, and Positive Affect

Across the overall sample, 16.8% of participants reported desire to try or be in a polyamorous relationship, 10.7% reported previous engagement in polyamory, and 6.5% reported knowing someone who has been or is currently in a polyamorous relationship. Among participants who had previously engaged in polyamory, a sizeable portion (30.4%) would be in a polyamorous relationship again. Among participants who have previously engaged in polyamory, 21.1% indicated that they were too possessive to cope, and 32.8% indicated that the emotional aspects of polyamory were difficult to navigate. Among people who indicated that they are not personally interested in polyamory, 14.2% reported positive attitudes toward people in polyamorous relationships; see Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Percentages of prevalence of polyamory: Previous engagement, desire, familiarity, and positive attitudes.

Sociodemographic Correlates: Desire, Previous Engagement, Familiarity, and Positive Affect

Previous engagement in polyamory. All regression coefficients are presented in Table 3. Only gender and education level were significantly related to past engagement in polyamory at the p ≤ 0.001 level. Men were over twice as likely as women to report prior engagement, along with people with lower education levels (vs. higher levels).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlates of previous engagement in polyamorous relationships.

Desire to engage in polyamory. All regression coefficients are presented in Table 4. Gender, age, and sexual orientation (heterosexual vs. gay/lesbian/bisexual) were significantly related to desire to engage in polyamorous relationships. Men were nearly three times more likely to report desire than were women. Younger people were more likely than older people to report desire for engagement (although, this was a relatively small effect). In addition, people who identified as a sexual minority were over twice as likely as heterosexual participants to report desire to engage in polyamory.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Correlates of desire to engage in polyamorous relationships.

Knowing someone who is/had engaged in polyamory. All regression coefficients are reported in Table 5. Age and sexual orientation (heterosexual vs. gay/lesbian/bisexual) were significantly related to exposure to people engaging in polyamory in one’s personal social network. Younger participants were more likely to report knowing someone practicing polyamory (or has in the past), and those identifying as sexual minorities were nearly twice as likely as heterosexual participants to report knowing someone who had or is currently engaged in polyamory.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Correlates of knowing someone who has tried polyamory.

Positive affect toward polyamory among people who were not personally interested in polyamory. All regression coefficients are reported in Table 6. Age, sexual orientation (heterosexual vs. gay/lesbian/bisexual), and political affiliation were significantly related to positive affect toward polyamory among people who did not have personal interest in polyamory. Younger (vs. older) participants, sexual minorities (vs. heterosexuals), and Democrats (vs. Republicans) were more likely to report that they respect people who practice polyamory (even if they were not personally interested in polyamory).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Correlates of “I respect polyamorous people but I couldn’t do it myself.”

Discussion

Given the centrality of relationships and family, changes in these patterns have powerful implications for social life. Adding to a growing body of research on diverse expressions of intimacy and family life, we examined previous engagement in polyamory, willingness to engage in polyamory, personally knowing someone who engages in polyamory, and positive affect toward polyamory in a national sample of United States adults. We expanded previous research on the prevalence of consensual non-monogamy in several novel ways. Our results are the first to document prevalence estimates related to polyamory in particular. Specifically, we found that willingness to engage in polyamory and previous engagement in polyamory is common. Approximately 1 out 6 people desire to engage in polyamory and 1 out of 9 people have engaged in polyamory at some point during their life. To help put this into perspective, desire to engage in polyamory is as common as how many Americans would like to move to another country (Espipova et al., 2018), and previous engagement in polyamory is as common as holding a graduate degree in the United States (United States Census Bureau, 2019). Moreover, approximately 1 out of 15 people know someone in their social network who is currently or has in the past engaged in polyamory. Among people in the present study who were not personally interested in polyamory, 14.2% of people reported that they respect people who engaged in polyamory. That is, the majority of people who were not personally interested in polyamory did not indicate positive attitudes toward polyamory.

We also found that desire to engage and previous engagement in polyamory is common among people from a range of diverse racial, political, income, religious, and geographic backgrounds. In fact, we found few links between sociodemographic factors and desire or previous engagement in polyamory. Of the few differences documented, people who identified as lesbian, gay, or bisexual (compared to people who identified as heterosexual) and men (compared to women) were more likely to report desire to engage in polyamory and previous engagement in polyamory (consistent with our hypotheses). Lesbian, gay, and bisexual individuals may be more inclined to desire polyamory because questioning a heteronormative model of relationships encourages considering alternative relationships styles (Klesse, 2016). Moreover, given engagement in consensual non-monogamy is higher among lesbian, gay, and bisexual people (compared to heterosexuals; Haupert et al., 2017a, b), having familiarity with or learning norms about consensual non-monogamy may reduce stigma toward these types of relationships among people. In terms of men’s, relative to women’s, high willingness to engage in polyamory, some scholars suggest that this reported desire is an artifact of gendered dating norms (Moors et al., 2015) while others suggests this finding illustrates evolutionary mechanisms for human mating (Mogilski et al., 2017). We also found that younger people, compared to older people, were more likely to indicate willingness to engage in polyamory (inconsistent with our predictions). Desire to try polyamory among younger adults could be related to younger adults’ tendency to hold progressive values (e.g., sex positive views, diversity values; Regnerus and Uecker, 2011; Parker et al., 2019), and potentially to younger adults being the target audience for various media that have recently depicted polyamory.

In terms of previous engagement, we found that men were more likely than women to have previously engaged in polyamory at some point during their life (consistent with our hypotheses and previous research on consensual non-monogamy; Haupert et al., 2017a; Fairbrother et al., 2019). Inconsistent with our predictions, however, was that people who identify as a sexual minority or as heterosexual are equally likely to have previously engaged in polyamory. Although previous research indicates that sexual minorities are more likely (compared to heterosexuals) to engage in consensual non-monogamy (Haupert et al., 2017a), this was not found when looking at polyamory specifically. Perhaps among sexual minorities, higher levels of previous engagement in consensual non-monogamy may be related to engagement in open relationships (which could drive the difference based on sexual orientation when looking at all consensually non-monogamous relationships). Earlier research that used convenience sampling have documented that gay men, in particular, tend to use the term ‘open relationship’ and focus on sexual relationships with other partners (e.g., Blasband and Peplau, 1985; Kurdek and Schmitt, 1986). Inconsistent with our predictions, we found that people with lower education levels (high school and some college) were more likely than people with higher educational levels to have previously engaged in polyamory. This finding is also inconsistent with speculations from researchers that people with higher education levels may have had more exposure to information about polyamory or more financial stability to pursue multiple relationships (Sheff and Hammers, 2011). In the United States, approximately 33% of people have earned higher levels of education (a bachelor’s degree or higher; U.S. Census Bureau, 2019). Thus, most people in the United States, have completed some college or high school. The finding that lower education levels are associated with previous engagement in polyamory could reflect that the majority of people in the United States hold high school diplomas or some college experiences (as opposed to college and beyond experiences).

A common stereotype about consensual non-monogamy is that these relationships yield high jealousy and are challenging (Moors et al., 2013; Grunt-Mejer and Campbell, 2016). Indeed, qualitative research has documented that similar themes are expressed by people in consensually non-monogamous relationships, especially those new to them (e.g., Aguilar, 2013). In the present study, we found that between 21 and 33% of people who had previously engaged in polyamory experienced issues with their own possessiveness and difficulty with navigating their related emotions. Although these are sizable minorities, we have no way of knowing whether jealousy is more prevalent in polyamorous versus monogamous relationships, as there are no population-based studies of jealousy available. However, prior research using large convenience samples have documented that people engaged in monogamy report higher levels of jealousy than people engaged in consensually non-monogamous relationships (e.g., Conley et al., 2017). Moreover, research has shown that jealousy is a common experience in monogamous relationships. Jealousy is one of the leading predictors of divorce in longitudinal studies (Amato and Rogers, 1997), and using data from the General Social Survey, researchers found that between 32 and 46% of separated or divorced women reported that their ex-husbands were sexually jealous and/or possessive (Brownridge et al., 2008). Further, research conducted using twin studies has suggested that the propensity for romantic and sexual jealousy is somewhat heritable, indicating a person-level factor independent of any relationship arrangement (Walum et al., 2013). Although multi-partner relationship dynamics may provide more varied instances that could facilitate jealousy than would monogamous relationships, jealousy is likely present in all relationship types.

In terms of familiarity with polyamory, sexual minorities and younger adults were more likely to report that they knew someone who is/was engaged in a polyamorous relationship (compared to heterosexual individuals and older adults). Given that sexual minorities are more likely to have previously engaged in polyamory and other forms of consensual non-monogamy (e.g., Haupert et al., 2017a), it is not surprising that they are more likely than people who identify as heterosexual to know someone in their network who practices polyamory. Moreover, lesbian, gay, and bisexual individuals are less likely to adhere to rigid gendered norms surrounding dating, including desire for monogamy and marriage (Moors et al., 2014). There is also evidence that consensual non-monogamy is less stigmatized among lesbian, gay, bisexual, and queer people (Moors et al., 2013, 2014), and indeed, we found that sexual minority participants were more likely than heterosexual participants to indicate that they respect people engaged in polyamory. Specifically, these people who indicated that they were not personally interested in polyamory, but respect it as a relationship option. Future research could explore whether familiarity is linked with holding positive attitudes toward polyamory (akin to research on attitudes toward sexual minorities; Herek and Glunt, 1993), as well as with socio-demographics related to more socially liberal attitudes, as we found with younger participants and those who identified as Democrats. Another research direction could be to explore the extent to which people who are or have engaged in polyamory hold positive or negative views about polyamory. Recent research suggests that people engaged in consensual non-monogamy can hold self-stigmatizing views about their relationships style, similar to the psychological phenomena of internalized homophobia (Moors et al., in press).

In the next section, we provide a high-level overview of the growing area of scientific inquiry on consensually non-monogamous relationships. Beyond the scope of this paper is a critical review of the current literature. Instead, we provide context of some of the current research and how this body of work can be applied to relationship, sexuality, and family science. For further insight on theoretical and research implications of understanding consensually non-monogamous relationships, see reviews by Brewster et al. (2017), Conley et al. (2017), and Moors et al. (2017). For insight on inclusive research practices related to consensual non-monogamy, see Moors (2019).

Future Directions and Implications for Relationship and Family Science

Finding a soulmate is central to mass media depictions of family life as well as social science theories of marriage and family. In fact, most people idealize monogamy and uphold a set of cultural assumptions that monogamous relationships are optimal and that monogamous romantic relationships should take priority over other relationships (known as mononormativity; DePaulo and Morris, 2005; Moors and Schechinger, 2014; Pieper and Bauer, 2014). That is, most people hold the belief that an exclusive coupled relationship is a “natural” part of the human experience and, subsequently, sexual behaviors outside of monogamous coupling are pathologized (a core concept related to queer theory; e.g., Rubin, 1984; Pieper and Bauer, 2014; De las Heras Gómez, 2019). The belief that monogamy is optimal is also an (implicit) assumption appears in many contemporary social science theories of intimacy, such as attachment theory and the investment model of relationships (e.g., Moors et al., 2015; Conley et al., 2017). One area ripe for future research is expanding relationship concepts and frameworks to include consensually non-monogamous relationship and family arrangements (see Olmstead, 2020, for a review focused on adolescence).

As found in the present study, societal views toward consensual non-monogamy tend to be negative and stigmatizing. Likewise, people engaged in consensual non-monogamy report a range of stigmatizing experiences based on their relationship (e.g., rejection from family and friends; child custody issues) and, often, hide their relationship style (Pallotta-Chiarolli, 2010; Sheff, 2015; Kimberly and Hans, 2017). These negative evaluations of consensual non-monogamy appear to be erroneous stereotypes. Research that has examined relationship qualities among people engaged in consensual non-monogamy and monogamy has generally found that people in both types of relationships report similar levels of relationship quality and psychological well-being (e.g., trust, commitment, love, depression; Rubel and Bogaert, 2015; Conley et al., 2017; Mogilski et al., 2017; Moors et al., 2017; Balzarini et al., 2019b). In some cases, people in consensually non-monogamous relationships report greater quality (e.g., lower jealousy, higher sexual satisfaction) and unique benefits, such as personal growth and diversified need fulfillment (Conley et al., 2017, 2018; Moors et al., 2017).

Furthermore, a growing body of research focused on relationship processes among people engaged in polyamory has documented a similar pattern of healthy relationship functioning. In terms of jealousy, people engaged in polyamory tend to experience low levels and use new words to describe mild forms of jealousy, such as “shaky” (Ritchie and Barker, 2006). Drawing on interpersonal relationship frameworks, Mitchell et al. (2014) investigated how meeting seven different needs (e.g., autonomy, closeness, emotional support, security) with a given partner affects relationship satisfaction and commitment with both relationship partners among people engaged in polyamory. Overall, need fulfillment across all needs were consistently high with both partners; moreover, the extent to which one partner met someone’s needs was unrelated to satisfaction or commitment with another partner. A similar pattern of results was found when looking at attachment dynamics and relationship quality among people engaged in polyamory (Moors et al., 2019). Specifically, Moors et al. found that people engaged in polyamory exhibited high levels of security with both of their partners (levels higher than established norms). Moreover, there was no association between avoidance and anxiety with one specific partner and the relationship functioning (e.g., satisfaction, commitment) in a different, concurrent relationship. These studies suggest that a relationship with one partner tends to function independently of a relationship with another partner, as both relationships were considered fulfilling, satisfying, and secure (essentially without influencing each other). In the context of the present studies’ findings, a future avenue to explore is the association between attachment bonds and reasons why some people thrive in polyamorous relationships while others experience jealousy or difficulty with navigating their emotions.

In the context of parenting, longitudinal sociological research illustrates the varied ways in which children raised by parents engaged in polyamory thrive (Sheff, 2011, 2015). For instance, children of parents engaged in polyamory report that they enjoy receiving attention from a variety of adults and sharing a diverse range of interests with adults in their lives (Sheff, 2010, 2015). In addition to benefits mentioned by children, parents engaged in polyamory expressed that multiple co-parents (or partners) helped with childrearing and household responsibilities. Although drawbacks such as breakups (and children reported that they missed these adults) can occur in polyamorous family units, this can be likened to feelings of loss that children of monogamous children experience when faced with divorce and separations. One limitation of the present study is that we did not examine whether people were parents and their experiences with or interest in polyamory. Future research could explore the extent to which people who are parents desire to or are engaged in polyamory.

To our knowledge, this study is the first to obtain information about the prevalence of polyamory, including previous engagement, desire, and familiarity, using a large United States national sample. Our study sheds light on the commonness of interest and previous engagement in polyamory among Americans. At the same time, our study focused on the experiences of people who are currently single, which limits the generalizability of our findings to people who are in relationships (including obtaining an estimate of current engagement in polyamory). Future research will benefit from understanding current engagement in polyamory as well as other specific types of consensual non-monogamy. Future research could also explore potential changes in desire or engagement in consensually non-monogamous relationships (or polyamory specifically) over time. A limitation of the present study is that it captures attitudes and behaviors related to polyamory at one time point.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institutional Review Board, Indiana University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to writing and editing the manuscript. AM led writing of introduction and discussion. AG led data analysis. JG and AG designed the survey and collected the data.

Funding

The yearly Singles in America study is funded by the relationship company Match.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that this study received funding from the dating company, Match. Match was involved in the design of the full survey and funded the data collection, but did not collect the data. Match was not involved in the study design, analysis, interpretation of data, the writing of this article, or the decision to submit it for publication.

Footnotes

  1. ^ A fully-detailed account of the methods for every wave of SIA can be found at www.kinseyinstitute.org/research/SIA.php

References

Aguilar, J. (2013). Situational sexual behaviors: the ideological work of moving toward polyamory in communal living groups. J. Contemp. Ethnogr. 42, 104–129. doi: 10.1177/0891241612464886

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amato, P. R., and Rogers, S. J. (1997). A longitudinal study of marital problems and subsequent divorce. J. Marriage Fam. 59, 612–624. doi: 10.2307/353949

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balzarini, R. N., Dharma, C., Kohut, T., Campbell, L., Lehmiller, J. J., Harman, J. J., et al. (2019a). Comparing relationship quality across different types of romantic partners in polyamorous and monogamous relationships. Arch. Sex. Behav. 48, 1749–1767. doi: 10.1007/s10508-019-1416-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balzarini, R. N., Dharma, C., Muise, A., and Kohut, T. (2019b). Eroticism versus nurturance: how eroticism and nurturance differs in polyamorous and monogamous relationships. Soc. Psychol. 50, 185–200. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000378

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barker, M. (2005). This is my partner, and this is my partner’s partner: constructing a polyamorous identity in a monogamous world. J. Constr. Psychol. 18, 75–88. doi: 10.1080/10720530590523107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blasband, D., and Peplau, L. A. (1985). Sexual exclusivity versus openness in gay male couples. Arch. Sex. Behav. 14, 395–412. doi: 10.1007/bf01542001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brewster, M. E., Soderstrom, B., Esposito, J., Breslow, A., Sawyer, J., Geiger, E., et al. (2017). A content analysis of scholarship on consensual nonmonogamies: methodological roadmaps, current themes, and directions for future research. Couple Family Psychol 6, 32–47. doi: 10.1037/cfp0000074

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brownridge, D. A., Chan, K. L., Hiebert-Murphy, D., Ristock, J., Tiwari, A., Leung, W.-C., et al. (2008). The elevated risk for non-lethal post-separation violence in Canada: a comparison of separated, divorced, and married women. J. Interpers. Violence 23, 117–135. doi: 10.1177/0886260507307914

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bumpass, L. L., Sweet, J. A., and Cherlin, A. (1991). The role of cohabitation in declining rates of marriage. J. Marriage Fam. 53, 913–927. doi: 10.2307/352997

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burleigh, T. J., Rubel, A. N., and Meegan, D. V. (2017). Wanting ‘the whole loaf’: zero-sum thinking about love is associated with prejudice against consensual non-monogamists. Psychol. Sex. 8, 24–40. doi: 10.1080/19419899.2016.1269020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buunk, B. P., and van Driel, B. (1989). Variant Lifestyles and Relationships. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.

Google Scholar

Cardoso, D., Pascoal, P. M., and Rosa, P. J. (2020). Facing polyamorous lives: translation and validation of the attitudes towards polyamory scale in a Portuguese sample. Sex. Relationsh. Ther. 35, 115–130. doi: 10.1080/14681994.2018.1549361

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carr, D., and Springer, K. W. (2010). Advances in families and health research in the 21st century. J. Marriage Fam. 72, 743–761. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2010.00728.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychol. Bull. 112, 155–159.

Google Scholar

Conley, T. D., Matsick, J., Moors, A. C., and Ziegler, A. (2017). The investigation of consensually non-monogamous relationships: theories, methods and new directions. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12, 205–232. doi: 10.1177/1745691616667925

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conley, T. D., Piemonte, J. L., Gusakova, S., and Rubin, J. D. (2018). Sexual satisfaction among individuals in monogamous and consensually non-monogamous relationships. J. Soc. Pers. Relationsh. 35, 509–531. doi: 10.1177/0265407517743078

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conley, T. D., Ziegler, A., Moors, A. C., Matsick, J. L., and Valentine, B. (2013). A critical examination of popular assumptions about the benefits and outcomes of monogamous relationships. Pers. Social Psychol. Rev. 17, 124–141. doi: 10.1177/1088868312467087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De las Heras Gómez, R. (2019). Thinking relationship anarchy from a queer feminist approach. Soc. Res. Online 24, 644–660. doi: 10.1177/1360780418811965

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeNavas-Walt, C., Proctor, B. D., and Smith, J. C. (2017). New American Community Survey Statistics for Income, Poverty and Health Insurance Available for States and Local Areas: Report CB17-157. Available online at: https://www.census.gov/newsroom/press-releases/2017/acs-single-year.html?CID=CBSM+ACS16 (accessed July, 2020).

Google Scholar

DePaulo, B. M., and Morris, W. L. (2005). Singles in society and in science. Psychol. Inq. 16, 57–83. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli162%263_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Espipova, N., Pugliese, A., and Ray, J. (2018). More than 750 Million Worldwide Would Migrate if they Could. Gallup. Available online at: https://news.gallup.com/poll/245255/750-million-worldwide-migrate.aspx (accessed July, 2020).

Google Scholar

Fairbrother, N., Hart, T. A., and Fairbrother, M. (2019). Open relationship prevalence, characteristics, and correlates in a nationally representative sample of Canadian adults. J. Sex Res. 56, 695–704. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2019.1580667

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finkel, E. J., Hui, C. M., Carswell, K. L., and Larson, G. M. (2014). The suffocation of marriage: climbing Mount Maslow without enough oxygen. Psychol. Inq. 25, 1–41. doi: 10.1080/1047840x.2014.863723

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, H. E. (1989). Evolution of human serial pairbonding. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 78, 331–354. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.1330780303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foster, J. D. (2016). “Serial monogamy,” in Encyclopedia of Family Studies, Vol. III: I-P, ed. C. L. Shehan (Chichester: Wiley Blackwell), 1739–1741.

Google Scholar

Glick, P. C. (1988). Fifty years of family demography: a record of social change. J. Marriage Fam. 50, 861–873. doi: 10.2307/352100

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, P. B., and Garcia, J. R. (2013). Evolution and Human Sexual Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Grunt-Mejer, K., and Campbell, C. (2016). Around consensual nonmonogamies: assessing attitudes toward nonexclusive relationships. J. Sex Res. 53, 45–53. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2015.1010193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haupert, M. L., Gesselman, A. N., Moors, A. C., Fisher, H. E., and Garcia, J. R. (2017a). Prevalence of experiences with consensual nonmonogamous relationships: findings from two National samples of single Americans. J. Sex Marital Ther. 43, 424–440. doi: 10.1080/0092623x.2016.1178675

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haupert, M. L., Moors, A. C., Gesselman, A. N., and Garcia, J. R. (2017b). Estimates and correlates of engagement in consensually non-monogamous relationships. Curr. Sex. Health Rep. 9, 155–165. doi: 10.1007/s11930-017-0121-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herbenick, D., Bowling, J., Fu, T.-C. J., Dodge, B., Guerra-Reyes, L., and Sanders, S. (2017). Sexual diversity in the United States: results from a nationally representative probability sample of adult women and men. PLoS One 12:e0181198. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0181198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herek, G. M., and Glunt, E. K. (1993). Interpersonal contact and heterosexuals’ attitudes toward gay men: results from a national survey. J. Sex Res. 30, 239–244. doi: 10.1080/00224499309551707

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimberly, C., and Hans, J. D. (2017). From fantasy to reality: a grounded theory of experiences in the swinging lifestyle. Arch. Sex. Behav. 46, 789–799. doi: 10.1007/s10508-015-0621-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klesse, C. (2006). Polyamory and its ‘others’: contesting the terms of non-monogamy. Sexualities 9:565. doi: 10.1177/1363460706069986

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klesse, C. (2016). “Contesting the culture of monogamy: consensual nonmonogamies and polyamory,” in Introducing the New Sexuality Studies, 3rd Edn, eds N. L. Fischer and S. Seidman (London: Routledge).

Google Scholar

Kurdek, L. A., and Schmitt, J. P. (1986). Relationship quality of gay men in closed or open relationships. J. Homosex. 12, 85–99. doi: 10.1300/j082v12n02_06

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehmiller, J. J. (2018). Tell me What you Want: The Science of Sexual Desire and How it can Help You Improve Your Sex Life. New York: NY: Da Capo Press.

Google Scholar

Levine, E. C., Herbenick, D., Martinez, O., Fu, T.-C., and Dodge, B. (2018). Open relationships, nonconsensual nonmonogamy, and monogamy among US adults: findings from the 2012 National Survey of sexual health and behavior. Arch. Sex. Behav. 47, 1439–1450. doi: 10.1007/s10508-018-1178-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsick, J. L., Conley, T. D., Ziegler, A., Moors, A. C., and Rubin, J. D. (2014). Love and sex: polyamorous relationships are perceived more favourably than swinging and open relationships. Psychol. Sex. 5, 339–348. doi: 10.1080/19419899.2013.832934

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, M. E., Bartholomew, K., and Cobb, R. J. (2014). Need fulfillment in polyamorous relationships. J. Sex Res. 51, 329–339. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2012.742998

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mogilski, J. K., Memering, S. L., Welling, L. L. M., and Shackelford, T. K. (2017). Monogamy versus consensual non-monogamy: alternative approaches to pursuing a strategically pluralistic mating strategy. Arch. Sex. Behav. 46, 407–417. doi: 10.1007/s10508-015-0658-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moors, A. C. (2017). Has the American public’s interest in information related to relationships beyond “the couple” increased over time? J. Sex Res. 54, 677–684. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2016.1178208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moors, A. C. (2019). Moving past the rose-tinted lens of monogamy: onward with critical self-examination and (sexually) healthy science. Arch. Sex. Behav. 48, 57–61. doi: 10.1007/s10508-018-1215-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moors, A. C., Conley, T. D., Edelstein, R. S., and Chopik, W. J. (2015). Attached to monogamy? Avoidance predicts willingness to engage (but not actual engagement) in consensual non-monogamy. J. Soc. Pers. Relationsh. 32, 222–240. doi: 10.1177/0265407514529065

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moors, A. C., Matsick, J., and Schechinger, H. (2017). Unique and shared relationship benefits of consensually non-monogamous and monogamous relationships: a review and insights for moving forward. Eur. Psychol. 22, 55–71. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000278

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moors, A. C., Matsick, J. L., Ziegler, A., Rubin, J., and Conley, T. D. (2013). Stigma toward individuals engaged in consensual non-monogamy: robust and worthy of additional research. Anal. Soc. Issues Public Policy 13, 52–69. doi: 10.1111/asap.12020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moors, A. C., Rubin, J. D., Matsick, J. L., Ziegler, A., and Conley, T. D. (2014). It’s not just a gay male thing: sexual minority women and men are equally attracted to consensual non-monogamy. J. Psychol. 22, 38–51.

Google Scholar

Moors, A. C., Ryan, W. S., and Chopik, W. J. (2019). Multiple loves: the effects of attachment with multiple concurrent romantic partners on relational functioning. Pers. Individ. Dif. 147, 102–110. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.04.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moors, A. C., and Schechinger, H. (2014). Understanding sexuality: implications of Rubin for relationship research and clinical practice. Sex. Relationsh. Ther. 29, 476–482. doi: 10.1080/14681994.2014.941347

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moors, A. C., Schechinger, H., Balzarini, R., and Flicker, S. (in press). Internalized consensual non-monogamy negativity and relationship quality among people engaged in polyamory, swinging, and open relationships. Arch. Sex. Behav.

Google Scholar

Munson, M., and Stelboum, J. P. (1999). The Lesbian Polyamory Reader: Open Relationships, Non-Monogamy, and Casual Sex. Binghamton, NY: Haworth Press.

Google Scholar

Newport, F., Jones, J. M., Saad, L., and Carroll, J. (2006). Americans and their Pets. Gallup. Available online at: http://news.gallup.com/poll/25969/americans-their-pets.aspx (accessed September, 2020).

Google Scholar

Olmstead, S. B. (2020). A decade review of sex and partnering in adolescence and young adulthood. J. Marriage Fam. 82, 769–795. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12670

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pallotta-Chiarolli, M. (2010). “’To pass, border or pollute’: polyfamilies go to school,” in Understanding Non-Monogamies, eds M. Barker and D. Langdridge (New York, NY: Routledge), 182–187.

Google Scholar

Parker, K., Graf, N., and Igielnik, R. (2019). Generation Z Looks a lot like Millennials on Key Social and Political Issues. Pew Research Center. Available online at: https://www.pewsocialtrends.org/2019/01/17/generation-z-looks-a-lot-like-millennials-on-key-social-and-political-issues/ (accessed September, 2020).

Google Scholar

Pieper, M., and Bauer, R. (2014). Polyamory: mono-normativity – dissident micropolitics – desire as transformative power? J. Psychol. 22, [Epub ahead of print].

Google Scholar

Regnerus, M., and Uecker, J. (2011). Premarital Sex in America: How young Americans Meet, Mate, and Think about Marrying. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Ritchie, A., and Barker, M. (2006). ‘There aren’t words for what we do or how we feel so we have to make them up’: constructing polyamorous languages in a culture of compulsory monogamy. Sexualities 9, 584–601. doi: 10.1177/1363460706069987

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodrigues, D., Fasoli, F., Huic, A., and Lopes, D. (2018). Which partners are more human? Monogamy matters more than sexual orientation for dehumanization in three European countries. Sex. Res. Soc. Policy 15, 504–515. doi: 10.1007/s13178-017-0290-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubel, A. N., and Bogaert, A. F. (2015). Consensual nonmonogamy: psychological well-being and relationship quality correlates. J. Sex Res. 52, 961–982. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2014.942722

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubin, G. (1984). “Thinking sex: notes for a radical theory of the politics of sexuality,” in Pleasure and Danger: Exploring Female Sexuality, ed. C. S. Vance (Boston, MA: Routledge and Kegan Paul), 267–319.

Google Scholar

Rubin, J. D., Moors, A. C., Matsick, J. L., Ziegler, A., and Conley, T. D. (2014). On the margins: considering diversity among consensually non-monogamous relationships. J. Psychol. 22, 1–23.

Google Scholar

Rubinsky, V. (2018). Bringing up the green-eyed monster: conceptualizing and communicating jealousy with a partner who has other partners. Qual. Rep. 23, 1441–1455.

Google Scholar

Scherrer, K. S. (2010). “Asexual relationships: what does asexuality have to do with polyamory,” in Understanding Non-Monogamies, eds M. Barker and D. Langdridge (New York, NY: Taylor & Francis), 154–159.

Google Scholar

Schmitt, D. P. (2005). Sociosexuality from Argentina to Zimbabwe: a 48-nation study of sex, culture, and strategies of human mating. Behav. Brain Sci. 28, 247–275. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x05000051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scoats, R., Joseph, L. J., and Anderson, E. (2018). ‘I don’t mind watching him cum’: heterosexual men, threesomes, and the erosion of the one-time rule of homosexuality. Sexualities 21, 30–48. doi: 10.1177/1363460716678562

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seltzer, J. A. (2000). Families formed outside of marriage. J. Marriage Fam. 62, 1247–1268. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2000.01247.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheff, E. (2010). “Strategies in polyamorous parenting,” in Understanding Non-Monogamies, eds M. Barker and D. Langridge (New York, NY: Routledge), 169–181.

Google Scholar

Sheff, E. (2011). Polyamorous families, same-sex marriage, and the slippery slope. J. Contemp. Ethnogr. 40, 487–520. doi: 10.1177/0891241611413578

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheff, E. (2015). Polyamorists Next Door: Inside Multiple-Partner Relationships and Families. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.

Google Scholar

Sheff, E. (2016). When Someone you Love is Polyamorous: Understanding Poly People and Relationships. Portland, OR: Thorntree Press LLC.

Google Scholar

Sheff, E., and Hammers, C. (2011). The privilege of perversities: race, class and education among polyamorists and kinksters. Psychol. Sex. 2, 198–223. doi: 10.1080/19419899.2010.537674

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheff, E., and Tesene, M. M. (2015). “Consensual non-monogamies in industrialized nations,” in Handbook of the Sociology of Sexualities, eds J. DeLamater and R. F. Plante (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 223–242. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-17341-2_13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Treas, J., and Giesen, D. (2000). Sexual infidelity among married and cohabiting Americans. J. Marriage Fam. 62, 48–60. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2000.00048.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

U.S. Census Bureau (2019). U.S. Census Bureau Releases New Educational Attainment Data. Available online at: https://www.census.gov/newsroom/press-releases/2020/educational-attainment.html#:|:text=In%202019%2C%20high%20school%20was,from%2029.9%25%20to%2036.0%25 (accessed September, 2020).

Google Scholar

U.S. Census Bureau (2020). Stats for Stories: Singles Awareness day: February 15, 2020. Available online at: https://www.census.gov/newsroom/stories/singles-awareness-day.html (accessed February 15, 2020).

Google Scholar

United States Census Bureau (2019). Number of People with Master’s and Doctoral Degrees Doubles Since 2000. Available online at: https://www.census.gov/newsroom/press-releases/2019/education-degrees-double.html (accessed September, 2020).

Google Scholar

Walum, H., Larsson, H., Westberg, L., Lichtenstein, P., and Magnusson, P. K. (2013). Sex differences in jealousy: a population-based twin study in Sweden. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 16, 941–947. doi: 10.1017/thg.2013.57

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, J., Desmarais, S., Burleigh, T., and Milhausen, R. (2018). Reasons for sex and relational outcomes in consensually nonmonogamous and monogamous relationships: a self-determination theory approach. J. Soc. Pers. Relationsh. 35, 632–654. doi: 10.1177/0265407517743082

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: consensual non-monogamies, consensual non-monogamous relationships, polyamory, polyamorous relationships, sexuality, family, romantic relationships

Citation: Moors AC, Gesselman AN and Garcia JR (2021) Desire, Familiarity, and Engagement in Polyamory: Results From a National Sample of Single Adults in the United States. Front. Psychol. 12:619640. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.619640

Received: 20 October 2020; Accepted: 24 February 2021;
Published: 23 March 2021.

Edited by:

Joana Carvalho, University of Porto, Portugal

Reviewed by:

Daniel Cardoso, Manchester Metropolitan University, United Kingdom
Reina Granados, University of Granada, Spain

Copyright © 2021 Moors, Gesselman and Garcia. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Amy C. Moors, moors@chapman.edu; Amanda N. Gesselman, agesselm@indiana.edu; Justin R. Garcia, jusrgarc@indiana.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.